Lomov

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 20

B. F.

Lomov

PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESSES AND COMMUNICATION

When psychological functions and processes first became the


object of concrete-scientific study (particularly experimental
studies), investigators saw their main objective to be to deter-
mine the laws and characteristics of these processes in their
pure form. Experimental methods were aimed at isolating a
given function (perceptual, mnemonic, intellectual, etc. ) as
completely as possible from the system of other psychological
phenomena and reducing to a minimum the influence of these
other phenomena on the function under study, which was then
investigated as a kind of unique natural property of the individ-
ual.
In this sense, in the first (analytic) stage of the development
of experimental psychology, psychological functions and pro-
cesses were viewed abstractly. Experimental studies deter-
mined the dependence of the dynamics of these processes and
functions on the specific features of the material being dealt
with by the subjects, on the conditions of the tasks o r problems
with which they had to cope, on their attitudes, motives, etc.

Russian text 0 1977 by "Nauka" Publishers.


From E. V. Shorokhova (Ed.) Metodologiya i metody sotsi-
al'noi psikhologii [ Methodology and methods in social psychol-
O W ] . Pp. 151-64.

3
4 B. F. Lomov

But this dependency was regarded as the result of the influence


of factors external to the processes themselves.
An important step in overcoming abstract functionalism was
made by Soviet psychology in developing a theory of activity.
Psychological processes and functions were now seen in relation
to the real activity of the subject, which enabled investigators
to discover their regulative role in this activity. Using this
-
approach, investigators reviewed the entire o r almost the
-
entire system of psychological functions and processes. The
development of sensory and perceptual processes, mnemonic
functions, ideas, conceptions, and thought were investigated in
the context of activity. The research done in this area is quite
well known.
It should be pointed out, however, that in the majority of ex-
perimental studies of psychological processes and functions,
only the relationship between subject and object is treated; and
activity (chiefly practical activity with objects) is viewed as
the activity of a discrete individual. The communication and
interaction of this individual with others are not dealt with,
though communication is an inseparable part of the realities
of human life. Studies have shown that psychological processes
unfold differently under conditions of communication from those
we observe when we study individual activity.
In our opinion one of the most important conditions for de-
veloping further the theory of psychological processes is study
of the dependence of their dynamics on the forms, methods, and
means of communication.
Let u s analyze the results of some extremely simple experi-
ments aimed a t determining the dynamic characteristics of cer-
tain psychological processes under conditions of direct commu-
nication in social interaction. ( 1 ) These experiments were
based on the theoretical positions set forth in our other arti-
cle. (2)
Of fundamental importance for our study was the organization
-
of joint activity of subjects, which inescapably required them to
communicate with one another. Small groups of two people
(called dyads in social psychology) participated in the experi-
Psychological Processes and Communication 5

ments. The subjects were students and older schoolchildren.


The experiments were conducted under natural conditions
(the subjects did not suspect that they were participating in ex-
periments). Their verbal communication with one another was
recorded on a tape recorder of which the subjects were una-
ware. Their behavior while coping with the tasks proposed to
them was also observed. (3)
We carried out three series of natural experiments.
In one of them the subjects together engaged in a visual search
for an object that was not readily noticeable (activity of obser-
vation).
In a second, we had them together draw a map of some lo-
cality.
In a third, they were to reproduce the text of a poem.
Some distinguishing features of the visual search for objects
under conditions of direct communication (results of the first
series of experiments).
In the activity of observation, the search for a perceived ob-
ject plays the dominant role. Sometimes this activity amounts
to nothing more than such a search, pure and simple. Studies
in experimental psychology aimed a t visual search taking place
as an individual activity have revealed many essential charac-
teristics of the dynamics of such a search and the dependence
of these characteristics on the structure of the field of vision,
the task a t hand, and the strategy adopted by the observer to
accomplish the task. Visual search turns out to be a complex
process, comprising the delimitation of zones of search, the
definition of systems of reference points, coordinates, and the
formulation and testing of hypotheses (B. G. Anan'ev, K. V.
Bardin, V. K. Gaida, V. P. Zinchenko, T. P. Zinchenko, A. A.
Mit'kin, V. F. Rubakhin, etc.).
In our study we attempted to examine the dynamics of visual
search under conditions of joint observation. (4-5)
Groups of two people, each observing a pictireof city life
under natural conditions, were requested to find some incon-
spicuous object with no special distinctive features such as i t s
color o r shape. The experiment was carried out as a game.
6 B. F. Lomov

The instructions were given verbally (the object and i t s distinc-


tive features were named), Sometimes subjects were given
drawings that reproduced either the shape o r the color of the
object o r both together. We were interested in seeing how the
process of communication among the subjects unfolded during
the course of this joint observation.
The task was carried out in several versions.
In some cases the subjects found the assigned object inde-
pendently of one another and almost a t the same time. They
talked with one another only to check each other's results, to
compare samples, as it were. But these cases were r a r e and
occurred only when the taek was sufficiently simple for both
subjects.
In other cases, in which the task was difficult for both sub-
jects, communication between them virtually pervaded the whole
process of search. The subjects defined common points of ref-
erence, sometimes agreed to divide up the field of search, and
marked out a general strategy. During the course of joint ob-
servation they would offer hypotheses, correct and test them
together, and define their model more precisely. But such
cases were also rare in our experiments.
The most typical kind of joint search (in a task of medium
difficulty) was one in which one of the subjects (let u s call him
A) found the assigned object earlier than the other (B)and be-
gan to talk with his partner in order to help him. Through
speech and gestures A would begin to guide the attention of his
partner, narrowing down the area of search and shortening i t s
t r a jec tory.
The chief objective of the first stage of communication was
generally Joint definition of common reference points, and it
was by no means always the partner who had already found the
object who staked out the reference points: sometimes i t was
the partner who had not yet found the assigned object. Usually
it was things that were distinguished by their color, shape, o r
position within the field of joint search that were singled out as
reference points. Sometimes subjects would take similar ob-
jects (but not the same ones) as reference points. This would
Psychological Processes and Communication 7

cause disagreements in the joint search and difficulties in the


exchange of models.
Once the common reference points were determined, subject
A would begin to construct (dictate) the direction of search for
subject B. In doing so he would never attempt to convey to sub-
ject B the same direction of searchhe himself had traversed. (6)
Already knowing where the object was, A would construct a n&
trajectory (which in his view was the most efficient) and would
convey this to his partner. In regulating the search operations
of subject B, subject A would not only indicate the position of
the sought-after object relative to the common reference points
(i.e., the direction of search) but would sometimes also formu-
late certain procedures for employing these operations. (7)
If A had arrived a t a wrong result, his partner would usually
discover this. Then both subjects would return to their model,
revise it, and begin a new cycle of joint search. In other words,
the combined search also included mutual correction of models.
The search culminated in coordination of perceptual models
and reaching a joint decision.
In discussing the kinds and means of interaction between the
subjects in joint visual search, one should point out that speech
communication in such an activity performs an auxiliary and
subordinate function. Speech in such cases is situational; i t s
vocabulary is sparse, and i t s structure is elementary. The
chief means of communication under conditions of joint visual
search is gestures. Three types of gesture may be distin-
guished: indicative, descriptive, and imitative.
An indicative gesture is employed to s e t the direction of the
partner's glance and narrow his area of search, i.e., an indic-
ative gesture is used as a means of regulating the selectivity
of perception (discriminating a figure against the general back-
ground). It is noteworthy that in using an indicative gesture,
a subject tries, as i t were, to make his visual field congruent
with the visual field of h i s partner (a gesture indicates direc-
tion relative to the partner's position).
A descriptive gesture is used as a means of reproducing the
shape of some object (a reference point) and also to aid per-
8 B. F. Lomov

ceptual selectivity (helping the partner discriminate the indi-


cated object).
Imitative gestures are used to regulate the trajectory of the
partner's search. They reproduce particular elements of the
trajectory and certain techniques for performing some visual
opera tion.
The expansiveness of the process of communication under
conditions of joint visual search depends on the complexity of
the task at hand and the degree to which the operations of the
partners are coordinated (and, above all, synchronized). The
more complicated the task and the less coordinated the opera-
tions, the more expansive is communication, and vice versa.
Some characteristic features of the dynamics of topographic
mental pictures under conditions of direct communication (re-
sults of the second series of experiments).
Studies of mental pictures as secondary sensory images dis-
closed a number of specific characteristics of such pictures
compared with perception. In empirical psychology a mental
picture was regarded as a faded copy of a perception; i t s chief
features were considered to be paleness, instability, incon-
stancy, and fragmentariness. But the work of Soviet psycholo-
gists has shown that the characteristics of a mental picture de-
pend to a considerable extent on the structure of the activity of
which it is a part (B. G. Anan'ev, L. M. Vekker, M. A. Dmitrip
em, V. N Pushkin, and others). It has been demonstrated that
a mental picture is a generalized and collective image; the path
from perception to mental picture is marked by a certain sche-
matization of the image, and the word plays a central role in
the formation of these characteristics (B. G. Anan'ev, M. V.
Gamezo, V. F. Rudakhin, and others).
Two basic forms of topographic mental pictures have been
distinguished: a ''road map'' and a "survey map"; and i t has
been shown that in a process of activity the first of these is
transformed into the second (F. N. Shemyakin). The qualities
of a panorama are typical for developed topographic mental
pictures (5.N. Vasileiskii and others).
Operation with mental pictures (the process of imagining)
Psychological Processes and Communication 9

Figure 1. Joint and separate drawing of a map of Palace Square.


A - -
map of Palace Square i n Leningrad; B map compiled by
- -
subject A; C map compiled by subject B; D, E, F succes-
sive variants of the map compiled by the two subjects jointly;
G - -
final version of the map. 1 Neva River; 2 Winter-
-
Palace; 3 Admiralty Building; 4 -Headquarters of Chief
-
of Staff; 5 -
Arch of Chief of Staff; 6 Alexandrian Column;
7 - -
Hertzen Street; 8 Nevski Prospect.

has also been studied; some of the operations employed in this


process have been delimited (transformation and scale, various
techniques, techniques of discriminating and combining the ele-
ments of an image); and the effectiveness of this process has
been found to depend on the degree of generalization, differen-
tiation, and flexibility of mental pictures.
Usually the dynamics of mental pictures has been studied
under conditions of individual activity. In this series of experi-
ments we attempted to trace out certain dynamic characteris-
tics of topographic mental pictures under conditions of direct
communication.
The subjects were asked to sketch a map of the Winter Palace
Square in Leningrad and i t s surrounding areas. A task like this
did not require a high level of graphic skills. The subjects in
the experiment were people who either lived in Leningrad o r
10 B. F. Lomov

visited i t frequently. First, the assignment was given to each


subject separately, and then (after several days) the subjects
had to carry out the assignment together.
The following is an abbreviated and partially edited protocol
from one of the experiments (joint performance of the task).
Figure 1 shows the maps drawn by subjects A and B doing
the exercise separately (Figure 1B and 1C). As we see, both
maps contain obvious mistakes (Figure 1 A shows the correct
map for comparison). In drawing up the map together, subject
A (who lived in Leningrad) took the leading role. He began the
drawing with the Nevski Prospect, and basically reproduced
his own original plan (Figure 1D).

B (interrupting A): I don't understand what vantage


point you're looking from.
B: I'm going along the Nevski Prospect to the Admi-
ral ty.
B: Oh. .. But I still don't understand. Where's the
Arch? (meaning the Chief of Staff Arch).
A: Here it is (indicative gesture).
B: And how do you get to the Arch?
A: Here's Hertzen Street (descriptive gesture).
B: I don't understand. Hertzen Street is perpendicular
to the Nevski Prospect.
A: But the Nevski Prospect goes into the Admiralty,
and behind it is the Neva (River). I'm going along here
straight to the Admiralty (imitative gesture).
B: Yes, I see that. But I don't understand how you're
doing this. From the Arch you go straight to the Neva,
and along the Nevski Prospect it looks like you can go
.
straight to the New too.. (jokingly) Maybe there are two
Admiralties ? Well, actually, here's the way i t should
be.. .. The Nevski curves a bit here (descriptive gesture,
Figure 1E).
A: Curves? What do you mean? The Admiralty isn't
visible from the Moscow Railroad Station.. ..
B: There's a little trick here. From the NevsM Pros-
Psychological Processes and Communication 11

pect along Hertzen Street you end up under the Arch like
this (imitative gesture). Maybe Hertzen Street bends a
little bit here (descriptive gesture) ?
A: Exactly. (Draws Hertzen Street, Figure 1F).
B: No, something's wrong, It curves, but only a little
bit, And it's very short.
A: 1 don't understand.
B: Well, the way i t is is that the Nevski Prospect is a t
an angle like this (descriptive gesture, then makes a cor-
rection in the drawing, Figure 1G).
A: Actually we can get to the Neva along Latvia Street,
too. But that's the long way; along Hertzen Street it's
shorter. Yes, that's the way i t is.. ..
In looking over the results of this series of experiments, the
first thing that strikes the eye is that the two subjects working
separately sketched wrong maps. So we put these two subjects
together in the same group. However, neither had any doubts
that his map was basically right (they had doubts only about the
details).
But under communicative conditions, what was wrong in both
maps was brought out, creating a problem situation and evoking
in each subject the need for communication. ( 8 )
In the initial stage of communication the chief objective (as
in the preceding series of experiments) was to define common
points of reference (common coordinates). In the experiment
whose protocol is given above, A picked the junction of the
Nevski Prospect with the Admiralty as his first point of refer-
ence, whereas B picked the Chief of Staff Arch. Communication
was aimed at determining the interrelationships between these
two points. During the course of their communication, both
subjects offered hypotheses that they then checked together. It
is interesting that the correct solution was proposed not by that
subject who seemingly should have known the Palace Square bet-
ter (i.e,, subject A, who lived in Leningrad), but by his partner
(subject B, who lived in Moscow); rather, the correct solution
was prepared in the process of joint discussion of hypotheses,
12 B. F. Lomov

in other words, in the process of communication.


In cases in which both subjects drew up a map of a locality
that was, in principle, the same, communication was aimed a t
increasing the accuracy of scale, the position of details, and
filling in gaps in the map. In these cases details were regarded
in terms of a common (i.e., accepted by both subjects) system
of reference points.
During this stage of communication, hypotheses were also of-
fered and tested, and either rejected or accepted by both subjects.
Since the subjects reproduced (with some mistakes) a map of
some locality in i t s entirety, we can call the topographic men-
tal pictures they had %urvey maps." But in the process of
communication specific elements in the topographic mental
pictures were expanded upon, as i t were, and the subjects
switched over to operating with a "road map," each subject
scanning the field in reference to a common system of refer-
ence points.
In dealing with tasks requiring the subject to invoke topo-
graphic mental pictures, the means of verbal communication
play a greater role than in joint visual search. In these cases,
too, however, the subjects made extensive use of gestures that
served as means, as i t were, of externalizing their respective
mental pictures. Through gestures (especially descriptive and
imitative gestures) one subject opened the way to his mental
picture for the other subject(i.e., made i t accessible to him). (9)
The mental picture of each subject was transformed in a cer-
tain sense, refined and enriched in the process of communica-
tion; and the end result of this was a unification o r amalgama-
tion of the mental pictures of both subjects.
As the results of this series ot' experiments showed, the ac-
curacy and thoroughness with which topographic mental p i c G e s
were reproduced were greater under communicative conditions
than under conditions of individual activity,

Some features in the reproduction of a poetry text under con-


ditions of direct communication (results of the third series of
experiments).
Psychological Processes and Communication 13

Soviet psychologists have devoted a considerable amount of


attention to the analysis of processes of reproduction under con-
ditions of individual activity (P. I. Zinchenko, L. V. Zankov,
A. N. Leont'ev, A. A. Smirnov, and others). The characteris-
tics of this process under conditions of communication have
been considerably less studied. This series of experiments
was designed to elucidate some of these characteristics.
In the preliminary study, each subject was asked individually
to reproduce the beginning of the first chapter of Pushkin's poem
Eugene Onegin. This first gave u s a baseline from which we
could work, and secondly enabled u s to screen out those sub-
jects who had only a partial recollection of the first chapter of
the poem.
In the main study (several days after the preliminary study)
groups of two subjects each were asked to.reproduce the same
material, but this time jointly under conditions of direct com-
munication.
In both cases reproduction or recall was recorded by a hidden
tape recorder.
In the case of separate (individual) recall, the classic picture,
quite well described in the literature on memory, was observed:
accurate recall of the beginning (and sometimes the end) of cer-
tain lines; substitution of certain words for others; rearrange-
ment of some words, lines, and sections; presentation of the
sense of a fragment instead of i t s literal recall; etc.
The situation was different in the case of joint recall, under
conditions of communication. In this case a certain shiftin roles,
mutual correction, and joint search for forgotten words and phrases
in connection with the formulation and testingof a hypothesis were
observed. Here are some excerpts from the protocols:

A: And then he read Adam Smith and became a political


economist.. ,
B: No, I don't think that's the way it is. There's no
such word as political economist, especially in Pushkin.
A: Something doesn't seem quite right to me either.. .
But what is i t 7
14 B. F. Lomov

B: It must be that he was some sort of an economist.. .


and became some sort of economist.. .
A: And became a scientific economist 3
B: Maybe a "deep". . .
A: Yes, that's it! And became a deep economist, i.e.,
he was able to make judgments about.. .
Both (in chorus):

Kak gosudars tvo bogateet


I chem zhivet i pochemu
Ne nuzhno zolota emu
.
Kogda . .ta-ta. ta- ta imeet.

[How the state became so rich


On what it lived and why
It i a d no need of gold
When it had ta-ta,ta-ta]

A: Some sort of product. . . Like in a natural economy. ..


but what product?
B: Maybe direct, that is, no I don't remember.
A: Simple?
B: Of course.

Both subjects recalled this part of the line accurately. We


should point out that when recall was done separately, neither
subject was able to reproduce this part of the stanza accurately;
both presented it for the most part in prose.
Here is how subject A reproduced the stanza: "Then the verse
goes on to say that Onegin read Homer, Democritus, or some
.
other Greek. . and read Adam Smith. . . . W e also went through
Adam Smith, h e read Adam Smith and then there was something
concerning evaluations of political economy. . . ."
Here is how subject B reproduced the stanza: "He studied
.
Adam Smith. . and then there was something about that the state
o r the government became rich and what it lived off of and why
it didn't need gold, when it had something. . .and, e r , his father
Psychological Processes and Communication 15

couldn't understand him and rented out h i s lands."


Neither of the subjects was able to reproduce this part of the
stanza accurately without communication, although each did re-
member the sense. On the other hand, the entire stanza was
recalled accurately after communication.
An interesting aspect of joint recall was the search for par-
ticular words. Some of the words found became key words in
recalling the following part of the text. In searching for a par-
ticular word each subject offered hypotheses, which they dis-
cussed and corrected together. Here is an extract from a pro-
tocol. The subjects for a long time were unable to recall the
part of the text in which the childhood years of Onegin a r e de-
scribed.

B: Sperva mamam za nim khodila


Potom starik ee smenil,
Rebenok by1 konechno, mil.

[At first the mother looked after him


Then the grandfather took her place
He was of course a gentle child]

A: Something's not right. It does say that h e was a


gentle child, but there's something that stands in contrast,
Oh, yes! Hereit is: "The child was friskybut lovely.''
B: Frisky? No, that's not the word.
A: But this was i n the 19th century. I remember i t for
sure that the child was frisky but gentle.
B: O.K.; maybe it was frisky. But a t the beginning we
said something about the old man that wasn't just right.
Then later on there's something about the gentle old man
who used to take Onegin for a walk in the summer gar-
dens. "And they went for walks in the summer garden."
.
A: Wasn't there some foreign w o r d , . something about
senior? Starik-sin'or .. . ? No, i t was French, The old
m'sieu, and not maman but madam.. .
B: That's it, m'sieu.. ,
16 B. F. Lomov

A: That's it. And then there's again that problem about


.
the old man.. In my opinion this is the way it is:

Sperva madam ea nin khodila


Potom m o d e ee smenil,
Pebenok by1 rezov, no mil.

[At first the nurse would look after him


Then the old sir replaced her,
The child was frisky, but gentle.]

..
"The old man da-da , a gentle Frenchman". ..
Well, let's repeat it.
Both (alternating and supporting one another) :
A: At first the nurse looked after him,
B: Then the old sir replaced her,
A: The child was frisky but gentle.
B: The old man.. .
.. ..
A: L'abbh , I remember. the last name
Both together:

Frantsuz ubogii,
Chtob ne iemuchilos' ditya,
Uchil ego vsemu shutya,
Ne dokuchal moral'yu strogoi,
Slegka ea shalosti branil
I v Letnii sad gulyat' vodil.

[ The gentle Frenchman,


So as not to torment the child,
Would teach him everything jokingly,
He did not bore him to death with strict
moral lessons,
Would reproach him lightly for his
mischief
And would take him walking in the summer
garden. ]
Psychological Processes and Communication 17

(The reproduction was close to the original: the word "m'sieu"


replaced the phrase "old man.") A little later the subjects re-
turned to this part of the text and reproduced i t again, this time
accurately.
One of the characteristic features of joint recall was that the
total scope o r volume of literally reproduced material was
greater than the sum of the volumes reproduced by each of the
participants separately. In addition, both the accuracy and con-
fidence in the correctness of the recall were greater.
It would be wrong to present the results of recall under com-
municative conditions as the superimposition of what each sub-
ject had stored in his memory on what was stored in the mem-
ory of the other subject. This is demonstrated by the diagram
in Figure 2. In the process of joint recall, what is first of all
recalled is what is stored in the memory of both subjects firmly
and accurately. These parts of the recalled material play the
unique role of a kind of general set of coordinates (building
blocks) and form a system of reference points with regard to
which the rest of the material is recalled. An important aspect
of the construction of such a system of reference points (in the
given case, consistency of exposition, a logical line marking the
beginning of the first chapter of the poem Eugene Onegin) is the
mutual reinforcement and the mutual correction of the subjects
with regard to the recalled material.
The basis for correction in joint reproduction was broader
than for individual reproduction; a sort of a joint memory bank
was formed, which both subjects used.
Aspects of recognition are also included in the recall process;
these give a new impulse, as i t were, to recall (A recognizes
what B has recalled, and this recognition dredges new parts of
the text up from his memory).
Also, processes of self-regulation are more active for each
of the subjects under conditions of communication.
In joint recollection, mistakes and doubtful places, i.e., those
parts of the material neither subject was able to recall accu-
rately, are recognized more clearly than in individual recall.
A clear distinction is made between what the subjects remem-
18 B, F. Lomov
A Figure 2. Diagram showing
=,,
m a
joint and separate recall of a po-
etry text.
--mc
-
A recall graph for subject A;
m,d
B - recall graph for subject B;
B e
-
AB graph for combined recall.
-- I 3 f
-
a correctly recalled lines;
_- b - lines whose sense was re-
called;
-
c,d omissions of individual
-- words in recall;
-
e unrecalled lines;
-- f- reshuffling of lines in recall.

bered well and what they were unable to remember o r remem-


bered o r reproduced poorly and inaccurately. It was in these
parts of the text of the recall material in which joint search for
what had been forgotten took place. Of course, such a search
also takes place in individual recall, but it is more intense in
the case of joint recall.
We should also point out that the very strategy of search is
different in a communicative situation. In individual recall,
when subjects discover a mistake they usually return again and
again to the beginning of the text (or stanza) and run through i t
anew each time. We might call this a scanning strategy. Often,
when a subject uncovers a mistake, he is unable to correct it
and gives up any further attempts a t recall.
Under communicative conditions, such gaps (blocks in recall)
become, so to speak, the focus of joint effort; and joint search
Psychological Processes and Communication 19

is organized with regard to them. We can call this focusing


strategy. The subjects uncover a gap and begin actively to offer
hypotheses, discuss them together, and correct them.
Reminiscence is more frequent in joint recall than in individ-
ual recall.
Finally, we should point out that, on the whole, the process of
joint recall of a text (like the map of a locality) takes place in
a more active and emotionally laden manner than does individ-
ual recall; and this is another factor contributing to i t s greater
efficiency.

* * *
The findings of our trial experiments enable u s to say a few
things about the conditions, functions, and structure of commu-
nication, and about certain features of the dynamics of the psy-
chological processes constituting it.
As we saw, the most important condition giving rise to the
need for communication is the emergence of a problem situa-
-
tion.-(10) This means that a problem (or task) arises for whose
resolution the knowledge, abilities, and skills of a single indi-
vidual taking part in a concrete activity are, for one reason o r
another, insufficient.
The chief functions of communication (in our case) consist in
an exchange of the results of cognitive activity of each individ-
ual, mutual regulation and correction of the operations carried
out by each of them, and the formation, on this basis, of a joint
group, an aggregate subject of activity.
The dynamics (and structure) of the process of communica-
tion in each particular case is determined by the conditions
under which it takes place. The dynamics depend on the nature
of the task to be resolved, the individual characteristics of the
persons taking part in communication, and the interrelation-
ships that are formed among them ( l l ) , on who communicates
-
what, for what reason, and - how, to whom.
The results of our study are still not sufficient to enable u s
readily to propose a satisfactory structural model of the pro-
20 B. F. Lomov
cess of communication, We should like only to point out the
most important aspects of communication from our point of
view. The first stage in this process is the determination of
common "coordinates"of joint activity (reference points, ref-
erence models). These serve as a basis that, in a certain
sense, guides the construction of the entire process of commu-
nication and the distribution and coordination of the operations
carried out by each member of the communicating group. In
the formation of these coordinates, contradictions sometimes
arise that impede coordination of actions. (12)
The process of communication itself unfolds along a sort of
spiral path: i t involves an alternation of functions of each of the
participants. The relationships among the participants are bi-
lateral and mutually reversible.
Synchronization of the actions of each participant in commu-
nication and mutual stimulation, regulation, correction, and
complementarity are all important aspects of communication.
The process of communication produces a common program
and common strategy for joint activity. A strategy formulated
in the process of communication is qualitatively different from
an individual strategy.
In all the experiments, the effectiveness of combined coping
with all the proposed tasks was greater than the effectiveness
of individual activity. This is not a new finding in itself: it has
long been known to social psychologists. (13) But our findings
indicate that a necessary condition for incFeasing the effective-
ness of communication is the formation of a special joint "fund"
of conceptions, ideas, and techniques for dealing with particular
problems, i.e., an interindividual psychological reserve.
In our experiments we studied psychologically different types
of activity. In one of them sensory/perceptual processes (vi-
sual search) played the leading role; in another, processes of
imagination (portrayal of a locality) was the major feature;
and in a third, the emphasis was on mnenonic processes (re-
production or recall of a poetry text). Our findings indicate
that the dynamics of these processes depended quite intimately
on communication. Communication may be regarded as one of
Psychological Processes and Communication 21

the most important determinants of the dynamics of these pro-


cesses.
Finally, it should be observed that the interrelationship of the
different means of communication is determined by the kind of
task to be resolved. In some cases speech plays the leading
role; in others, i t is gestures (especially in the case of tasks
that require spatial orientation o r recall o r reproduction of the
spatial features of objects). In communication associated with
the exchange of emotional states, imitation probably plays the
dominant role.
As we have said, our experiments are only preliminary. Our
findings should be seen merely as a framework within which
further studies may be planned and conducted. Much more ef-
fort is required to work out rigorous empirical methods, meth-
ods for concise description of empirical data, and models of
processes of communication.

Notes

1) A theory of communication and rigorous methods for study-


ing the dynamics of psychological processes in communication
have not yet been developed in psychology. This is a task for
the future. The studies we have described in this article are a
contribution to the solution of this task.
2 ) See this same book, pages 124 through 135.
3 ) Unfortunately, we did not have any means of accurately
recording the behavior of our subjects.
4 - 5 ) Unfortunately, we were unable to make use of any means
for accurate recording of the process of search (in particular,
the eye movements of each of the subjects).
6 ) As an analysis of the accounts of the subjects shows, they
usually were unable to reproduce the path of their individual
search completely, but they did reproduce quite accurately the
course taken in their joint search.
7) For a more detailed study of the characteristics of the
trajectory o r pathway of search under communicative condi-
tions, i t would be quite useful to do a comparative analysis of
22 B. F. Lomov

the trajectories of eye movements in individual and combined


search.
8 ) When the maps drawn by each of the subjects were iden-
tical (or almost identical), communication was terse and es-
sentially confined to mutual confirmation of the identity of the
maps.
9 ) A comparison of sketches of the actual actions in imitat-
ing gestures performed by the subjects shows that a gesture
reproduced an operation only schematically and in a generalized
form (findings of our colleagues D. Gaida and A. Charchani).
10) Of course, this is not the only condition. A need for com-
munication also arises in connection with the need, for example,
to alter an emotional state (the need for distraction) o r a set
of interpersonal relationships, o r the need to organize joint
activity, etc.
11) In our experiments we tried a s far a s possible to neu-
tralize this factor by choosing subjects who could enter into
joint activity on an equal basis with their partners.
12) The sources of these contradictions require a special
study.
13 ) Of course, contrary findings have also been recorded in
social psychology. The efficiency of joint activity evidently de-
pends on many factors: interrelationships among i t s partici-
pants, the organization of activity, group structure, etc.

You might also like