Emotion Regulation of Others and Self (EROS)
Emotion Regulation of Others and Self (EROS)
Emotion Regulation of Others and Self (EROS)
DOI 10.1007/s12144-011-9099-9
Abstract Research on affect regulation has blossomed in recent years. However, the
lack of validated scales assessing individual differences in the use of strategies to
achieve alternative types of affect regulation, e.g., the regulation of others’ affect and
the worsening of affect, has hampered research on these important processes. This
paper presents the development and validation of a brief new measure of individual
differences in the use of strategies to regulate one’s own and other people’s feelings:
the Emotion Regulation of Others and Self (EROS) scale. Two distinct samples (N=
551 and N=227) confirmed a four-factor structure: intrinsic affect-improving,
intrinsic affect-worsening, extrinsic affect-improving and extrinsic affect-worsening.
In line with predictions, these factors were associated with existing measures of
affect regulation, personality and affect. Both intrinsic factors were positively
associated with emotional exhaustion, while all factors except extrinsic affect-
improving were positively associated with health-related impairments. Convergence
between self- and other-reported scores on the extrinsic factors in a third sample (N=
50 dyads) demonstrated further evidence of validity.
Are the people who try to cheer others up also the people who try to cheer
themselves up? Do individuals who try to worsen their feelings suffer poor
psychological and physical health? Questions such as these concern individual
differences in affect regulation, but to answer them would be difficult because
existing measures do not assess different types of affect regulation simultaneously or
D. Holman
University of Manchester, Manchester, UK
54 Curr Psychol (2011) 30:53–73
people’s perceived ability to regulate their own or others’ feelings (e.g., the Survey
of Emotional Intelligence, SEI, Tett et al. 2005). We argue that it is particularly
important to measure affect regulation at the behavior level of strategy use, because:
i) the strategies that people use can have benefits or costs for their well-being,
performance and relationships (Gross and John 2003); and ii) beliefs and ability do
not always translate into behavior in practice (Mikolajczak et al. 2009).
Currently, there are a number of scales available to measure individual differences
in the use of affect regulation strategies, perhaps the most commonly-used being the
Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ, Gross and John 2003). However, there are
three key shortcomings of existing scales that limit the insight that researchers can
provide into affect regulation, which we discuss in the next section. As such, the aim
of the current paper is to develop and present validity data for a new measure of
individual differences in the use of the four main types of affect regulation strategies.
It was our intent to develop a relatively brief measure, so that researchers could use it
in combination with measures of other constructs. The new measure is used to
address some key questions about the nature of as-yet under-researched types of
affect regulation, notably extrinsic affect regulation and affect-worsening.
The first limitation of existing measures of affect regulation strategies is that scales
have generally focused on measuring intrinsic affect regulation. Yet we know that
people also try to regulate the feelings of those around them; sometimes as a means
of giving care (e.g., doctors making their patients feel calmer to alleviate their
distress; Francis et al. 1999) and sometimes for personal gain (e.g., making one’s
partner feel guilty to gain attention, Vangelisti et al. 1991). Indeed, studies have
highlighted the importance of extrinsic affect regulation in a variety of social
contexts, including parent–child relationships, relationships with peers, family
members and romantic partners, support groups and work organizations (Gross
and Thompson 2007; Thoits 1996). However, to date research on this process has
been largely exploratory and descriptive in nature, owing primarily to the lack of
existing scales that assess the use of extrinsic affect regulation strategies. A few
scales do measure the ability to regulate others’ affect (e.g., the SEI, Tett et al. 2005),
but these scales cannot contribute towards understanding how the use of extrinsic
affect regulation might affect regulators and those around them. Thus, important
questions about the effects of extrinsic affect regulation have yet to be explored. For
instance, research has shown that the regulation of one’s own affect is effortful and
potentially damaging for people’s health and well-being (e.g., Grandey 2003), but
has not looked at whether the regulation of others’ affect might also contribute to
emotional exhaustion and health impairment.
The second key limitation of existing measures is that they usually only assess
strategies intended to improve affect. Yet people also try to worsen their own affect
and the affect of other people; sometimes for instrumental purposes (e.g., making
oneself feel angry to stand one’s ground during an argument, Riediger et al. 2009)
and sometimes as a dysfunctional way of responding to emotions in oneself or others
(Phillips and Power 2007). Studies have highlighted the use of intrinsic affect-
56 Curr Psychol (2011) 30:53–73
worsening amongst all age groups, but especially amongst those in adolescence
(Riediger et al. 2009); and the use of extrinsic affect-worsening within close familial
and romantic relationships (Vangelisti et al. 1991) and in work contexts such as
prisons (Niven et al. 2007). Some intrinsic affect regulation scales do include
subscales concerning the dampening of positive emotions (e.g., Feldman et al.
2008), but these scales do not cover strategies aimed at inducing negative emotions.
Other measures include strategies that often result in poorer affect (e.g., depressive
rumination, Nolen-Hoeksema and Morrow 1991), but these tend to be strategies
enacted with the intention of improving as opposed to worsening one’s affect. Thus,
to date no scales allow researchers to directly assess the full range of strategies that
can be used to worsen one’s own or other people’s affect. This may be an important
omission, because a large body of research indicates that negative events and
emotions are more salient than positive ones (Baumeister et al. 2001), and so it could
be the case that individual differences in the use of affect-worsening strategies
explain substantial variance in people’s well-being and health.
A third limitation of existing measures is that they are often not based on a
general theoretical model of affect regulation and therefore only cover part of the
range of available strategies. For instance, several scales are based on models of
particular approaches to affect regulation (e.g., the Cognitive Emotion Regulation
Questionnaire, Garnefski et al. 2001). Likewise, some scales are based on models
that concern only specific emotion states (e.g., the Anger Rumination Scale,
Sukhodolsky et al. 2001).
Structure of EROS
As discussed above, existing theories (e.g., Gross and Thompson 2007; Parrott
1993) suggest that there are four main types of affect regulation: intrinsic affect-
Curr Psychol (2011) 30:53–73 57
To understand some of the important differences between the four types of strategies, we
propose a theoretical model regarding the nomological net of the factors (Figure 2).
First, we predict links between the factors and personality traits. Because extraversion
and neuroticism are associated with proneness to experiencing positive and negative
affect, respectively (Gross and John 2003), and people are motivated to experience
affect that is trait-consistent as this validates their self-theory (Tamir 2005), we expect
that individuals high in extraversion and those low in neuroticism would be more
likely to use intrinsic affect-improving strategies, and less likely to use intrinsic affect-
worsening strategies. With respect to extrinsic affect regulation, we consider two key
traits: agreeableness and interpersonal control. As individuals high in agreeableness
have a strong need to develop and maintain positive relationships (Diefendorff et al.
2005), and those with high interpersonal control typically use their control to develop
social relationships and maintain harmony (Paulhus 1983), we expect that individuals
high in these traits would be more likely to use extrinsic affect-improving strategies,
and less likely to use extrinsic affect-worsening.
We also include potential outcomes of affect regulation in the model. Because
intrinsic strategies are directed towards changing one’s affect, it seems likely that the
use of intrinsic affect regulation will be related to people’s current levels of affect
(Parkinson and Totterdell 1999). There is also preliminary evidence suggesting that
engaging in extrinsic affect regulation may have congruent self-consequences for
strategy agents’ affect, as a result of actual and anticipated feedback from strategy
targets (Niven et al. 2007). Thus we expect that intrinsic and extrinsic affect-
improving strategies will be positively associated with current levels of affect, while
intrinsic and extrinsic affect-worsening strategies will be negatively related to affect.
We also anticipate links between the use of intrinsic strategies and two further
outcomes: emotional exhaustion, which is a state of depletion resulting from
excessive demands (Maslach and Jackson 1981); and health-related impairments,
which refer to problems in performing daily activities as a result of physical and
emotional health issues (Ware et al. 1996). Performing acts of self control, like
intrinsic affect regulation, can be physically and emotionally demanding (Muraven
and Baumeister 2000), and the sustained use of intrinsic affect regulation may have
negative implications for mental and physical well-being (Grandey 2003). Thus, we
To determine whether our new measure produces valid data, we conducted two
studies. Study 1 presents the development of the new measure and tests the proposed
factor structure (H1). Study 2 retests the proposed factor structure (H1) in a different
population, in which affect regulation is a core daily activity, and tests the
convergence between the factors and existing measures of affect regulation (H2-5)
and theoretically related constructs (H6-9).
regulation. Third, the distinctions made in each framework are salient to everyday
understandings of affect regulation, because they were developed by testing
theoretical predictions against laypersons’ perceptions of similarities and differences
amongst a wide range of strategies.
Parkinson and Totterdell’s (1999) framework primarily distinguishes intrinsic
strategies according to the implementation medium of the strategy (cognitive or
behavioral). A secondary distinction is made according to where a person directs his
or her attention (towards an issue or affective state or away from it). Niven et al.’s
(2009) framework primarily distinguishes between extrinsic strategies according to
the motive behind their use (affect-improving or affect-worsening), and then
distinguishes between strategies that engage the target’s attention on an issue or
feeling and those that divert attention onto the target’s relationship with the agent.
Further categories of strategy types are distinguished at lower levels of both
frameworks, based on the means used to achieve affect regulation (e.g., humor,
distraction, support, rejection).
The items in our scale were based on affect regulation strategies located within
these two theoretical frameworks. The strategies represented real life examples of
affect regulation, as they were generated by the framework authors using interview
and questionnaire studies involving a wide range of people who were asked about
the types of strategies they used to influence affect. We formed our item set to reflect
the major distinctions made in the frameworks. Thus there were equal numbers of
cognitive and behavioral strategies, and engagement and diversion strategies, within
the item set. The authors of each framework had identified the prototypicality of
strategies within each distinct category of affect regulation, and so prototypical
strategy examples were largely used. Because the intrinsic affect regulation
framework of Parkinson and Totterdell (1999) did not cover strategies to worsen
affect, we obtained such strategies from other relevant research (e.g., Parrott 1993;
Riediger et al. 2009), and a focus group of psychology researchers, who were asked
to discuss situations under which people might try to make themselves feel worse,
and then to anonymously note down examples of strategies they had used or heard
of others using.
The result of this process was a list of 24 strategies, six representing each of
the four types of affect regulation (the final set of items is listed in Table 1, and
the further omitted items are listed in the results of Study 1). Content validity was
confirmed by providing a group of ten expert psychology researchers with a
description of the four categories of affect regulation and asking them to place
each strategy into a category. The researchers each placed all of the strategies in
the expected category, and so no strategies were dropped at this point. The
strategies were rephrased as scale items that emphasized the intentional nature of
the act (e.g., ‘laughing’ became ‘I laughed to try to make myself feel better’). The
instructions for the scale asked individuals to report the extent to which they had
used the strategies over the past four weeks to try to change their own feelings
(intrinsic items) or someone else’s feelings (extrinsic items). The intrinsic and
extrinsic subscales were presented separately because of the differing instructions.
All items had the same response options marked on a five-point Likert-like scale
(1 ‘Not at all’, 2 ‘Just a little’, 3 ‘Moderate amount’, 4 ‘Quite a lot’, 5 ‘A great
deal’).
Curr Psychol (2011) 30:53–73 61
Method
Results
Table 1 Factor pattern coefficients and standardized item coefficients for Study 1
Item EFA factor pattern coefficients (N=240) CFA model results (N=311)
Extrinsic affect- Extrinsic affect- Intrinsic affect- Intrinsic affect- Standardized item Standard
improving (Factor 1) worsening (Factor 2) worsening (Factor 3) improving (Factor 4) coefficient error
Extrinsic subscale
9. I spent time with someone .77 .81 .03
8. I listened to someone’s problems .67 .75 .03
6. I discussed someone’s positive .66 .77 .03
characteristics
3. I did something nice with someone .64 .66 .04
1. I gave someone helpful advice .55 .64 .04
7. I made someone laugh .44 .69 .04
5. I explained to someone how they had .72 .59 .06
hurt myself or others
4. I acted annoyed towards someone .65 .66 .06
2. I told someone about their .59 .58 .06
shortcomings
Intrinsic subscale
5. I thought about my shortcomings .82 .91 .02
1. I looked for problems in my current .71 .75 .03
situation
8. I thought about negative experiences .67 .79 .03
4. I expressed cynicism .64 .68 .04
9. I thought about something nice .73 .81 .03
10. I thought of positive aspects of my .70 .80 .03
situation
Curr Psychol (2011) 30:53–73
Table 1 (continued)
Item EFA factor pattern coefficients (N=240) CFA model results (N=311)
Extrinsic affect- Extrinsic affect- Intrinsic affect- Intrinsic affect- Standardized item Standard
improving (Factor 1) worsening (Factor 2) worsening (Factor 3) improving (Factor 4) coefficient error
characteristics
3. I laughed .57 .58 .04
7. I sought support from others .57 .50 .05
In the final scale, items were presented in two separate subscales in the order specified in this table
63
64 Curr Psychol (2011) 30:53–73
1 2 3 4
*p<.05; **p<.01. Figures in brackets above the diagonal are standard errors
related to the underlying latent factor (see Table 1). When the whole sample was tested
using CFA, χ2(146, N=551)=370.08, p<.01, the results indicated similarly good
model fit for the specified four-factor model (RMSEA=.06; SRMR=.05; CFI=.94),
and competing 3-, 2- and 1- factor models did not yield a better fit to the data.
Correlations between the latent factors are shown in Table 2. Although most of
the correlations were significant, only the correlation between Factor 1 (extrinsic
affect-improving) and Factor 4 (intrinsic affect-improving) was high (r=.68, p<.01),
and no correlations exceeded the .85 recommended limit (Kline 1998). Discriminant
factorial validity was then confirmed by checking the confidence intervals (± two
standard errors) around the correlation estimate between the latent factors (Anderson
and Gerbing 1988). As none of the intervals included 1.00, this indicates that the
factors may be seen as discrete but related constructs. The internal consistency of
each factor was satisfactory (extrinsic affect-improving, α=.82; extrinsic affect-
worsening, α=.74; intrinsic affect-worsening, α=.80; intrinsic affect-improving,
α=.82), and none was improved by removing any scale item. Overall, this first test
of the EROS measure produced data that showed good internal reliability and
construct validity.
Method
Additional Measures
Affect Regulation Strategy Use Two established scales measured participants’ use of
strategies to improve their own affect. The first was the ERQ (Gross and John 2003),
a 10-item measure that assesses the functional strategy of reappraisal (e.g., ‘I control
my emotions by changing the way I think about the situation I’m in’; α=.80) and the
dysfunctional strategy of suppression (e.g., ‘I keep my emotions to myself’; α=.76),
on a seven-point scale ranging from ‘Strongly disagree’ to ‘Strongly agree’. The
second was the REQ (Phillips and Power 2007), a 21-item measure of how people
respond to their emotions that has four subscales relating to different types of
strategies: internal functional (e.g., ‘I put the situation into perspective’; α=.76),
external functional (e.g., ‘I ask others for advice’; α=.79), internal dysfunctional (e.g., ‘I
harm or punish myself in some way’; α=.61) and external dysfunctional (e.g., ‘I bully
other people’; α=.65). The scale is answered on a five-point scale ranging from ‘Never’
to ‘Always’.
Affect Regulation Ability Two subscales of the SEI (Tett et al. 2005) were used to
assess perceived affect regulation ability. The first subscale comprised 12 items
concerning perceived ability to regulate one’s own emotions (e.g., ‘I can keep myself
calm even in highly stressful situations’; α=.87), while the second comprised 12
items regarding perceived ability to regulate others’ emotions (e.g., ‘When a friend is
feeling down, I can usually find a way to cheer them up’; α=.82). For all items,
participants rated their agreement on a six-point scale ranging from ‘Disagree very
much’ to ‘Agree very much’.
Traits Extraversion, neuroticism and agreeableness were measured using the short
version of the Big Five Inventory (Rammstedt and John 2007). Participants
responded to two items assessing each of the three traits (e.g., ‘I see myself as
someone who is outgoing, sociable’ for extraversion) on a five-point scale ranging
from ‘Disagree strongly’ to ‘Agree strongly’. Interpersonal control was measured
using the Interpersonal Control scale from the Spheres of Control battery (Paulhus
1983). The scale comprised 10 items (e.g., ‘I find it easy to play an important part in
66 Curr Psychol (2011) 30:53–73
most group situations’; α=.77), and participants rated agreement on a six-point scale
from ‘Disagree very much’ to ‘Agree very much’.
Current Affect A six-item measure assessed current affect. The measure comprised
mood state items from the UWIST checklist (Matthews et al. 1990) representing
each end of hedonic tone (‘Happy’ and ‘Gloomy’), tense arousal (‘Anxious’ and
‘Calm’) and energetic arousal (‘Energetic’ and ‘Sluggish’), with negative items
reverse coded. Participants indicated the extent to which they were currently feeling
each state on a seven-point scale from ‘Not at all’ to ‘A great extent’ (α=.82).
Emotional Exhaustion The four highest loading items from the emotional
exhaustion subscale of the Maslach Burnout Inventory (Maslach and Jackson
1981) were used to assess emotional exhaustion. Participants were asked how often
they had experienced the indicators of emotional exhaustion (e.g., ‘I felt emotionally
drained’) over the past four weeks, responding on a five-point scale ranging from
‘Never’ to ‘All of the time’ (α=.89).
Results
Survey 1 (N=227)
Intrinsic affect-improving 3.08 0.93 –
Intrinsic affect-worsening 1.31 0.57 .14* –
Extrinsic affect-improving 3.53 0.88 .53** .13 –
Extrinsic affect-worsening 1.20 0.43 .03 .38** .09 –
Age 38.50 12.90 −.01 −.08 .20** −.20**
Sex (0=male, 1=female) 0.59 0.49 .12 .05 .23** −.21**
Reappraisal 4.88 1.03 .41** −.17* .27** −.11
Suppression 3.92 1.31 −.12 .05 −.06 .05
Internal functional 3.13 0.61 .33** −.09 .26** −.08
Internal dysfunctional 1.83 0.53 .03 .37** .07 .32**
External functional 2.95 0.74 .40** −.02 .34** −.05
External dysfunctional 1.31 0.32 −.06 .31** −.06 .44**
Affect regulation ability (self) 4.10 0.86 −.07 −.31** .03 −.23**
Affect regulation ability 4.60 0.61 .04 −.23** .17* −.23**
(other)
Extraversion 3.50 1.05 .12 −.18* .16* −.11
Neuroticism 2.42 1.02 −.04 .25** −.08 .17*
Agreeableness 3.98 0.89 .09 −.18* .10 −.26**
Interpersonal control 4.33 0.68 .09 −.28** .17** −.24**
Affect 4.78 1.18 .06 −.28** .05 −.20**
Emotional exhaustion 2.55 1.04 .17* .30** .04 .10
Health-related impairments 1.92 0.82 .22** .26** .08 .22**
Survey 2 (N=50)
Extrinsic affect-improving 3.34 0.91 – – –
Extrinsic affect-worsening 1.40 0.68 – – .11 –
Relationship partners’ 3.36 0.95 – – .39** −.05
perceptions of extrinsic
affect-improving
Relationship partners’ 1.35 0.58 – – .14 .51**
perceptions of extrinsic
affect-worsening
*p<.05; **p<.01
positively related to the perceived ability to regulate others’ affect (r=.17, p<.05) and
to relationship partners’ perceptions of participants’ use of extrinsic affect-improving
strategies (r=.39, p<.01). Also as predicted the EROS extrinsic affect-worsening
factor (H5) was negatively related to perceived ability to regulate others’ affect
(r=−.23, p<.01) and was positively related to relationship partners’ perceptions of
participants’ use of extrinsic affect-worsening strategies (r=.51, p<.01). As such,
Hypotheses 2 –5 were largely supported.
68 Curr Psychol (2011) 30:53–73
Table 4 Partial correlations between EROS Factors and hypothesized outcomes in Study 2 (N=227)
Controlling for Gross and John’s (2003) reappraisal and suppression factors
Affect −.09 −.26** .01 −.19**
Emotional .27** .28** .13 .12
exhaustion
Health-related .28** .26** .12 .23**
impairments
Controlling for Phillips and Power’s (2007) internal and external functional and dysfunctional factors
Affect −.11 −.14* .08 −.14*
Emotional .26** .18* .09 .01
exhaustion
Health-related .29** .15* .07 .14*
impairments
Controlling for Tett et al.’s (2005) ability to regulate own emotions and ability to regulate others’ emotions
factors
Affect −.06 −.14* .03 −.14*
Emotional .16* .18* .09 .03
exhaustion
Health-related .22** .17* .11 .14*
impairments
*p<.05; **p<.01
Curr Psychol (2011) 30:53–73 69
suggests that the EROS factors have incremental validity, and that three of the four
factors (all except extrinsic affect-improving) add unique explanatory power for
important outcomes like emotional exhaustion and health-related impairments.
Additional analyses (shown in Table 3) revealed that both age and sex were
related to the use of extrinsic but not intrinsic strategies. Age was positively related
to the extrinsic affect-improving factor (r=.20, p<.01) and negatively related to the
extrinsic affect-worsening factor (r=−.20, p<.01), while females were more likely to
use extrinsic affect-improving strategies (r=.23, p<.01) and less likely to use
extrinsic affect-worsening strategies (r=−.21, p<.01). Overall, the data collected in
Study 2 provide further evidence for the internal reliability and construct validity of
scores on the EROS factors.
Discussion
Acknowledgement The support of the Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC) UK is gratefully
acknowledged (RES-060-25-0044: “Emotion regulation of others and self [EROS]”). We also thank Adam
Thompson, Nadia Hanif and Paul Woodhead for their help with data collection.
References
Anderson, J. C., & Gerbing, D. W. (1988). Structural equation modeling in practice: a review and
recommended two-step approach. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 411–423.
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good.
Review of General Psychology, 5, 323–370.
Comrey, A. L., & Lee, H. B. (1992). A first course in factor analysis (2nd ed.). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
Conway, J. M., & Huffcutt, A. I. (2003). A review and evaluation of exploratory factor analysis practices
in organizational research. Organizational Research Methods, 6, 147–168.
Cronbach, L. J., & Meehl, P. E. (1955). Construct validity in psychological tests. Psychological Bulletin,
52, 281–302.
Diefendorff, J. M., Croyle, M. H., & Gosserand, R. H. (2005). The dimensionality and antecedents of
emotional labor strategies. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 66, 339–357.
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Guthrie, I. K., & Reiser, M. (2000). Dispositional emotionality and
regulation: their role in predicting quality of social functioning. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 78, 136–157.
72 Curr Psychol (2011) 30:53–73
Feldman, G. C., Joormann, J., & Johnson, S. L. (2008). Responses to positive affect: a self-report measure
of rumination and dampening. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32, 507–525.
Francis, L. E., Monahan, K., & Berger, C. (1999). A laughing matter? The uses of humor in medical
interactions. Motivation and Emotion, 23, 154–177.
Garnefski, N., Kraaij, V., & Spinhoven, Ph. (2001). Negative life events, cognitive emotion regulation and
depression. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 1311–1327.
Grandey, A. A. (2003). When “the show must go on”: surface acting and deep acting as determinants of
emotional exhaustion and peer-rated service delivery. Academy of Management Journal, 46, 86–96.
Gross, J. J. (1998). Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: divergent consequences for
experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 224–237.
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications
for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348–362.
Gross, J. J., & Muñoz, R. F. (1995). Emotion regulation and mental health. Clinical Psychology: Science
and Practice, 2, 151–164.
Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In J. J. Gross (Ed.),
Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 3–24). New York: Guilford.
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis:
conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55.
Kline, R. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York: Guilford.
Koole, S. L. (2009). The psychology of emotion regulation: an integrative review. Cognition and Emotion,
23, 4–41.
Maslach, C., & Jackson, S. E. (1981). The measurement of experienced burnout. Journal of Occupational
Behavior, 2, 99–113.
Matthews, G., Jones, D. M., & Chamberlain, A. G. (1990). Refining the measurement of mood: the
UWIST Mood adjective checklist. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 17–42.
Mikolajczak, M., Petrides, K. V., Coumans, N., & Luminet, O. (2009). An experimental investigation of
the moderating effects of trait emotional intelligence on laboratory-induced stress. International
Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 9, 455–477.
Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: does self-
control resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126, 247–259.
Niven, K., Totterdell, P., & Holman, D. (2007). Changing moods and influencing people: the use and
effects of emotional influence behaviors at HMP Grendon. Prison Service Journal, 172, 39–45.
Niven, K., Totterdell, P., & Holman, D. (2009). A conceptual classification of controlled interpersonal
affect regulation strategies. Emotion, 9, 498–509.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1991). A prospective study of depression and posttraumatic stress
symptoms after a natural disaster: the 1989 Loma Prieta earthquake. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 61, 115–121.
Parkinson, B., & Totterdell, P. (1999). Classifying affect regulation strategies. Cognition and Emotion, 13,
277–303.
Parrott, W. G. (1993). Beyond hedonism: Motives for inhibiting good moods and for maintaining bad
moods. In D. M. Wegner & J. W. Pennebaker (Eds.), Handbook of mental control (pp. 278–305).
Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.
Paulhus, D. (1983). Sphere-specific measures of perceived control. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 44, 1253–1265.
Phillips, K. F. V., & Power, M. J. (2007). A new self-report measure of emotion regulation in adolescents:
the regulation of emotions questionnaire. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 14, 145–156.
Rammstedt, B., & John, O. P. (2007). Measuring personality in one minute or less: a 10-item short version
of the Big Five inventory in English and German. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 203–212.
Riediger, M., Schmiedek, F., Wagner, G. G., & Lindenberger, U. (2009). Seeking pleasure and seeking
pain: age-related differences in pro- and contra-hedonic motivation from adolescence to old age.
Psychological Science, 20, 1529–1535.
Sukhodolsky, D. G., Golub, A., & Cromwell, E. N. (2001). Development and validation of the Anger
Rumination Scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 31, 689–700.
Tamir, M. (2005). Don't worry, be happy? Neuroticism, trait-consistent affect regulation, and performance.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 449–461.
Tett, R. P., Fox, K. E., & Wang, A. (2005). Development and validation of a self-report measure of
emotional intelligence as a multidimensional trait domain. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 31, 859–888.
Thoits, P. A. (1996). Managing the emotions of others. Symbolic Interaction, 19, 85–109.
Curr Psychol (2011) 30:53–73 73
Totterdell, P., & Leach, D. (2001). Negative mood regulation expectancies and sports performance: an
investigation involving professional cricketers. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 2, 249–265.
Vangelisti, A. L., Daly, J. A., & Rudnick, J. R. (1991). Making people feel guilty in conversations:
techniques and correlates. Human Communication Research, 18, 3–39.
Ware, J. E., Kosinski, M., & Keller, S. D. (1996). A 12-Item Short-Form Health Survey: construction of
scales and preliminary tests of reliability and validity. Medical Care, 34, 220–233.