We Are Electric Sally Adee - Livro Electoma
We Are Electric Sally Adee - Livro Electoma
We Are Electric Sally Adee - Livro Electoma
canongate.co.uk
The right of Sally Adee to be identified as the author of this work has been
asserted by her in accordance with the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act
1988
Introduction
We Are Electric is about the natural electricity that surges through all our
bodies, and the truly head-spinning ways the world will change if we learn how
to manipulate it. Over the next few hundred pages I will teach you about the
substance that courses through all living things, underpinning their every move
and intention. This natural electrical current predates nervous systems and
even humanity itself; it was coursing through the bodies of our ancestors long
before the first fishy mutants even squelched onto dry land. It is the most
ancient thing about us. It is among the most ancient things about life itself.
My brief foray into professional marksmanship is just one example of the
promise and peril of harnessing our body’s natural electricity. We are
fundamentally electrical creatures, but the full extent of our electrification
would shock you. It is hard to overstate how wholly and utterly your every
movement, perception, and thought are controlled by electrical signals. This is
not the electricity that comes from a battery or the kind that turns on the
lights and powers the dishwasher. That kind of electricity is made of electrons,
which are negatively charged particles flowing in a current.
The human body runs on a very different version: ‘bioelectricity’. Instead
of electrons, these currents are created by the movements of mostly positively
charged ions like potassium, sodium, and calcium. This is how all signals travel
within the brain and between it and every organ in the body via the nervous
system, enabling perception, motion, and cognition. It’s fundamental to our
ability to think and talk and walk and why our knee hurts after a fall, and why
the scraped skin heals. It’s what makes gummy bears taste sour, why we can
pick up a glass of water to wash away the taste, and how we know we were
thirsty in the first place.
The stuff that comes out of your wall socket is created by a power plant.
For the stuff in your body, the power plant is you. Every one of the 40 trillion
cells in your body is its own little battery with its own little voltage: when it’s at
rest, the inside of a cell is (on average) around 70 millivolts more negatively
charged than the extracellular soup outside. To keep it that way, the cell is
constantly shuttling ions in and out of the membrane that surrounds it, always
striving to maintain that -70mv. All of this may sound very petite to you,
somewhat beneath your notice. And, yes, at the scale of our lives, a difference
of 70 millivolts is insignificant; it’s about one thousandth of the amount of
electricity required to power a hearing aid. But from a neuron’s perspective, it
is anything but. When a nerve impulse comes roaring down a nerve fibre,
channels open in the neuron and millions of ions get instantly sucked through
them into and out of the extracellular space, taking all their charge with them.
The electrical field generated by this mass migration of charge works out to
about a million volts per metre, which at that scale would feel like passing an
entire bolt of lightning from one of your outstretched hands to the other. That’s
what it feels like to be every neuron in your body, every moment of your life.
Biologists have known for a long time that these kinds of bioelectrical
signals are responsible for all communication between the brain and the
nervous system: you can think of them as the telephone wires that help the
brain’s command centre communicate with your muscles to operate your
limbs.
But bioelectricity isn’t confined to our brains. Over the past couple of
decades it has become clear that these signals are pressed into service by every
cell in your body, not just those that govern your perception and motion.
Each of your skin cells has its own voltage, which it combines with
neighbouring cells to generate an electrical field. You can even measure your
skin’s electricity with a voltmeter: just stretch a piece of skin and connect it to
electrodes and the ‘skin battery’ will light up a light bulb. You could power the
same bulb with a prostate battery. Or a breast battery. When that field is
disrupted by injury, you can feel it. That tingling when you bite your tongue or
the inside of your cheek? It’s the wound current, calling to the surrounding
tissue to send help.
Similarly, the cells in your bones are electric. Your teeth are electric. Your
organs are electric – and so is the coat of epithelial tissue that hugs each one.
Blood cells too. Every single one is a microscopic power plant generating a
tiny voltage to communicate within and among themselves.
We used to think those non-nervous-system cells mainly used bioelectric
signalling for trivial janitorial and maintenance tasks – for example, waste
disposal and energy management. But new research is making it increasingly
clear that they do far more. You and I are so much more electric than is
commonly acknowledged.
Recently it has been discovered that electrical signals also send out beacons
as we grow in the womb to guide us into the eventual shape we will take – two
arms, two legs, two ears, a nose. When this signal is interrupted in utero, things
go terribly wrong, so scientists are now working out ways to prevent
physiological birth defects by retuning our electrics. And what we can do for
birth, we can also do for death: cancer cells have their own unusual voltage,
and recent evidence indicates that they use electric signalling to communicate
about their host environment. Disrupting these signals could keep cancer cells
from metastasising.
Nor is this natural electricity confined to us animals – the same signals
have been detected in everything from algae to E. coli. Plants use them to send
messages across far-flung parts of them-selves, warning of predators and
turning on defences. Fungi use them to communicate when their delicate
tendrils have sussed out good sources of food. Bacteria use them to make
decisions about when to grow their communities into antibiotic-resistant
strongholds. Even organisms we haven’t quite figured out how to
taxonomically classify – we shove them into an all-purpose box labelled
‘protists’ – use these electrical communication signals.
I tell you all this to underscore that ‘bioelectricity’ is no mere metaphor, no
elegant stretching of a humdrum biochemical truth. You and I are literally
electric. The basis of all life is electric. When our cellular battery runs out, we
all die.
So what if we learned how to control the switch?
If you still can’t quite get your head around this (or remain suspicious of my
enthusiastic contentions), you are not alone. The entire history of bioelectricity
has been marked – and in some ways defined – by the scepticism levelled at
the researchers from both the physics and biology establishment.
History is filled with tales of the uphill battles biologists faced when trying
to suggest that biological phenomena have electrical underpinnings. Today,
looking at EEG readouts of the brain’s activity is commonplace, but you might
not be aware of the ridicule its inventor, Hans Berger, endured – or that he
ended up dying by suicide in 1941 before he could see how his device changed
the world. Even the most quotidian electrical functions of electricity in the
body were only accepted after a knock-down, drag-out fight; in the 1960s,
Peter Mitchell spent ten years and a great deal of his own money to build his
own lab to convince the scientific establishment that electricity is central to the
way a cell generates energy. (He was one of the few to live long enough to see
his ideas achieve acclaim, receiving the Nobel Prize for Chemistry in 1978.)
Maybe all this scepticism can be traced to the contentious battle that
attended bioelectricity’s origin story. Luigi Galvani’s discovery in the late
eighteenth century that electricity is what lets us move our muscles is perhaps
the original electro-controversy: you may have heard of his experiments
zapping frogs, but you might not know that doubts about his findings started a
scientific war that divided all of Europe. This origin story of bioelectricity
profoundly shaped the way subsequent generations of scientists would
approach the topic, not least by shaping the structure of science itself. As a
result, the scientific knowledge of the electrical underpinnings of life is now
scattered across a wide range of disciplines, many of which think the others are
peddling poppycock.
Even today, many biologists probably don’t know the whole story of
bioelectricity. In 1995, when Mustafa Djamgoz, a cancer researcher at Imperial
College London, first proposed his theory that electrical signals were involved
in cancer, his colleagues openly dismissed his ideas. Even now, with research
awards piling up around him, Djamgoz frequently finds himself re-explaining
his research – and needing to start from scratch because sometimes the same
concept that elicits a ‘well, obviously’ from one researcher sounds like science
fiction to another.
This reflects a set of calcified notions embedded in the framework of
science: biologists stick to biology, leaving the study of electricity to the
physicists and engineers. They just don’t speak the same language. ‘If you
major in biology, you get maybe half a semester of physics, if that,’ says
another cancer biophysicist, Richard Nuccitelli. ‘You don’t even touch
electrical engineering.’ And forget about computer science. This might seem
like an obvious and unproblematic division of labour, but it means aspiring
PhD students in physics are taught about Tesla and his alternating currents,
but not about the bioelectricity running through their own bodies – and
biology students get neither. This tacit assumption that each field should ‘stay
in their lane’ has been putting limitations both on biology and scientific
advancement for decades. What we need is a new framework to bring the
body’s different electrical parameters under one roof and study them
coherently, together.
Call it the electrome.
N ormally, it’s difficult to muddle together a coherent story out of all the
complex mixtures of culture and history that go into making
something the way it is today. But in the case of the confusion about
bioelectricity, there’s an identifiable chain of causality: a savage battle that
helped split science into the constituent disciplines we see today, pitting
biologists and physicists against one another in a death match that ultimately
determined who gained custody of electricity. Biology lost, physics won, and
the consequences would ripple through the next 200 years of science. This
original schism profoundly shaped the way subsequent generations of
scientists approached the idea of electricity in biology.
CHAPTER 1
Artificial vs Animal:
Galvani, Volta, and the battle for
electricity
By the 1800s, we knew little more about our bodies than we had a full
millennium earlier. Meanwhile, the scientific revolution had taken the
understanding of electricity from strength to strength.
Like animal spirits, electrical phenomena had been observed for centuries
without generating great insight. The Ancient Greeks, for example, had
noticed strange stones that seemed to pull metal to them as if by an invisible
force. They had seen that when lightning struck people, it often killed them.
Electric eels were known to deliver a fulsome shock to their prey. Then there
was amber – the insect-trapping resin that also had a strange tendency to
attract bits of dust and fluff, the same way the stones attracted metal. Give the
amber a vigorous little rub, you might get a little zap and see a spark. But
before the seventeenth century, all these observations had not been compiled
into any kind of explanatory framework.
In fact, electricity got its name long before we understood how it was
involved in any of the above. The word was coined in 1600 by William Gilbert,
who – in keeping with what I mentioned earlier about the disciplines –
identified as a physician, physicist, and natural philosopher. He borrowed from
the Ancient Greek word elektron, for amber, owing to that material’s unique
ability to reliably elicit the magic spark.
The scientific revolution vastly upgraded the tools to investigate the
phenomenon. In 1672, Otto von Guericke invented the first device that made
it possible for scientists to generate electricity themselves: an ‘electrostatic
generator’ was a glass globe you could rub with silk to accumulate a small
amount of electrical charge. Touch it and you’d get a zap. (This, incidentally, is
where we get the phrase ‘static electricity’. The globe trapped electricity on its
surface so it wouldn’t go anywhere – it didn’t move. It was in stasis.)
Electrostatic generators allowed the accumulated electricity to be dispelled in
bigger jolts than amber, and that allowed people for the first time to decide
how, when, and where to direct the jolts. More machines followed, some
making it easier to charge the generator by having hand cranks, so your arms
wouldn’t get tired from all that rubbing glass with silk. Bigger glass tubes
yielded stronger jolts. The shock they generated was weak, but it was enough
to start a century of parlour-game science, from the ‘kissing Venus’ – an
electrified woman whose kisses stung gentlemen’s lips with a trivial zap – to
young boys charged up to attract bits of paper and other flotsam as if by
magic.
But all of these generators had the same problem: the very act of touching
these sources of accumulated static electricity released it all in one go (which is
also what’s happened when touching your doorknob zaps you with a sharp
spark of pain). There was no way to store up a large quantity of electricity for
later use.
About a century after the first electrostatic generator, several scientists
separately converged on the idea of a special jar that could siphon the
mysterious invisible substance from a generator and store it for later. To avoid
the thorny question of paternity, the new invention was dubbed the Leyden jar,
an indirect credit to Pieter van Musschenbroek, who did a lot of the early work
in this Dutch city. Scientists competed to see who could concentrate the most
electricity in their jar, because of course they did, and this had exactly the
unfortunate consequences you might expect. When van Musschenbroek
stuffed his Leyden jar to capacity, rather like overpacking a suitcase, it
promptly exploded on him. The shock was enough to send the temporarily
paralysed physicist to bed for two days.
As people got better at stuffing these increasingly capacious vessels to
capacity, Leyden jar demonstrations grew progressively more dramatic, from a
crowd of 200 monks connected by lengths of iron wire and shocked by a
single Leyden jar, to a practical joke in which a specially designed wine glass
was electrified for the amusement of picnic guests (less fun for its unfortunate
target).7 Though high society loved these demonstrations, even they agreed
that electricity was at best a novelty, and no one could quite deduce how this
circus of wonders might prove useful . . . until the mid-1740s, when a Scottish
electric showman called Dr Spencer sent his apparatus to the Philadelphia
residence of a young Benjamin Franklin.8
Franklin is often credited with single-handedly turning the carnival of
electricity into a science. And while it is a bit more complicated than that,
Franklin’s famous kite demonstration did begin the unification process that
proved that different electrical phenomena – lightning, amber, electrostatic
generators – were just different manifestations of the same ethereal substance.
Franklin – famous polymath and politician – was among the vanguard of
investigators trying to develop a grand unified theory of electricity that would
link ‘natural electricity’ (lightning) to the stuff produced by generators and
stuffed into Leyden jars (‘artificial electricity’). He attached a key to a long
string, suspended by a kite, during a lightning storm. If he could charge a
Leyden jar with the proceeds of a lightning storm, his point would be proved.
It was a stupendously dangerous experiment, but it worked so well that kids
are still forced to read about it in school. Upshot: lightning was electricity.
Franklin’s experiment was hugely consequential and helped pave the way
for a new understanding that was formalised into a branch of science, whose
practitioners referred to themselves as electricians. (This word carried a rather
more glamorous connotation back then – you can think of eighteenth-century
electricians as the ‘rocket scientists’ of their day.) What’s more, there was now
an understanding of electricity as an invisible fluid that could be collected in a
jar, cross vast distances, and travel along strings, hollow or not.
What else was electricity? By 1776, people had begun to wonder if this
‘immaterial fluid’ wasn’t germane to those animal spirits everyone had been
wondering about. That year, the notion got its first bit of supporting evidence
when John Walsh experimented with an electric eel.
Walsh was a classic natural philosopher: colonel, MP in the House of
Commons, all-round rich person. He moved in the same circles as Franklin,
who was just starting to cultivate an obsession with electric fish. After their
electrical organs had been described, Franklin became convinced that the
shock the creature delivered was another manifestation of the phenomenon of
electricity, so he convinced Walsh to ‘devote his scientific energies’ (read: a
boatload of his ample fortune) to devising experiments that would prove ‘fish
electricity’ was real.9
The way to do that was to put an electric fish into a dark room and get it
to deliver its jolt – in the hope that doing so would yield a visible spark. That
would be the smoking gun. Incredibly, it seems Walsh was able to do it. Several
historical accounts by people deep in the audience at his 1776 demonstration
reported this convincing evidence that electric eels were, in fact, electric. The
British Evening Post reported ‘vivid flashes’.
While the experiment didn’t provide any direct evidence of a link between
‘fish electricity’ and anything that might be involved in human processes, the
idea was out there nonetheless: a form of electricity might be at work in the
action of nerves and muscles. If an eel could make a spark, perhaps we could
create our own internal sparks.
And that’s how electricity found Luigi Galvani.
Volte-face
Spectacular pseudoscience:
The fall and rise of bioelectricity
G iovanni Aldini was looking for the perfect body. Not something that
had been dragged out of a grave – it should be as fresh as possible to
minimise the dissipation of its vital powers. It shouldn’t be someone who had
died slowly, from one of the ‘putrefying diseases’ that might contaminate their
humours.1 Not too dismembered, either. The ideal body would be one whose
previous owner had been healthy and intact until the moment of death.
Aldini’s star had been rising in Europe as he demonstrated Galvani’s
experiments on much larger animals than frogs, to often macabre effect. In an
echo of some of the early electricity spectacles – but with a darker twist – he
had recently electrified a decapitated dog to entertain a crowd that included
royalty.2 He was desperate to prove that the animal electricity Galvani had
discovered was present in the same way in all animals – that what was true for
frogs was true for humans. He was willing to use Volta’s battery and any
amount of theatrics to prove it.
Aldini was in the right place at the right time: it was 1803 in the UK, and
the Murder Act had for well over half a century included a provision that
would serve up exactly the corpse he was after. After a convicted killer’s public
hanging, their naked body would be flayed in a public dissection. If that seems
over the top, it was absolutely intended to be – this ‘further terror and peculiar
mark of infamy’ had been added to give would-be murderers just that extra bit
of pause, all the better to prevent ‘the horrid crime’.3 It was unclear whether,
as Aldini would later write, it also better helped them atone for their sins, or
whether there was a more convenient secondary benefit; as there were laws
against digging up corpses, this law provided a steady stream of cadavers to
upskill medical students and lecturers at the Royal College of Surgeons.4 The
fellows there had invited Aldini from Italy to demonstrate the experiments
that had recently made him famous around Europe.5 They were happy to
supply the necessary materials. And so, after the convicted murderer George
Forster was hanged on the gallows at Newgate prison, his body was carried
across town to the Royal College of Surgeons, where Aldini nervously waited.
The room was crowded with luminaries, scientists, and gentlemen standing
elbow to elbow. Assisting Aldini in his efforts would be the rising star Joseph
Carpue, a surgeon and anatomist at the Duke of York’s Hospital who had
extended the invitation, and Mr Pass, the beadle of the Surgeons’ Company,
who was tasked with making sure all proper protocols were followed during a
dissection.6 But the crowds weren’t what had Aldini sweating; he was used to
performing in front of high society.
What was worrying him today was the cold: it was January, and the body
had been left hanging for an hour in temperatures two degrees below freezing.
The chill might stunt the flow of animal electricity through the body, rendering
his experiment a humiliating, public flop. He was putting his faith in the
enormous piles of alternating zinc and copper discs sitting on the slab where
Forster’s corpse was now laid out, ready to dispense their ‘galvanic juices’ into
the dead man’s nervous system.
Aldini moistened the tips of two metal wires attached to either end of the
pile by dipping them into saltwater. When he threaded them gingerly into
Forster’s ears, the results did not disappoint. The dead man’s jaw, according to
a report in The Times, began to quiver: ‘the adjoining muscles were horribly
contorted, and the left eye actually opened’, giving the impression of a ghastly,
lewd wink.7 Over the next several hours, Aldini’s team exposed every nerve
and muscle on the man’s body, from the thorax to the gluteus, for electrical
experimentation.
Forster wasn’t Aldini’s first criminal corpse. He had spent the previous year
in Bologna and Paris perfecting his galvanic technique on the heads and bodies
of other hanged and beheaded convicts, not to mention the scores of lambs,
dogs, oxen, and horses, living and dead, that joined Italy’s population of frogs
on his table. These animal experiments had given him the idea for an especially
dramatic demonstration.
When Aldini plugged one of the wires into the dead man’s rectum, the
convulsions that wracked the corpse were ‘much stronger than in the
preceding experiments’, Aldini wrote. So strong, in fact, ‘as almost to give an
appearance of reanimation’. At this point, according to The Times, ‘some of the
uninformed bystanders actually thought the wretched man was on the eve of
being restored to life’. Some in the audience clapped; others were deeply
disturbed. Mr Pass was so shaken by what he saw on the table that he went
home that night and died.8 As far as Aldini was concerned, the experiment had
been a success.
This spectacular public demonstration begat many copycats, and historians
trace a line from the Forster galvanisation to Mary Shelley’s idea for
Frankenstein. So it may come as a surprise that Aldini didn’t start out with the
goal of titillating vapid royalty by raising the dead. He was pushed down his
path by an altogether more noble impulse: to restore the reputation of his
beloved uncle. But not unlike Dr Frankenstein, his obsession caused him to
reach beyond what science could provide, and eventually turned him into a
mockery. He would become a scientific pariah. Instead of reviving his family’s
legacy as well as decapitated bodies, his experiments would play a major role in
banishing the serious study of animal electricity into a desert of quacks and
mountebanks for the next four decades.
Aldini’s gambit
Aldini’s loyalty to Galvani wasn’t just a matter of family honour. He had also
been his uncle’s closest and most important scientific collaborator. He had
written some of the anatomist’s famous communications himself – some of
the liveliest ripostes between ‘Galvani’ and Volta had actually involved just
Volta and Aldini.9 But after Galvani’s death, few champions remained to take
forward the serious scientific inquiry into animal electricity.
In 1801, Napoleon’s French Académie launched a commission (the fifth in
as many years), offering a prize of 60,000 francs for anyone who could do for
animal electricity what Volta had done for the metallic or artificial variety.10 (In
today’s money, this would have been worth around £860,000.) Generous as it
was, however, the prize went unclaimed. No one was in the position to make
something as consequential as a battery for animal electricity. In addition, the
false perception that acceptance of metallic contact theory and animal
electricity had to be mutually exclusive meant, for many, that because Volta (so
heavily favoured by Napoleon) had been demonstrably right, Galvani must by
definition have been wrong.
Aldini was desperate to stop this becoming the official received wisdom.
He had understood the scientific foundation his uncle was trying to build, and
he had noted the sleight of hand that undermined it. In particular, Aldini was
still pained that their most triumphant paper – the one Spallanzani had hailed
as ‘one of the most beautiful and valuable of the eighteenth-century Physics’,
in which Galvani had showed up Volta once and for all by successfully proving
that nerve electricity could excite nerve tissue – was already being forgotten.
This was the paper that should have put the lie to Volta’s insistence that the
only reason contractions could be evoked in a dead frog was that some version
of metallic electricity was generated by the meeting of two dissimilar kinds of
meat. Instead, the paper had been buried under the fanfare around the voltaic
pile.
And so, Aldini’s initial investigations after his uncle’s death focused on
buttressing the basic science underlying this experiment, and how it could
advance a deeper understanding of animal electricity. He had assumed the
chair of physics at Bologna in 1798, just before Galvani passed away. This was
a prestigious post from which to carry on his uncle’s work, and Aldini used it
to launch the Galvanic Society of Bologna.
Galvani had experimented almost exclusively on frogs. Aldini’s first
experiments therefore extended his uncle’s investigations into warm-blooded
mammals. His 1804 publication Essai théorique et expérimental sur le galvanisme is
filled with long, repetitive accounts of experiments in which he and his
Galvanic Society collaborators sought to understand ‘intra-animal’
electrification. In one characteristic experiment, he placed several calves’ heads
in an electrically conducting line called a ‘series’, and used the resulting animal
current to violently electrify a dead frog. But when he tried to reverse the
experiment, applying the animal electricity of frog nerves to the decapitated
heads of oxen, he found the results less dramatic and even disappointing. All
these experiments successfully replicated Galvani’s original idea that the same
electrical substance coursed through all animals, but none yielded grand
dramatic outcomes or novel insights.
At some point, it seems to have become clear to Aldini that to maintain
excitement about scientific galvanism, he would need to do what the five
commissions hadn’t been able to: find a way to make his uncle’s discoveries
medically relevant for humans. It was around then that his focus rather quickly
shifted, revealing a sudden new appreciation of the ‘galvanic juices’ dispensed
by Volta’s pile. ‘The battery imagined by Professor Volta gave me the idea of a
cleaner means than any of the ones we have used so far to estimate the action
of the vital forces,’ he recalled in the 1804 Essai.11
It must have been hard for Aldini to hold his nose and use the instrument
of his uncle’s doom, but once he got the hang of it, he was prolific. He used
the pile’s ability to dispense a steady flow of electricity to stage big, dramatic
experiments on dead animals. He inserted wires into their rectal cavities, often
detailing the inevitable violent expulsion of faeces that followed. He also
began to experiment with touching different areas of the animals’ brains, as
well as his own; when he administered a jolt from the pile to his own cranium,
it led to a few days of insomnia but also a strange feeling of elation.
Such experiments fascinated the other members of the Galvanic Society: if
a jolt to the head could make Aldini feel euphoric, what else could it do? They
analysed and repeated these kinds of experiments until they eventually accreted
into new theories about how electrical ministrations could improve ailments.
Most promising were epilepsy, a type of paralysis called chorea, and what was
then called ‘melancholy madness’, which we understand today as treatment-
resistant depression. Now they just needed test subjects.
In 1801, at Sant’Orsola Hospital in Bologna, Aldini found a twenty-seven-
year-old farmer called Luigi Lanzarini who had become catatonic with
melancholy madness and had been declared a lost cause.12 He shaved
Lanzarini’s head and stimulated the man’s scalp with a weak battery. Over the
next month he slowly increased the current, and Lanzarini’s symptoms seemed
to lessen, eventually enough for him to be released into Aldini’s custody. After
about a month, Aldini deemed him well enough to send back home to his
family.
Word of this achievement spread quickly enough that, by 1802, French
scientists founded their own Parisian branch of the Galvanic Society. They
devoted themselves to Aldini’s goal of elevating the reputation of galvanism as
a legitimate pursuit, by any means necessary. Joseph Carpue – the rising-star
surgeon who had assisted in the Forster experiment – reported that a M. La
Grave of the Parisian Galvanic Society had made a voltaic pile out of sixty
layers each of human brain, muscle, and hat material (you read that right)
moistened with salt water.13 Its effect was allegedly ‘decisive’ – generating a
current that provided yet another piece of evidence that animal electricity was
just as relevant and present in human tissues as it was in animal tissues.
It was never entirely possible to extricate galvanism from its associations
with woo and quackery – ‘a couple of [the Galvanic Society’s] members drifted
into “galvanic magic”,’ notes the historian Christine Blondel – but most of the
group’s research was greeted with interest by French and foreign scientific
journals and even encouraged.14 The attention-seeking experiments were
doing their job. Famous French psychiatrists began to consult Aldini about use
of the pile to restore their patients to health.
But by then, Aldini already had his eye on an entirely new patient
population: he started investigating electrification as a way to revive the dead.15
To be clear, his goal was never to stitch together some kind of undead golem –
Aldini was referring to the evidently reversible state of ‘suspended animation’
that followed accidental drowning, apoplexy, or asphyxia.16
Aldini was campaigning to get galvanism – specifically a jolt of electricity
to the head – included in the go-to methods for emergency resuscitation,
which included ammonia and a kind of proto-CPR that involved exhaling into
the lungs of the temporarily deceased. Adding an electric shock to either of
these remedies, Aldini insisted, ‘will produce much greater effect than either of
them separately’. He also began to lobby to have electrification adopted as a
research tool to determine whether someone was truly, irreversibly dead. ‘It
would be desirable to establish by public authority, in all nations, by people
enlightened and able to make the necessary tests to determine whether death is
real or not.’
Of course, it’s well known today that his hunch was correct – electrical
defibrillation can bring a person back from what would have been certain
death. But Aldini’s speculations were not based on any specific mechanism or
evidence. He had no access to any of the information that we take for granted
200 years later: that a meaningful resuscitation is largely determined by
whether or not the patient is brain-dead; that it’s crucial to keep oxygen
moving to the brain; that there is a small window of time after which any
attempt at resuscitation becomes useless. Unfortunately, even the most
fundamental mechanism eluded Aldini – that the organ that should be
stimulated is the heart, not the brain. In fact, he repeatedly and explicitly
refuted the idea that the heart could be affected by electrification at all. His
focus on spectacle over basic science had misled him.17
So it came as no surprise to him that none of his experimental subjects –
human or animal – were ever shocked back to life. Neither was such an
outcome ever his goal for the hanged Forster. ‘Our object here was not to
produce reanimation, merely to obtain a practical knowledge of how far
Galvanism might be employed as an auxiliary to other means in attempts to
revive,’ he wrote in an 1803 account of the experiment. This writing also
provides clues to how he thought galvanisation might act to restore the dead
to life: by ‘re-establishing the muscular powers which have been suspended’, in
addition to preparing the lungs to receive resuscitation.
However, these were not the promises that got royalty crowding around
his table. It was the extras: the grimaces, the rectal probes, and the unspoken
possibility that, just maybe, one of the malefactors might rise from the dead.
Word had begun to spread of his work on deceased criminals in Bologna in
early 1802.18 He had managed to raise the forearm of a corpse to the height of
eight inches seventy-five minutes after death, ‘after we put in his hand a fairly
considerable weight, such as an iron pincer’. Stimulation of the arm caused the
hand to rise and curl into a position that looked for all the world like an
accusing finger pointing at the assembled audience, several of whom promptly
fainted. His colleagues at the Galvanic Society, Professors Giulio, Vassalli, and
Rossi, quickly repeated these experiments in Turin on three recently
decapitated men.19 It wasn’t long before such demonstrations piqued the
interest of the Royal Humane Society of London, though maybe not for the
reasons you might expect.
These days, a person who identifies as humane might find cause for
concern in the public dissection of dead criminals for entertainment. Not
these officials. They had more pressing concerns, like how to distinguish
between people who were genuinely dead and those who were going to wake
up.20 Before the wide availability and awareness of reliable methods of
resuscitation, burials could be a pretty hasty affair, and more than one
unfortunate had found themselves waking from a comatose or cataleptic state
(or just a deep and drunken sleep) inside a little box under six feet of earth.
Sometimes, their screaming was heard in time. (In one especially grisly case,
this fate befell the same poor woman twice.) ‘A host of facts have repeatedly
shown us that people were rushed to the tomb before death had irrevocably
struck them. Shouldn’t we give our fullest attention to preventing such deadly
events?’ Aldini had written, scandalised by the haunting stories of these
potentially ‘murderous burials’.21 Busy, commercial, and maritime Britain was
seeing a glut of drownings and mining accidents, and so for the Royal Humane
Society, having some way to distinguish the dead from the ‘not actually dead
even though they sure look like it’ was very much at the top of the agenda.
In late 1802, they sponsored Aldini for a long tour of Oxford and London,
and that’s how he came to be in the room with Mr Pass and Mr Forster that
chilly morning. Did he think the man would wake up there on the table?
Certainly not. Did he think the experiment would contribute to better
resuscitations? Sure – but there is little evidence in his writing of any empirical
understanding of how the stimulation might accomplish that. So in some way
he must have understood that what he was doing there that day was to a large
extent more showmanship than science.
Unfortunately, in trying to preserve what was left of his uncle’s nascent
science, Aldini failed. He did, however, have great success at blurring the line
between ‘legitimate’ galvanism and the unscientific electroquackery that had
begun to proliferate long before Galvani touched his first frog. And the quacks
came marching over it.
Almost as soon as the Leyden jar was invented in the mid-1740s, people were
convinced its zaps could dispense powerful cures.22 In Italy, its invention
prompted the opening of no fewer than three schools of electrical medicine.
Treatments there varied – some doctors simply shocked their patients and
hoped for the best; others hoped that electrical stimulation would boost
topical medicines’ ability to reach deep beneath the skin. The practice was said
to cure a spectrum of ailments so wide it bordered on the miraculous.
No ailment was spared the intervention of the Leyden jar, including but
not limited to gout, rheumatism, hysteria, headache, toothache, deafness,
blindness, irregular menstruation, diarrhoea, and, as ever, venereal disease.23
By the 1780s, electricity was rumoured to beget miracles, as in the report of a
couple who, after ten years of infertility, ‘regained hope through electricity
thanks to a few turns of the crank and some shocks in the appropriate parts’ –
the Abbé Bertholon, reporting this, ‘demurely did not specify which’.24 It
wasn’t just a continental fad: Britain’s medico-electric quack culture also
thrived, adding ‘weak ligaments’, testicular and urinary conditions, and ague
(that’s ‘the shivers’ to you) to the list of conditions electricity would relieve. It
was hard to beat the electrical apparatus devised in 1781 by the London
medical electrician James Graham, who guaranteed that his electrically
stimulating Celestial Bed, in a special wing of his Temple of Hymen, would
cure sterility and impotence.25 What put this electro-quackery a cut above the
usual sort was that no actual electricity was involved – merely the idea of it, as
Graham figured ‘the fashion of electrical vapours’ would suffice to cure his
patients.26 A night in this contraption would set you back £50, around £9,000
in today’s money,27 but if you still had money burning a hole in your pocket,
you could hit the temple gift shop on your way out for a take-home
aphrodisiac called Electrical Ether. (Given that the temple closed its doors
within two years, ‘homeopathic electricity’ does not seem to have been an
unqualified success.28)
But it was Galvani’s science that would inspire the most brazen of these
quacks: Elisha Perkins. ‘Among the delusions which have succeeded in
imposing on men of education and position, it is pre-eminent,’ wrote Francis
Shepherd, describing ‘Perkinism’ in the Popular Science Monthly in 1883.29
Perkins – who at the time of Galvani’s ‘De viribus’ publication was
practising medicine in Connecticut – had keenly followed accounts of the fight
raging on the Continent, and saw an opportunity in the argument about dual
metals.30 One that could make him rich. In 1796, he unveiled his contribution
to medical galvanism – a pair of sharp-tipped three-inch rods, one iron, one
brass, which he called ‘tractors’. Drag them over your afflicted parts for a few
minutes, and he claimed that you would soon be rid of rheumatism, pain,
inflammation, and even tumours. Perkins’ patented tractors took off in
America among the wealthy and influential. Even George Washington bought
a set for his family, along with Chief Justices Oliver Ellsworth and John
Marshall.31
The Connecticut Medical Society was having precisely none of it. In a
scorching rebuke of Perkins, they began expulsion procedures with a letter
stuffed to the margins with indignation. Castigating Perkins’ inventions as
‘delusive quackery’, they accused him of using the auspices of his Society
membership to spread his ‘mischief ’ to the south and abroad. ‘We consider all
such practices as barefaced imposition, disgraceful to the faculty, and delusive
to the ignorant,’ the Society fulminated. With that, they invited Perkins to
‘answer for his conduct, and render reasons why he should not be expelled
from the Society, for such disgraceful practices’.32
Whatever reasons Perkins may have offered, they failed to sway the
opinion of the Society, which in 1797 expelled him for violating their
prohibitions against nostrums (medical treatments prepared by unqualified
chancers). This goes some way to explaining why Perkins’ son soon took the
family business to the Continent. They were a wild success. In 1798, the Royal
Hospital in Copenhagen officially adopted the tractors for treatment. In
London, the Royal Society ‘accepted’ the tractors and the accompanying book
(there’s always a book), and by 1804, a Perkinean Institution had been
established. Members included fellows of the Royal Society. Soon a hospital
was built whose sole treatment was ‘tractoration’. Testimonials abounded: not
least from bishops and clergymen, whom Perkins had slyly provided with that
most ancient grift: free review samples. ‘I have used the tractors with success
in several cases in my own family,’ wrote one recipient, channelling the logic of
a multilevel marketing scheme. ‘Since experience has proved them, so no
reasoning can change the opinion.’
Over time, galvanism was conscripted into a pre-existing, ever-widening
gyre of pseudoscience that included Franz Mesmer’s animal magnetism,
hypnosis, and electrifying wearables, variously said to be associated with
earthquakes, dowsing, and volcanic activity. The whole line of research was
evidently starting to exasperate the public. In an 1809 poem, Lord Byron
lumped galvanism in with tractors eleven years after Galvani’s death,
apparently channelling the public mood that had started to conflate both:
‘A prostitution of galvanism’
In the end, Aldini’s efforts to clear his uncle’s name had the opposite effect.
They created a self-perpetuating spiral that destroyed what was left of
Galvani’s reputation as the father of animal electricity: the more quacks who
co-opted galvanism for their own purposes, the fewer legitimate researchers
were willing to be associated with the relationship between electricity and life;
the less serious research took place, the more ground was ceded to ludicrous
claims. As the years went on, an increasing number of scientists and historians
looking back on the Volta–Galvani feud began to ad lib historical details about
Galvani that confirmed the cynical new perspective on animal electricity – and
his ignorance in believing it to exist. One of the most enduring of these is the
pernicious origin myth that Galvani accidentally bumbled upon the idea of
animal electricity as his wife was preparing a frog for soup using a metal knife,
rather than in a decade-long series of ever more finely honed replications.
At the same time, the sciences were now rapidly beginning to diverge into
their fields, and biology was defining itself as a discipline. Not wanting to make
Galvani’s mistakes, those who pursued the legitimate study of biology recoiled
from electricity and returned to a largely descriptive anatomical and taxonomic
focus, a study of pieces instead of the forces and processes that govern the
whole.
People who studied electricity seriously – the electricians – were desperate
to restore respectability to their endeavours, and that meant separating the
object of their study from vitalistic connotations, and focusing strictly on the
advances being made by chemists and physicists thanks to Volta’s battery.
These multiplied quickly. In 1800, his proto-battery helped scientists
electrolyse water, decomposing it into hydrogen and oxygen. In 1808, an
improved version helped chemists to discover sodium and potassium and
alkaline earth metals. Equations were devised to define the relationships by
which electricity could act on the world. In 1816, the first working prototype
of a telegraph was built in Hammersmith, powered by voltaic piles. The
physicists and engineers had created an electric force field around themselves
that no one could touch, protecting themselves from both the biologists and
the charlatans.
Medical professionals also separated themselves from animal electricity in
due course, even as some of them continued to deploy the artificial electricity
that could zap people’s ailments. In the 1830s, a young doctor called Golding
Bird – after seeing the quacks make money hand over fist – set up ‘electric
baths’ facilities at Guy’s Hospital in London, where he charged his posh
patients a hefty fee to alleviate vague maladies.
But not everyone abandoned the project of building a legitimate discipline
around investigating animal electricity. Behind the scenes, another scientist had
been working to keep its study on life support: Alexander von Humboldt had
reviewed Galvani’s work for the French commission throughout the 1790s,
and had grown to strongly suspect that Volta and Galvani’s theories did not
contradict each other after all, and that Volta had in fact been wrong to
dismiss animal electricity out of hand.34
Humboldt would go on to become chamberlain to the Prussian king and a
leader of the Enlightenment, shaping how we conceive of nature itself as a
single interconnected system. But during the electricity wars he was still in his
early twenties, having recently graduated from university into a position as a
mining inspector. Quickly his polymathic tendencies jumped from geology to
botany to comparative anatomy. When he became aware of the Volta–Galvani
controversy, he set his mind to solving the mystery.
To that end, Humboldt conducted about 4,000 experiments over five
years, several on himself. (His friend Johann Wilhelm Ritter, whom he often
convinced to join him, scrambled his nervous system so much with this type
of self-experimentation that he died at thirty-four.) Of these investigations,
arguably the most shocking was Humboldt’s decision to insert a silver wire
connected to a galvanic pile into his own rectum, an experiment the historian
Stanley Finger calls ‘almost unimaginable’.35 This elicited all the unpleasant
results Aldini had obtained with large animals, but performing the experiment
on himself gave Humboldt the added benefit of first-hand experience. Thus he
gained the insight that the involuntary faecal expulsions were accompanied by
painful abdominal cramps and ‘visual sensations’. Not content to stop there,
he forced the wire further into his anus and found that ‘a bright light appears
before both eyes’. It would be hard to go further to prove one’s dedication to
understanding animal electricity.
In 1800, he undertook a journey to Venezuela for the purposes of
investigating John Walsh’s experiments with live eels, which didn’t tend to
survive the journey out of their native habitats. Using pack animals as bait to
draw out the eels (some of which were five feet long and discharged 700-volt
shocks – enough to stun the horses and mules) he saw for himself the
unambiguous power of animal electricity. After the trip, he began to draw
connections between this kind of powerful defensive biological electricity and
the more quotidian variety that underpinned everyday motion and perception.
In his subsequent writings on electric eels, he concluded, in carefully written
prose, that at some future point ‘it will perhaps be found that, in most animals,
every contraction of the muscular fibre is preceded by a discharge from the
nerve into the muscle; and that the mere simple contact of heterogeneous
substances is a source of life in all organized beings’.36
Instead of adopting Aldini’s strategy of charging ahead with his belief that
Galvani had been right, Humboldt played the long game to bring experimental
physiology back: he encouraged promising young scientists to study animal
electricity. When Humboldt returned to Berlin from his travels in the late
1820s, he became a patron of the up-and-coming physiologist Johannes Müller
and helped install him to lead the anatomy department of the world-beating
university his brother Wilhelm von Humboldt had founded two decades
earlier.37
The electroquacks had so thoroughly discredited the official record of
animal electricity that when the first real evidence of its existence finally
presented itself, even the scientist who rediscovered it didn’t understand quite
what he had found. In 1828, Leopoldo Nobili, a physicist in Florence, was
working on ways to improve the sensitivity of electrometers, which were
becoming increasingly important as they were crucial to the running of
transatlantic telegraph cables. Electricians used them to confirm that current
was flowing and that messages were being delivered. The early versions
suffered from limited accuracy because the earth’s magnetism interfered with
the measurement of the wake of the current. No one could figure out how to
get rid of it.
To do that, you needed a much more sensitive electrometer. (By now, these
were starting to be known as galvanometers, thanks to a sly nod from the
French physicist André-Marie Ampère.) To test that his improved version
really was better, Nobili needed to find the weakest possible current. He
remembered Volta’s assertion that Galvani had witnessed not some special
‘animal electricity’, but nothing more than the vanishingly faint currents
generated from contact between two dissimilar materials. He realised that if his
device could measure something as infinitesimal as the current travelling
through a dead frog, its superiority would be incontestable. Sure enough, his
new meter detected this flow, which he immediately christened ‘corrente di
rana’ (frog current).38 It allowed him to make the first-ever recording of
electrical activity from the neuromuscular preparation. But Nobili did not
actually believe it was intrinsic to the frog – he was still firmly in Volta’s camp.
It was all, he insisted, to do with metals.
It would take another ten years for another scientist to correctly interpret
the significance of what Nobili had measured, and finally put bioelectricity
back on its pedestal.
Carlo Matteucci chopped the last of the frog thighs off its erstwhile owner and
fitted it carefully onto the pile. He had killed ten frogs, removed their thighs,
and carved each into a shape roughly resembling a halved orange – intact on
one side, bisected on the other. He had then stacked these amphibian pieces
atop each other into a biological inversion – some might say perversion – of a
voltaic pile where zinc and copper were replaced with muscle and nerve.
Matteucci had just finished constructing the world’s first battery made
exclusively of frogs.39
When he tested the current, he saw the output: the more thighs he
connected, the more the galvanometer needle was deflected, indicating an
increase in the flow of current. But that wasn’t the end of the experiment.
When he was satisfied that his pile contained enough biological material, he
picked up the wire attached to his biological battery, and delicately touched it
to a separate frog lying limp on a plate nearby – or rather, what was left of it.
Unlike the frog battery, this one had been prepared in the style popularised by
Galvani many years ago: flayed, head and midsection almost entirely missing
save for the two crural nerves that still connected its spine to its legs, which
remained whole. When the wire made contact, the hideous little half-puppet
immediately jerked into the familiar dance. Animal electricity – and animal
electricity alone – had caused a dead frog’s legs to move.
Here, forty years after Galvani’s death, was the first real progress in
electrophysiology since the days of Galvani himself.
Matteucci was another of the promising young scientists who had been
mentored and funded by Humboldt during the decades animal electricity fell
out of favour. Humboldt had been inspired by Matteucci’s enthusiasm for the
promise of an underlying electrical force in the nerves, and had recommended
the young scientist for the professorship he now held at the University of Pisa.
He also defended Matteucci against attempts to discredit his discovery of the
nerve centres torpedo fish used to control their shocks. So when Matteucci
told Humboldt of his frog battery, his patron was so thrilled he immediately
disseminated the paper to his entire social network, including Müller at the
University of Berlin, who pressed Matteucci’s paper into the hands of his eager
young student, Emil du Bois-Reymond.40 Humboldt was – again – a mentor
to this young physiologist. ‘He is studying a matter, the deep natural secret of
muscle movement,’ Humboldt wrote to the German cultural minister in 1849
to secure funding for du Bois-Reymond’s research, ‘with which I, too, was
preoccupied in the earlier half of my life’. When he sparked du Bois-
Reymond’s interest with Matteucci’s experiment, the match caught.
Though he found Matteucci’s grotesque experiment unscientific (‘no one
can feel more deeply than myself how much this examination leaves to be
desired in focus and clarity’), the work du Bois-Reymond did to build on it
over the next two decades would finally resuscitate the long-dead field of
bioelectricity and bring it back under the umbrella of legitimate scientific
inquiry. He was incredibly ambitious and passionate about making his name,
and du Bois-Reymond’s fifty-five-year tenure at the University of Berlin turned
into an attempt to secure his place in history by usurping Galvani as the father
of animal electricity.
He was Galvani’s heir in many ways. His commitment to good science and
rigour was legendary. To characterise and measure the currents in nerves more
precisely, he went to lengths that might be considered obsessive. He spent
years on trial-and-error assembly of his own design for a special galvanometer
sensitive enough to measure the faint electrical current flowing not through
telegraph lines, but through frog muscles and nerves. He acquired so many
frogs his Berlin flat turned into a ‘frog kennel’.41 When severing his frogs’
muscle and nerve fibres, to avoid introducing any accidental source of external
electricity, he would bite them in half instead of using a metal implement. He
nearly blinded himself with his constant exposure to the irritants in frog skin.
Berlin, like Italy decades before, began to run out of frogs. But this tenacity –
inflamed by his determination to refine and take credit for Galvani’s
experiments – paid off.
Using his new galvanometer, du Bois-Reymond was able to see for himself
the distinct electrical disturbance on his meter that accompanied muscle
contractions. The needle on his galvanometer swung whenever the current
passed through the area he measured. Whereas Galvani had only indirectly
detected the electrical impulse travelling through the muscle by the evidence of
a frog’s leg twitch (making the frog the world’s first galvanometer, in a twisted
way), here du Bois-Reymond was directly seeing the animal electricity as it
excited the muscle. The eighty-year-old Humboldt gamely lent himself as a
guinea pig for these studies: even though he was now enough of a big deal to
be taking ‘his daily dinner at the King’s side’, he would roll up his sleeve and
flex until he deflected the needle of du Bois-Reymond’s galvanometer.42
While most researchers greeted the early experiments coldly – the zeitgeist
being still firmly against the idea of thoughts and intentions producing
measurable electricity43 – by the end of the eighteenth century du Bois-
Reymond and his colleagues had successfully established bioelectricity as an
aspect of neurobiology. The notion of electricity running through nerves and
muscles was approaching respectability. A couple of outstanding questions
remained – how did it travel? And why was this electricity so much slower than
the kind that ran down telegraph wires?
But now you could measure it. Du Bois-Reymond and his colleague
Hermann von Helmholtz called this electrical jolt that the nerve sent to
activate the muscle the ‘action current’. Other scientists soon joined the hunt
to characterise it precisely, and while acrimonious fights broke out over many
of the details, the existence of electrical phenomena in the nervous system
became largely accepted. Du Bois-Reymond proved that electricity was
relevant in the human body. Nerves ran on the stuff. He had made von
Humboldt proud and usurped Galvani.44 ‘I have succeeded in restoring to life
in full reality that hundred-year-old dream of the physicist and physiologist, the
identity of the nerve substance with electricity,’ he wrote.45
At the same time as du Bois-Reymond’s research had resurrected the
legitimacy of biological electricity, there had been new advances in mapping
the brain and the nervous system. As had happened in the past, new tools cast
doubt on old science, and a fresh uncertainty loomed. How could a single
electrical impulse be responsible for such an enormous variety of discrete
sensation and motion? Science at that point conceived of the nervous system
as a vast uninterrupted network of fused strings. The best available metaphor
was plumbing: Rather than being comprised of a bunch of separate cells,
scientists still saw a series of tubes. Except instead of animal spirits, it was now
electricity that coursed through them.
Thanks to better tools – like sensitive galvanometers and Volta’s battery –
and Humboldt, du Bois-Reymond, and Helmholtz’s commitment to the
rigours of the scientific method, the millennia-old mystery of animal spirits
had finally been solved. Animal spirits, the things that carried the brain’s
impulse and intent to the limbs to carry out, and carried back the sensations of
the world outside, were electric. Animal spirits were animal electricity. But
instead of calling it animal electricity, the new term was ‘nervous conduction’.
It meant the same thing; it was just science instead of philosophy. Galvani was
vindicated.
PART 2
D uring the centuries of argument over the existence and nature of the
nervous impulse, sceptics had many reasons to doubt that actual
electricity runs in the animal nervous system. Investigations into the intriguing
powers of electric fish and eels had yielded an obvious source: a giant electrical
organ specialised to store up electrical charge and then dispense it in one big
paralysing zap. No anatomist had yet succeeded at locating anything like that
in a human body. And without a power source, how were we supposed to be
sending electrical current down our nerves? This led to lingering suspicions
that electricity was just an unsatisfying metaphor for the otherwise mysterious
conduction mechanism of nervous signals.
All that changed in the latter years of the twentieth century – when we
found the source. The new technique that aided this discovery caused a step
change in the disciplines of electrophysiology and neuroscience. The
consequent advances were so swift, and so numerous, that science historians
Marco Bresadola and Marco Piccolino call them ‘comparable to that of
quantum mechanics in Max Planck’s day’.1
CHAPTER 3
B y the end of the nineteenth century, animal spirits had been rescued
from millennia of airy philosophical conjecture and placed onto the
firm ground of the scientific method. Alexander von Humboldt, Emil du
Bois-Reymond, and Hermann von Helmholtz had vindicated the work for
which Galvani had given his life: what are the animal spirits in our nerves,
these things that animate our every sense and motion? They are electric.
Yet even they could not have anticipated what their foundational tools and
insights would set in motion over the next 150 years. Today, our understanding
of bioelectricity is in the process of another metamorphosis as we begin to
grasp the outlines of the electrome.1
The electrome transcends the bioelectric signals whose outlines Galvani
and du Bois-Reymond glimpsed. Those were the drivers of the nervous
system’s ability to help us sense and move in the world, and today are well-
characterised, thanks to copious investigations that helped establish the
modern discipline of neuroscience. But in the past twenty years or so, a new
picture has been emerging, one that shows with increasing clarity how
instrumental bioelectric signals are beyond the nervous system, and how much
they do in the rest of the body. Just as the genome describes all of the genetic
material in an organism – the DNA that writes the instruction set to build it,
the As, Cs, Ts, and Gs that make up the code this instruction set is written in,
and other elements that control the activity of the genes – a complete
accounting of our electrome would catalogue all the profound ways different
electrical signals shape biology.
Mapping the electrome would furnish a unique blueprint of the electrical
properties that determine almost every aspect of our life and death. It would
include a profile of the dimensions and properties of our electrics from the
level of organs to cells to the tiny constituents of those cells, including
mitochondria, and to the behaviours of the electrical molecules themselves.
As you saw in the first part of the book, the earliest glimpse of the
electrome came to us courtesy of the electrical activity of nerves and muscles.
‘Animal spirits’ became nervous conduction, and the scientific discipline that
coalesced around its study was neurology. The insights from neurology (and
electrophysiology, the field that united eighteenth-century electricians with
theoretical physiologists) were codified in the 1960s into the formal discipline
that is today known as neuroscience: the study of the animal nervous system.
The twentieth century brought tremendous advances in characterising the
patterns hidden in the electrical activity of the nervous system. We began to
crack the code that explains how it transmits information to and from the
brain. As you’ll see in the next few chapters, almost all of these insights were
delivered by probing the nervous system with metallic electricity. This led us to
discover that artificial electricity could even, to varying degrees of success,
modify our own bioelectricity – and health, thoughts, and behaviour. That was
startling enough, but then, towards the end of the century, we found out how
much deeper the rabbit hole goes.
But before we go any further, we’ll need to establish some of the basics of
neuroscience, so that we can be on the same page about how the nervous
system works and why people were so hot to prod it with artificial electricity.
That’s what this chapter is for. Please join me for a speedrun through 150
years of electrophysiology.
Figuring out how electrical messages are sent through the body got a lot easier
once we figured out the structure of the brain and spinal cord and the special
cells that enable them to pass communications. These are called nerve cells, or
neurons. All this was established in a series of groundbreaking insights known
as the Neuron Doctrine, which won Camillo Golgi and Santiago ramón y Cajal
a Nobel Prize in 1906. It was the first time we understood how the nervous
system worked. (Before this, as the conversation around animal spirits makes
clear, we thought the nervous system was just a single connected network of
tubes that went from the brain through the whole body, which is why it made
sense that you could fill them with water or hydraulic fluid – and why not
much else about it made any sense.)
What Ramón y Cajal and Golgi figured out (though again via plenty of
backbiting and disagreement) is that the nervous system was composed of
cells – separate special cells that had been coined ‘neurons’ – that could pass
electrical signals from the brain to the nerves and muscles, and back.
No one had ever realised that the nervous system is made of cells, and
that’s because neurons don’t look like your standard cell. Most cells look like a
sphere that got a bit squashed. Not neurons. A neuron has three distinct parts.
It has a cell body (this bit does look like a normal cell), but sprouting from
that cell body in all directions are branching protuberances of different lengths.
These come in two flavours: the first – called dendrites – are very short, and
they bring incoming messages to the cell body. The second – axons – can be
up to a metre long, and their job is to send messages from the cell body to
other neurons or muscles.
While some of the brain’s 86 billion neurons exist only in the brain, a vast
number of them extend down your spine and into your skin, heart, muscles,
eyes, ears, nose, mouth, organs, intestine – in other words, into absolutely
everything in your body – to make it move and feel and much more besides.
The ‘feeling’ neurons that bring sensation and perception into the brain
are part of the ‘afferent system’, which brings you news of the outside world:
the sights, sounds, smells, scratches, and bumps it confers on your body. These
neurons are also called sensory neurons. The ‘moving’ neurons, which bring
your intention down into your body to actuate it, are part of the ‘efferent
system’, which lets you respond to the sensations carried by the afferent
system.
Whether you’re feeling or moving, the signals that are in charge of
transmitting information to and from the brain are sent by way of an electrical
mechanism: the action potential. This was the little needle-moving blip du
Bois-Reymond knew as the action current or nervous impulse. Nerve impulse,
action potential, you might have heard it called a spike – it’s all the same thing:
the small electric signal that relays a message between two neighbouring nerve
cells in the brain, or from nerve to muscle. When a dendrite receives a
message, it passes the signal on to its cell body, which then evaluates whether
to pass that signal on to the axon. If it passes the message on, it zips to the
end of the axon, where it jumps to the next cell’s dendrite. People had started
fighting about whether the nerve signal was electrical or chemical almost from
the moment du Bois-Reymond and Helmholtz started measuring it. But the
fight graduated to near war with the discovery of how the signal hops from
one cell to the next.
This is because that message hits a little speed bump at the end of the
axon. There, it encounters a tiny gap that separates the axon of one cell from
the dendrite of another. This gap is called a synapse, christened the same year
the Neuron Doctrine won its progenitors the Nobel. The discovery of a gap
between cells that were meant to be transmitting an electrical signal revived a
lot of doubts about the still-fragile idea that animal electricity is real and that
the nerve impulse is electric. After all, an electric signal can’t travel over an air
gap in telegraph wires, so why should it be able to do so in the wires of the
nervous system?
In 1921, the discovery of chemicals called neurotransmitters, which float
across the synapse’s gap, only deepened those doubts. That briefly led to a
fight over the nature of the nervous signal between opposing groups of
scientists who called themselves the Soups (team chemistry) and those calling
themselves the Sparks (team electricity).2 It was like science’s own West Side
Story.
In the end – after fierce fights against the Soups’ insistence that no
electrical aspect existed – the Sparks won the day. Their champions were Alan
Hodgkin and Andrew Huxley, two Cambridge University physiologists whose
names may engender a very faint rustling in the back of your brain, in the part
that had to memorise something about their endeavours in school. The reason
their work is canon in the history of science is because they established that
electricity is a crucial arbiter of the nerve impulse. They finally ended all the
nattering between the Soups and the Sparks in the 1950s. Their experiments
showed for the first time, in indisputable detail, exactly how the action
potential is carried down a neuron by electrically charged particles, without
whose electrical properties and activity nothing would happen at all.
Those particles are called ions. Ions are atoms with a positive or negative
charge. The fluid that bathes every one of your cells – there’s a lot of it, and it’s
the reason you always hear that you are 60 per cent water – is teeming with
them. The ions that are dissolved in this so-called ‘extracellular fluid’ bear a
strong resemblance to the constituents of seawater: mostly sodium and
potassium, with smaller amounts of other stuff including calcium, magnesium,
and chloride. Their precise concentrations inside and outside each neuron are
the primary determinants of whether an electrical signal is permitted to pass.
They got their name from Michael Faraday, in honour of their weird
tendency to move as if of their own accord. It was thanks to Volta’s pile that
he discovered this tendency, in fact. After Volta gave him one of his early
prototype batteries in 1814,3 Faraday used it to devise the principles of the
electric motor and induction, and unify the laws of electricity. But much more
important for our purposes here, it was instrumental to his discovery of the
existence of ions. Faraday would experiment with putting various compounds
into water and passing an electrical current through the water to see what it
would do to them. Compounds are materials composed of a mix of two or
more separate elements: under an electric current, the compound would
dissolve back into the two separate elements that had made it, a bit like a cake
neatly disgorging its sugar and flour. In this metaphor the ‘sugar’ bits, having
segregated themselves out of the mix, would migrate over to the electrode that
was passing the current through the water; meanwhile, the ‘flour’ particles
would congregate at the other electrode.4 Faraday didn’t know what to make
of this back then. What was it that was travelling through the water and
accumulating on his electrodes? In 1834, he christened the mysterious particles
‘ions’, which is about as far as anyone got with them for the next half century.
Then, in the 1880s, the Swedish scientist Svante Arrhenius realised that
ions’ movement was a result of them being pulled by electrical forces – which
made sense, as ions were just atoms that, rather than being neutral, were either
positively or negatively charged. This explained how they wandered through a
solution as if of their own accord. They weren’t doing that at all; rather, the
positive ions had been attracted to the negative pole of the battery, and the
negative ones wanted to get to the positive side. Finally, a clear explanation for
Faraday’s observations.
These properties apply in all solutions – including the biological soup that
bathes the inside and outside of all the cells in all biological tissues. Ions are
the ingredients that keep us alive. If you’ve ever been on an IV drip, you have
ions to thank, and the nineteenth-century physiologist Sydney Ringer, who
figured out the precise recipe of sodium, potassium, and other electrolytes to
flood your vasculature with this facsimile of extracellular fluid. It enabled him
to keep organs from failing even after they had been removed from the body
that previously sustained them. His first experiment was on a frog heart,
which, when placed into his new ‘physiological saline’, was able to keep
beating for several hours normally, sans frog.5 The broth was originally called
Ringer’s solution, and has been monumentally consequential for biology.
But why were ions so important? What was so special about them that we
couldn’t live without them? As the twentieth century dawned, the consensus
slowly emerged that ions could be the main agents in the electrical
transmission of the nervous impulse.
As the Neuron Doctrine was coming into view, here’s what we knew. One:
biochemists had established that positively charged atoms like sodium, and
negatively charged chloride, carried their electrical charge with them wherever
they went. Two: it was also understood – thanks to people like ringer – that
ions populate the spaces inside and outside our cells. And finally, three: we
knew action potentials created electrical activity strong enough to make
galvanometer needles swing as the nervous signal flew past. Together, this was
circumstantial proof that electric charges were moving in the nerve or muscle.
But just as we had had a collection of unconnected facts about animal
electricity in the eighteenth century with no framework to unite them, we
didn’t yet have a way to make sense of all these separate facts about the
nervous system and ions. Not until the 1940s anyway, when a series of
experiments showed exactly how ions were the main agents in the electrical
transmission of the nervous impulse.
Alan Hodgkin and Andrew Huxley knew if they could prove that ion
concentrations changed differently inside and outside a nerve cell during the
course of an action potential, it would prove once and for all that electricity
was involved in the very heart of the generation of the bioelectric signal – as a
causative agent, not just as an echo of some chemical process.6 Again, frogs
met their end – but their nerves were definitely too small for any existing tool
to be able to analyse the ion content inside their membranes. Next, Hodgkin
and Huxley tried crabs. Still too small. Finally, they found an animal with a
nerve big enough to stick an electrode into: the squid.
This creature has a pair of uncommonly large axons – on the order of
millimetres, a thousand times the diameter of anything in a human, earning
them the nickname ‘giant axon’ – as they must send the brain’s ‘run away!’
instructions instantly down a squid’s massive body.7 This left plenty of room
for Hodgkin and Huxley to insert the recording equipment necessary to
monitor the electrical properties of the cell. They wanted to know how these
would change as the nerve fired, and how the ion concentrations would
change, inside and outside the cell, in response. They found a way to stick one
electrode inside, and another outside, and in so doing, for the first time
measured the electrical difference between the inside and the outside of a cell.
That difference was pretty big: the outside of the cell was 70 millivolts higher
than inside when the nerve wasn’t actively firing, just resting.
This number is called the cell’s membrane potential. It measures the
difference between the charged particles inside the membrane and outside it.
Remember how ions are positively or negatively charged atoms? That means
they bring their charge with them wherever they go. A sodium is carrying a
+1, for example. So is potassium. Chloride drags around its -1, and I can’t help
thinking of it as being perpetually low-key ashamed. Fancy calcium poses
conspicuously with its +2. Outside the neurons, a mix of these ions (and their
various charges) congregates in the free space of the extracellular fluid.
Because there’s only so much room in any given neuron, the comparatively
lower population of ions inside them creates a situation where the sum of
charges is lower inside the cell than out. That’s why it’s 70 millivolts lower in
any neuron than in the spaces outside it, and that 70 millivolts is exactly how
the neuron likes it. For that reason, it’s called the ‘resting potential’. It’s the
neuron putting its feet up, conserving its energy.
But when an action potential raced through, Hodgkin and Huxley found
those numbers changed wildly. The charge difference between the inside and
outside of the cell quickly zeroed out, getting less and less pronounced until
there was no difference between the inside and the outside of the cell. (Then,
it kept going just a little bit past zero, until the inside of the cell was
momentarily more positively charged than the soup outside.) But when all the
fuss was done, it always returned to its 70-millivolt happy place.
While all this electrical commotion was underway, Hodgkin and Huxley
also noticed that the different ions did very different things. During the resting
potential, lots of potassium ions were inside the cell. But when the action
potential happened, suddenly it was all sodium in the cell, which belched out a
big wave of potassium. The cell’s return to happiness was accompanied by a
return of all the potassium ions. This phenomenon cascaded down the nerve,
carrying the nerve impulse like a wave. That’s how Hodgkin and Huxley finally
proved that action potentials are unquestionably generated by changes in the
concentration of ions.8 The sodium and potassium were somehow responsible
for the signal travelling down the axon – the electrical charge passed on by the
precisely choreographed comings and goings of these ions.
So there it was: the solution to the mystery of Ringer’s solution. The
reason this precise mix of ions is crucial to keeping a body alive is that they are
what makes the nerve impulse travel down the nerve. Without ions, the
nervous signal couldn’t jump. Then we couldn’t breathe in or out or swallow
and our heart wouldn’t beat.
In 1952, Hodgkin and Huxley published the results of years of work that
showed how sodium and potassium ions swap places in a cell, carrying their
electrical charges in and out, to create the action potential. Revealing the
mechanism of the action potential for the first time netted them the Nobel,
but for Hodgkin the real triumph was the concrete evidence that electricity
was not just a side effect but the cause. As he put it in his Nobel lecture in
1963, ‘the action potential is not just an electrical sign of the impulse, but is
the causal agent in propagation’.9
Their discovery was momentous, and should have set off a coordinated
new quest to understand the information carried by these ions (and for a short
time it did – one report quoted how the oceans around the main research
centres briefly ran out of squid). But the spike in interest was short-lived. Just
when animal electricity should have taken the spotlight once more, a cloud
passed over the sun. No sooner had Hodgkin and Huxley revealed the elusive
mechanism of the nerve impulse, than two other young researchers stole the
show with a discovery deemed far more monumental: the double helix. In
1953, James Watson and Francis Crick – and Rosalind Franklin – unveiled
their discovery of DNA. ‘There are only molecules. Everything else is
sociology,’ Watson pronounced,10 and the importance of bioelectricity was
sidelined by a ‘bigger’ discovery once again, just as it had been after Galvani.
Hodgkin and Huxley had shown that an action potential depends crucially
on a cell holding tight to potassium and expelling sodium. But apart from the
glamour of DNA, the bigger reason the avenue of research they had opened
didn’t continue into humans was because we didn’t have small enough
equipment to peer into the ever tinier nooks and crannies we’d have to explore
to see ions going in and out of a cell and find out how it was happening. As a
result, big questions went unanswered.
A longstanding theory going back to du Bois-Reymond’s time had it that
the cell’s membrane wall would just disappear every so often, becoming
transparent to a bunch of ions, like a curtain being pulled aside.11 But that had
never made much sense – and it made even less sense now in the wake of
Hodgkin and Huxley’s findings. Seeing sodium and potassium swap places like
that made Hodgkin realise the membrane was not just being drawn back like a
curtain. It was actively choosing what to let in and out. But what was the
mechanism? Did neurons have special holes for particular ions?
How did nerve cells know how to only get rid of sodium, while potassium
was untouched? This was especially weird given that potassium is about 16 per
cent smaller than sodium, lending extra mystery to the question of how a cell
could momentarily expel all potassium while allowing in sodium.
During their long years of experimentation, Hodgkin and Huxley
formulated a theory that the ions were entering and exiting through tiny holes
that perforated the membrane like a sieve – maybe some of these holes liked
sodium, and others potassium? People were starting to develop theories and
language about these dynamics – but they didn’t get a name until they were
christened ‘ion channels’.
I am ion man
What exactly is an ion channel, anyway? Since the 1960s, there had been
growing suspicion that these pores were actually proteins that tunnel through
the cell membrane. But no one managed to get any further until the early
1970s, when Erwin Neher, a physicist, and Bert Sakmann, a physiologist, got
their hands on the problem at the Max Planck Institute for Biophysical
Chemistry in Göttingen, West Germany. They reasoned that if those little
holes actually existed, it should be possible to detect the teeny-tiny currents
stirred up as the ions shuttled in and out. But since that’s about a hundred
thousand millionths of the current that powers your toaster, detecting it would
require more sensitive equipment than anything that could be built.
So Neher and Sakmann created a new device that could isolate a little
patch of neuron containing just a few, or maybe even a single one, of these
putative holes. The ions and the holes were still too small to see with the
equipment of the time, but when they were able to record the telltale current
coming out of a single ion pore in a living cell membrane, Neher and Sakmann
proved the holes were actually there. They existed.
What’s more, they figured out how they worked. The shape of these
current pulses made it clear that these little pores could only ever be in one of
two states: all the way open or all the way closed. They were never half ajar.12
And when they were open, boy, were they open. A single open pore let
potassium and sodium ions flood in and out of a cell at a rate ranging from
10,000 to 100,000 ions per millisecond. That is a lot of +1s.
A few years later, in 1978, William Agnew and his team at the California
Institute of Technology (Caltech) finally identified what a sodium channel
actually is: it’s not just a hole in a sieve, it’s a protein.13 And with that insight,
molecular biology went from show-stealing enemy to bioelectricity’s best
friend. That’s because Watson and Crick’s DNA discovery had given scientists
the ability to read any protein’s genetic code – which meant that if you could
isolate and sequence it, you could clone it. And that meant you could start
messing around with ion channels in earnest. You could make cells that only
had shut versions, or open versions, and see what the effect was on an
organism.
In 1986, Masaharu Noda was the first to clone a voltage-gated sodium
channel (this is a kind of sodium channel that opens if it detects a change in
the voltage pressing on the membrane around it).14 Scientists started
synthesising proteins in different shapes and cloning different cells with
different kinds and numbers of ion channels.15 You could create cells with
particular channels edited out altogether. If you were really enterprising, you
might make cells with ‘designer’ Frankenchannels that had been edited
together – and see what happened next. This research soon gave scientists a
full index of all the ion channels – sodium channels, calcium channels, chloride
channels, potassium channels. Never mind the transparent curtain – it was
these proteins that decided which ion was allowed to go where, when.
How did they make these complicated decisions? That puzzle was solved
in 1991 – the same year Neher and Sakmann got their Nobel for kick-starting
this avalanche of research – by the biophysicist Roderick MacKinnon.
Many complex metaphors have been used to describe the incredibly
complicated system MacKinnon uncovered. But I like to think of ion channels
as shape sorters – you know, the toy you give a baby so it can shove different-
shaped pegs into a wooden box through matching holes. Some of the pegs are
round, others are triangular, or squares, or stars. The square holes
accommodate the square pegs, and so on. So, while some holes may be
technically bigger than their non-matching counterparts, they still won’t fit
through. They are incompatible with the dimensions of the channel, and
therefore it remains impenetrable. (It’s actually a little more complicated still,
because the holes in this baby toy shape-shift to accommodate the peg they
like best.)
After MacKinnon completed the picture of the cell membrane, we
understood for the first time the array of interlocking mechanisms that
underpin biological electricity: how the proteins in the membrane work with
ions to generate the action potential, and how everything goes back to square
one again when the action potential has passed. Once we understood ion
channels, we were able to understand the action potential in full.
It is remarkably similar to how you manage an exclusive night club.
In the club
I should mention that the following analogy ignores the entire universe of
complexities inside and outside a cell, focusing only on the place where the
voltage is generated. But after all, this is a book about bioelectricity.
You can therefore think of the cell as a highly micromanaged nightclub.
Ions take the role of the patrons, and the ion channels act as bouncers staffing
VIP access doors. These participants orchestrate the three stages of the action
potential. (My ridiculous ion nightclub piggybacks on a wonderful explanation
in Frances Ashcroft’s The Spark of Life.)
At rest, when no action potentials are passing through, the nerve cell is in
what’s known as its ‘resting potential’. This was the 70-millivolt difference
Hodgkin and Huxley found. In this state, the inside of the cell is more
negatively charged than the soup in the extracellular space.
Inside our club, the crowd is mostly made up of positively charged
potassium ions, which cram into the tiny space at a concentration fifty times
greater than they mill around outside. Outside the club, a long line of hopefuls
– mainly sodium ions, also carrying a positive charge – press against the doors.
But alas, most of those doors are shut to them. Management has a clear
preference for members of the potassium group – a strict NO SODIUM
ALLOWED policy is in effect. Those sodium ions though, they’re just like us,
so they cram in greater and greater numbers against the doors, wanting to get
in. But management is not messing around. If a bouncer, known as an ion
pump, finds a sodium ion has somehow sneaked in, it is briskly escorted out –
and in a final insult, any three available potassium ions are waved through
security in its place.
As for the potassium ions – well, they’re just like us too. They get tired of
the heaving conditions inside the club and occasionally depart, leaving a
negative charge behind them. There are no barriers to them leaving.
This delicate balancing act of managerial vigilance, the sodium ions’
desperation, the potassium ions’ general standoffishness, and other variables, is
what keeps the cell membrane’s resting potential perpetually hovering at -70
millivolts. (More positively charged sodium ions outside than positively
charged potassium ions inside make the inside negative compared to the
outside.)
Little wonder the cell biologist Robert Campenot describes the state of a
nerve cell before an action potential as being on a hair trigger. All it needs is
an excuse. Any minute change in the equilibrium will pitch it all into chaos.
Before we get to that, though, there’s just one more thing. Those doors
with the bouncers I’ve described so far are not the only doors. There are also
emergency doors: the voltage-gated sodium channels. They bang open if they
sense a change to the carefully maintained resting potential. If the amount of
charge outside the club changes the right amount, then in an instant, all the
potential energy that has thus far been held in delicate suspension is released.16
Which is to say, if the crowd of sodium outside the club gets too rowdy, the
velvet ropes topple, and they start to force themselves into the club. And then
it’s panic at the disco.
Relative to the size of the cell, the membrane potential is massive. The cell
membrane is about 10 nanometres across, and one side of it is 70 millivolts
more negative than the other. If you had the equivalent voltage difference
across your body, it would feel like 10 million volts. That is . . . a lot. An
intense static shock that will make you turn the air blue with swear words
packs about 10,000 volts.
The shock of an action potential is way more intense for our little ion
channel bouncer friends. The emergency doors are flung open. The sodium
ions take advantage of the confusion to rush into the club, triggering a
feedback loop: the bigger the change in membrane potential, the more sodium
channels open and the more sodium ions crowd in – and the more sodium
ions come in, the more the voltage goes positive, the more sodium channels
open, and so on. In an instant, sodium has taken the place by storm.
Step 3 – repolarisation
Now that millions of sodium ions have mobbed this formerly exclusive club,
jostling and crowding the horrified potassium ions, the inside of the cell can
briefly reach 100 millivolts more positive than the outside. Less than a
millisecond later, the potassium channels open and the potassium ions,
disgusted, leave the club en masse.
Inside the club, the mass exodus of potassium ions has returned the cell
membrane to its resting state. But now the customers are all wrong!
Management is desperate to win back the fleeing potassium ions. They lock
the place down again. The bouncers start cracking their knuckles. Most of the
sodium ions leave of their own accord.17 It takes a while to convince the
potassium ions to re-enter the despoiled premises. Eventually, though,
management coaxes them back. Then it’s just a matter of time before the
whole thing happens all over again.
The voltage opens and closes the channels.18 Sodium and potassium
channels that react to the voltage changes mediate the generation of action
potentials, which allow signals to be propagated from one end of a neuron to
another. The same mechanism was ultimately even what controlled the
chemical neurotransmitters. At the very end of the axon, where the action
potential terminates, there’s another set of bouncers: voltage-gated calcium
channels. When the action potential hits, these open, and calcium from the
extracellular saltwater floods into the end of the axon. That releases
neurotransmitters (your familiar serotonins, dopamines, oxytocins) to drift
across the axon terminal and onto the nearby entry point of the dendrite of
the neuron next door. And that sets off the next action potential, and the
whole sequence starts all over again. All these things – chemical and electrical
aspects – are ultimately controlled by the membrane’s voltage, its electrical
status.
And that’s the story of the nerve impulse – the thing that’s responsible for
our every sensation, motion, emotion, and heartbeat. Its electricity is the
central generator. The source of your electricity, and mine, is not a separate
electric organ like the one in an eel, but a self-regenerating mechanism
produced inside the cells themselves, by the exquisitely coordinated dance of
ions through proteins.
The basic mechanism that was in charge of all of this complexity was
startlingly simple. Stack more charged ions on one side of a membrane than
another, and you got an electrical potential. Change the voltage, and you
released all that energy. That’s essentially how a battery works: one side has a
different amount of charge than the other. Nerve and muscle cells, it became
clear, were tiny rechargeable batteries.
Forty trillion batteries
But they were not the only cells that acted like batteries. After it became
possible to properly study ion channels using the tools of molecular biology, it
became clear that ion channels (and the ions they admitted and didn’t) were
also present in every other cell in the body. This was the wake-up call: what
were they doing there? What use did all these other cells have for electrical
properties?
We found out in due course. In 1984, the ion channel physiologist Frances
Ashcroft found that the pancreas, for example, uses a particular potassium ion
channel to issue the electrical commands that precisely synchronise its insulin-
secreting beta cells. (These travel ten times faster than the chemical kind, so it’s
the only way to synchronise that many cells to act in unison.) This potassium
channel needs to be in perfect working order to coordinate the insulin release.
In the early 2000s, Ashcroft and Andrew Hattersley found the mutation that
locked it open – and thereby caused a variant of diabetes.
Insights such as these multiplied and soon transformed medicine. Ion
channel physics emerged as a major biomedical discipline in its own right.
Now scientists had the tools and understanding to investigate how ion
channels in muscle and nerve cells sustained the most basic workings of the
human body. And more importantly, what happened when they didn’t. Most
importantly, they finally had a new tool in the arsenal for manipulating that
electricity more precisely, and it would prove the most consequential tool for
bioelectricity researchers since the invention of the battery.
The first ideas for drugs that could manipulate the electrics came from
neurotoxins. In the 1960s, research on neurotoxins had clarified that many of
these natural poisons affect sodium and potassium balances, throwing into
chaos the delicate mechanisms that make cellular communication possible –
acting like a reverse Ringer’s solution.19 The reason you’re not supposed to eat
puffer fish (unless it has been prepared by someone with an advanced degree
in precision-filleting a puffer fish) is that some of its parts carry a defensive
poison called tetrodotoxin. Get even the tiniest amount of that inside you, and
it can quickly paralyse the muscles that drive everything in your body, including
your lungs, and then you asphyxiate. The exact mechanism of how this works
was unlocked by the improved understanding of ion channels brought by
Neher and Sakmann: tetrodotoxin keeps sodium ions from entering the cell.20
It wedges itself into those channels, blocking the doors, and if there’s no
sodium influx, then there’s no outward potassium stampede, and that prevents
all the rest of the dominoes that cascade into an action potential. Other kinds
of neurotoxins pry open all the doors, which has the same eventual effect: the
cell can’t communicate any signals to other nerves or muscles. No cell can
survive without functional ion channels.
Once researchers realised how nature made neurotoxins – by throwing a
wrench into those all-important ion channels – they realised they could make
their own bespoke neurotoxins to jam shut, or pry open, only the channels of
their choice. (Ashcroft and Hattersley figured out that an existing drug could
close the errant ion channels and reverse this rare form of diabetes.) And that
was the dawn of the age of ion-channel drugs.
Ion channel drugs are a bedrock of modern medicine. They underpin
treatments you get for some snake bites by artificially pumping up the
communication between the nerves and muscles. They underpin drugs for
heart arrhythmias. Now researchers are investigating a whole slew of
movement disorders, epilepsy, migraines, and some rare inherited diseases for
possible mutant ion channels.21 All over biology, ion-channel physics
revolutionised the treatment and conceptualisation of disease and disorder. ‘It
is difficult to exaggerate our misunderstanding of heart action potentials
before we knew about calcium channels,’ wrote one cardiac
electrophysiologist.22
Ion channels are important drug targets, but our understanding of them is
incomplete. We keep finding more unexpected variations of them. One is gap
junctions, which were first noted in the heart but now seem to be in every one
of our trillion cells. A gap junction is a special ion channel that pokes between
two neighbouring cells, creating a sneaky door only they share, like adjoining
hotel rooms. In heart cells, gap junctions synchronise the activity of cells that
need to operate in tandem, but they also festoon the membranes of skin cells,
bone cells, heart cells, and they even occur on blood cells. They are
everywhere. They all talk to each other using these electrical synapses. What on
earth for?
New ion channels aren’t the only surprises. Another recent observation is
the electron current expelled by cancer cells as they make the transition out of
good health.23 On a larger scale, there are also aspects of the nervous system
we didn’t appreciate until the turn of the twenty-first century, when it started
to emerge that the nervous system doesn’t just act on the feeling and moving
bits, but also regulates organ function and the immune system. These are the
kind of insights that are beginning to form the outlines of the electrome.
Until recently, knowledge of these disparate electrical features of biology
was sequestered in narrow subdisciplines. That’s because the study of
bioelectricity had been increasingly siloed into neuroscience, and in
electrophysiology, which focused a lot on nerves and neuroscience – to the
extent that scientists assumed bioelectrics were only used by nerves.
One of the more astonishing features of the electrome is that animal
electricity is by no means confined to animals. We’re not the only ones with
these ion channels. All the other kingdoms are run on the same stuff.
Electric kingdoms
We had had glimpses of that reality, too, much earlier than we could reasonably
account for it. In 1947, the physiologist Elmer Lund found electric fields
coming off algae.24 He wasn’t alone; these confounding electric emanations
seeped from every other biological surface people thought to measure: Venus
flytraps, frog and human skin, fungi, bacteria, chick embryos, fish eggs, and oat
seedlings.
Reports from disparate fields of study indicate that the electrical signals
used by plants, bacteria, and fungi are all weirdly similar to our own, and the
research is beginning to suggest that they use these signals to very similar
effect. Bacteria use electrical calcium waves to coordinate themselves into
biofilm communities (disrupting these electrical control signals is a hot
research topic in the fight against antibiotic resistance).25 Fungi use them
(among other things) to communicate along their long tendrils whether they’ve
found a nourishing food source or a dud.26 Plants use electricity to activate
chemical defences against predators. The list goes on and on.
We have wondered in the past twenty years, as we discover ever more
similarities between their electrics and ours, why these signals (in bacteria, in
fungi, in protists) are so similar to those in our nervous system. But now a lot
of people are starting to wonder if perhaps we’ve been getting the question
backward: why are we so similar to them, and what does that mean about our
electrics?
All creatures, brains or no, use a collection of similar ions to create
voltages across their cells. We all use these voltages as a basis of
communication. Animals use them to make their nervous systems function as
a command and control centre; other kingdoms use them for signalling and
communications without a nervous system. ‘Flipping the voltage potential is
how, I think, all signalling probably began,’ says Scott Hansen, an
electrophysiologist at the UF Scripps Biomedical Research Institute at the
University of Florida.
And that is raising a wild idea: could we have another communications
system running in parallel to the nervous system? Recent research strongly
suggests our bodies are running at least two – if not more – electrical
communications networks.
Evidence has begun to accumulate that the bioelectricity in the nervous
system – the animating force behind animal spirits – is not the only electrical
communication network used by the animal body. Strange electrical features
and behaviours connect all the cells in our body. Skin, bone, blood, nerve –
any biological cell – put it in a petri dish and apply an electric field, and they all
crawl to the same end of it. It’s as if they can sense the electric field, even
though we don’t yet understand how cells could possibly sense those things.
All we know is that electric fields affect the bioelectric properties of a cell –
any cell, and sometimes whole organs – in a way that can be used to make it
do things it normally wouldn’t.
It is for this reason that some scientists are beginning to think
bioelectricity can be understood as a component of epigenetics – which
describes how the environment can cause changes that alter the way your
genes work without changing the actual DNA. ‘More and more epigenetic
factors which drive the organisation of biological information patterns and
flows are being discovered,’ writes the physicist Paul Davies.27 Bioelectricity is
emerging as a major – if as yet poorly understood – epigenetic factor, he
thinks, providing a powerful way for cells to manage epigenetic information.
But other researchers are finding that it may be more than just another aspect
of epigenetics. The word ‘epigenetic’ means ‘above the genes’. And maybe
electrical signalling functions as a kind of ‘meta-epigenetics’ – one ring to bind
them, if you will. As you will see over the next few chapters, electrical guidance
exerts control over a great many complicated aspects of biology, from how
genes are expressed to whether inflammation will commence in the immune
system.
When people think about the origin of life, obviously the first thing that
comes to mind is the genetic code. How did DNA and RNA evolve in the first
place, and lead to reproducible life, and all that jazz? There’s a second thing
that should come to mind, but usually doesn’t. How is it that you got a cell
membrane?
The cell membrane is important for a number of reasons. The first is just
practical. All the DNA and RNA in the world, reproducing all the elements, all
the nucleotides and amino acids you could possibly want for life – well, they’d
just float away in a big soup if they didn’t have a container. To do anything
remotely useful with the constituents of life, you need something to hold them
all together. So that was the membrane: the most underappreciated
evolutionary innovation.
But there’s a bigger reason the membrane is so important. As soon as
there’s a membrane, there’s a separation between an inside and an outside. And
since every cell we know about has always contained different kinds of ions,
the second you had a membrane separation, you had a voltage. That’s just
physics. After that, you just needed the proteins to form passages in the
membrane that allowed all those ions to get in and out of the cell.
These ion channels, as a group, are something like three billion years old.
Plants, fungi, animals, the whole lot of us inherited them from our eukaryotic
ancestors. Signalling certainly didn’t start with sodium channels – those only
evolved around the time the first nervous systems did, about 600 million years
ago.28 In 2015, the neurobiologist Harold Zakon published a deep
evolutionary history of ion channels and found most of the same ion channel
families are present all the way back to our last known ancestor.29 Our sodium
channel’s building blocks, Zakon found, were found in the first ion channel,
potassium. In fact, the potassium channel is the little Lego from which most
of the other channels – sodium, calcium and so on – were later formed. ‘The
motif that allows potassium to permeate the channel is very ancient, very
conserved. It’s pretty much the same from bacteria to us,’ Zakon says. ‘We
have it, every cell in our body has it, probably every cell on earth has that gene
for that channel.’
In fact, you can still find that molecular motif of the first ion channels in
bacteria today. Every subsequent channel and pump comes from that ancestral
gene.
The upshot is this: separating and moving ions across membranes is
fundamental to all living things. Nervous systems did not invent it, and we are
still nowhere near understanding the full extent of how nature recruits its
electric potential. Although literally all cell types use this self-generated
electricity, the breathtaking range of functions for which it is instrumental is
utterly underappreciated. It’s certainly not covered in textbooks of
introductory biology, at least not in such a way that would make anyone
actually appreciate the importance of the electrical dimension of life or the
deeper significance. Those elements we all ferry across our membranes –
sodium, calcium, chloride – are fossil stardust. If there are any other cells out
there in the universe, we might share that with them, too. ‘Probably every cell
in the universe,’ Zakon noted.
We knew none of this when we first started to experiment with animal
spirits and found the first hints of what would later become the bioelectric
code. We didn’t know about ion channels or patterns, and the only tools we
had to probe the animal spirits were cousins of Volta’s pile. Which is why the
first glimpse of the electrome came to us courtesy of the electrical activity of
nerves and muscles. This – as you’ll see in the next three chapters – was how
we started to learn that we could use electricity to take control of our hearts,
brains, and central nervous system.
PART 3
I n the twentieth century, better tools began to reveal the first hints that
there were patterns in bioelectric signals that might indicate health or
disease. This quickly led to the notion that electrical stimulation could be used
not only to understand the body but to improve it – by overriding the faulty
patterns with healthy ones. We could electrically control ourselves back to
health.
CHAPTER 4
Each pump of blood through the heart is set into motion by a group of cells
that is best understood as the conductor. The group of cells, located in the
upper right part of the heart, is called the sinus node. This conductor
coordinates all the cells of the heart into a precise rhythm that ensures blood
only ever enters one specific kind of chamber and only ever exits another
specific type. Blood enters a set of upper chambers called the atria, and flows
down to the ventricles (the lower chambers), which contract about half a
second later, one ventricle sending its blood to the lungs and the other sending
its blood around the body. This is quite a precise rhythm to orchestrate, and
high stakes! If you do it wrong, the heart can’t coordinate the blood
distribution around the body properly and the body will die. And it all depends
on the electrics.
The conductor kicks off all of this with an action potential, but these are
not the familiar action potentials of the nervous system. That’s because the
muscles of the heart don’t have their own nerves driving them in the same way
that nerves drive skeletal muscle. The heart is all muscle, but it’s an unusual
kind of muscle. It’s a kind of self-determining muscle that moves without you
being in charge of it – as you well know, your heartbeat is not under your
control. With a lot of practice and focus, you can learn to slow it, but you can’t
stop it in the same way you can close your eyes. Rather like nerves, the heart
muscles generate their own action potentials, except without chemical
synapses.
So how does the action potential pass from cell to cell? How is the
conductor’s signal sent to all the muscle cells through the heart? Turns out,
instead of being connected by standard synapses, they’re all connected by
direct, electrical high-speed lines – those gap junctions I referred to in the
previous chapter.9 These adjoining hotel room doors are usually left open so
the signal can instantaneously zap between rooms. Whatever one cell knows or
experiences diffuses immediately through the connecting door for its
neighbour to know or experience instantaneously. This mode of
communication is about ten times faster than a regular chemical synapse
because it does away with neurotransmitters and gaps between cells.
This is how the heartbeat’s rhythm shimmies down from the top of the
organ to the bottom to ensure that the outgoing blood is always pumped just
precisely half a second later than the incoming blood comes in.
It was this synchronised wobble that Waller was picking up. But his early
equipment was too primitive to see the details, which were only made legible
when Einthoven deployed his fancy string. That’s when we got our first look at
those sawtooth-shaped blips that you may know from medical dramas (or if
you’ve ever been hooked up to a heart monitor).
What was much more interesting than seeing the normal heartbeat,
however, was that Einthoven’s crisper readings made it possible to see when
things were not all right with the heartbeat. You could now not only visually
distinguish the signature of a healthy heart from a sick one, you could start to
detect specific ailments – for example, an abnormally slow heartbeat. This
condition, called bradycardia, means the blood can’t deliver enough oxygen to
the brain and other body tissues, so a person with this condition often feels
dizzy or weak, or faints.
Long before we fully understood how all these signals travelled and
worked, people started to use electricity to whip those errant signals into
shape.
If you think nuclear power would be a bad choice for powering an implant
close to your heart, there are 139 people who would disagree with you.14 In
the 1970s, several manufacturers rolled out pacemaker designs that were
powered by a bit of plutonium. The heat generated as this radioactive isotope
decayed was transformed into electricity that powered the circuits in the
model,15 but don’t worry, they were ‘shielded well enough to deliver very little
radioactivity to the patient’. Battery designs for implantable pacemakers only
got weirder from there, including one that ran on biological electricity not
conceptually dissimilar to Matteucci’s frog thigh battery.16
In 1958, Wilson Greatbatch found an enduring source of power that was
less unsettling than plutonium, with the invention of a pacemaker that used a
lithium ion battery, and there we still are today, for the most part.17 Within a
couple of decades, his invention had been refined into the small, implantable
gadget we understand today as the pacemaker.
The concept is simple enough and the pacemaker is implanted much the
way Hyman did it. Fortunately, no one uses a needle to pierce the heart any
more. Instead, an electrode is surgically implanted on the faulty spot that’s
causing the trouble. The electrode is attached to the pulse generator by a wire
that carries the stimulating electrical charge, conceptually not entirely unlike
the kite string Ben Franklin used to bring lightning down from the sky. Except
instead of atmospheric lightning, this conductor conducts the tiny, contained
lightning from a battery-powered stimulation unit: the pacemaker. These are
improbably tiny now, considering their trolley-top origins – about the size of a
ten-pence piece – and they keep getting smaller.
The most common use for a pacemaker is to speed up a slow heart rate (as
in bradycardia). The tiny lightning overwrites the heart’s own bioelectricity,
applying miniscule, regular electric shocks to drive the heart at the right rate.
When it reaches the muscle cells in the sinus node (that conductor in
charge of the first domino), the electrical stimulation forcibly changes the
membrane potential of the cell.18 The muscle is depolarised, which opens the
voltage-gated sodium channel, and then triggers the action potential. And then
that sets off all the rest of the cascading actions of the heartbeat.
Some of the most advanced models today don’t just zap; they can listen to
make sure they’re dispensing the right kind of zap at the right time. They sense
the wearer’s heart rhythm so as to modulate it in real time. That ability to
respond to real-time feedback puts the pacemaker in a category known as
closed-loop devices.
After Greatbatch added the lithium ion battery, things happened fast. By
the 1960s, several of the twentieth century’s biggest technological
breakthroughs – plastics, transistors, microchips, batteries – conspired to make
the pacemaker implantable and reliable.19 Engineers and scientists who
adapted it into a working device went on to found a medical device company
called Medtronic. Over the next twenty years, the number of patients with
pacemakers quickly rose from half a dozen to nearly half a million.
In the late 1960s, a neurosurgeon in Wisconsin took Medtronic’s
implantable cardiac pacemaker outside its intended environment for the first
time, repurposing it for his chronic pain patients. It was implanted in the spine
– but that was just the beginning for the pacemaker’s strange journey, which
would soon find a new home in the brain.
So did Waller’s early tracings, for that matter. After first allowing doctors to
diagnose heart conditions in hospitals, and then laying the groundwork for the
first recordings of the activity of the brain, the electrocardiograph became the
basis of much of the electrical imaging that proliferates today to diagnose sleep
and neurological disorders. These advanced brain diagnostics, in turn, opened
the door to the idea that animal electricity is the body’s way of digitising
information so that it can speak to itself in a kind of specialised neural code,
an idea that took root in the twentieth century and has flowered into the
defining idea of neuroscience in the twenty-first. Many are now convinced
that, with these descendants of Waller’s early contraption, we are mere steps
from reading the electrical activity of thoughts – and perhaps unlocking the
secrets of consciousness itself.
CHAPTER 5
I n 2016, a Silicon Valley start-up called Kernel emerged from ‘stealth mode’
to publicly announce that they were building a prosthetic memory – a
brain-implantable microchip that would not only help people with traumatic
brain injuries regain their ability to recall information but also eventually help
the rest of us become more intelligent. The possibilities were limitless, if you
believed Kernel’s founder, Bryan Johnson, who had just bet $100 million on
the idea. ‘Could we learn a thousand times faster?’ Johnson said at the time.1
‘Could we choose which memories to keep and which to get rid of ? Could we
have a connection with our computers? If we can mimic the natural function
of the brain, and we can truly work with the neural code, then I posit the
question – what can’t we do?’
If you had been reading the science journals and tech press, you might
have thought Kernel’s plan was airtight. The rate of progress in brain implants
over the previous decade had been astonishing, and Johnson had tapped into a
growing body of apparently promising academic work. He had plucked one of
the world’s most prolific biomedical engineers, Theodore Berger, from the
University of Southern California to lead the project as its chief science
adviser. Berger had been working for twenty years on writing electrical signals
into the neurons of rats and primates. He had just created an algorithm that
could decipher the code sent by one part of the brain to another, and in so
doing had apparently improved several rats’ ability to form short-term
memories.2 Now, with an infusion of Kernel’s cash, it was time for human
trials. The Matrix was upon us.
Or was it? The belief that the right kind of implants can overwrite our
normal brain activity has become practically an article of faith among the
technocracy. ‘The future of the human race depends upon our ability to learn
how to read and write our neural code,’ wrote Johnson in a subsequent Medium
post.3 But why? And where did it come from, this idea that we’ll soon let
academics and tech companies programme our minds like a PC? The story of
Kernel’s memory chip turns out to be a good parable for the limitations of our
current metaphors for the inner workings of the brain. But to understand the
problem in full requires a brief deep-dive into what people mean when they
speak of the ‘neural code’.
The muscles in the heart either respond to a stimulus or they don’t; that much
had been clear to scientists since the 1870s. The rate of a heartbeat can vary
but the beats themselves do not: there are no small or big heartbeats or half
heartbeats. A heart either beats or it doesn’t. Similarly, in early experiments, if
you stimulated a muscle fibre, it either twitched or it didn’t; there seemed to be
no such thing as a half twitch. This was why du Bois-Reymond called it ‘all or
nothing’. For the heart, this binary made sense, because a functioning heart
has one job: it just needs to beat.
But how could nerves and muscles use this same system to transmit more
complicated information to and from the brain? How could they vary the
content of the information they were carrying if all they could do was either
fire or not fire? Nerves and muscles were clearly capable of acting on much
more complex gradients of information. For example, you can choose to flex
your arm either lightly, incompletely, or max out to exhaustion. And we are all
familiar with the feeling of an initial sensation like sitting down in a chair or
putting on a soft jumper, which after a while attenuates so we stop feeling it at
all. Actions and sensations like these are hardly ‘all or nothing’.
To find out whether muscles and nerves were really part of this all-or-
nothing club, at the start of the 1910s the Cambridge University engineer and
electrophysiologist Keith Lucas dispatched the usual complement of frogs. He
was able to confirm that muscle fibres responded only when a stimulus was
strong enough to exceed a certain threshold.
So all muscles obeyed the same binary rule – they either twitched or they
didn’t. Was the same rule true for nerves? And if it was, how on earth were
they able to deal with complicated information?
Two problems blocked further understanding. The first: wherever you find
them, nerves and muscles are not single wires, but bunched together in cables.
These are a bit like the strands of fine wire ferrying signals along the ocean
floor between continents. The signals aren’t sent in one wire but separately
along individual fibre-optic lines cabled into tight bunches of varying
thickness. Similarly, the nerve ‘cables’ in the body vary in thickness, some
being very thick (like the spinal cord) others consisting of maybe a few tens of
nerves.4 The brain sends the message to the muscle, via the nerve, that it
should contract. So whenever you try to listen to them, you’ll get the din of
loads of different neurons shouting over each other. Isolating an individual
neuron to listen to its monologue was out of the question: first because it was
surgically impossible to unpick a single (live) nerve from its fibre, and second
because an instrument to detect its action potential didn’t exist.
Even when listening to the loudest multi-neuron fibres in concert, you
weren’t listening to their natural conversation. All the way back to Galvani,
every nerve or muscle signal ever measured had been ‘induced’ by artificially
stimulating the nerve to fire by applying an electrical zap. (I guess if we’re
going to stay with this metaphor, this is the equivalent to giving the nerve a
giant static shock and listening to its enraged scream.) This method placed
limits on how much you could learn about how the nervous system really
worked in the wild.
Like any good physicist, the first thing Lucas did was find someone clever
to take over the grunt work in his lab at Trinity College: a young PhD student
in physiology, Edgar Adrian. Adrian’s assignment: find out how the nervous
signal is conducted and whether it also obeyed the same all-or-nothing
principle Lucas had found in muscles.
They started by narrowing down the number of nerves in a muscle fibre
that they had to contend with. Lucas found a muscle in the frog that was
innervated by only ten nerve axons. When he stimulated it with an electrical
zap, he found that the resulting muscle contraction depended on the intensity
of the applied jolt. But that wasn’t true of the individual nerves. They
responded the same no matter the intensity of the electrical jolt: either firing or
not. More stimulation caused more of the nerve fibres to fire, and that was
what changed the amount of muscle contraction. The binary message in
individual nerves never changed.
Here was solid evidence that nerves obey the same all-or-nothing law as
muscle.5 But the team’s quest was disrupted by the First World War. Lucas left
the lab and joined the Royal Aircraft Factory, putting his engineering skills to
use for the war effort by devising new compasses and bomb sights. While
testing one of these devices in 1916, he died in a mid-air collision. When
Adrian returned to Cambridge after his mentor’s death, his obsession with
Lucas’ question had deepened. How was he going to listen to an individual
nerve impulse? No one had made a machine powerful enough to record the
signals themselves, but could you make something that amplified them
sufficiently for existing machines to record?
During the war, Adrian’s American friend Alexander Forbes worked on
wireless radio receivers, early radar tools, and new devices called vacuum tubes
which could enhance audio signals. The war had made them cheap and easy to
get your hands on. After the war ended, Forbes used them to make a new
amplifier, stuck the thing onto an Einthoven string galvanometer – and voila.
A nerve’s infinitesimal action potential could now be amplified by an
unprecedented factor of fifty, which rose over the next few years to 7,000.6
Such a great device – now if only he could find a way to listen to a nerve
bundle in its natural state, not one that was being artificially jolted into firing
by the application of electrical stimulation. Adrian got himself the blueprints
to build a device of his own, and put in an order for frogs.7
The trick would be finding a scenario where nerve firing was predictable
enough to be caught in situ and recorded. One day, he was recording the
‘resting’ state in a frog muscle. This was intended to provide a silent baseline
against which to compare the natural signals he hoped later to find. The frog
leg was just hanging there, not doing anything and not being stimulated.
Obviously no signal should have been present. And yet, every time he tried to
get a good recording of this baseline resting state, the same annoying,
unaccounted-for noise interfered, the same kind of oscillations that he had got
when actively stimulating the muscle. The interference started to drive him
around the bend, and Adrian laid the frog down on a glass plate – instantly, the
mysterious signal stopped. He picked up the frog, letting its legs dangle once
more. Signal. He put it down. No signal.
That was when Adrian suddenly realised what he was seeing. He
understood the nature of the signal he had been detecting: the nerves attached
to the legs were alerting the central nervous system that they were being
stretched. He had found the signal they used to transmit this complicated
information.
Now he’d need to find a way to record a single one of these signals as it
travelled along a single nerve. Adrian set to work, and in 1925 he and his
colleague Yngve Zotterman managed to reduce a muscle group to only one
single strip, within which remained only one single muscle, within which
remained only one single nerve. This sensory neuron was tasked with
communicating just one thing: how much the muscle was experiencing the
sensation of stretch. ‘Under strong emotional stress we hurried on,’ Zotterman
wrote, ‘recording the nerve’s response to varying degrees of stimulation.’ The
signal they recorded from the single nerve was a series of clean, steady blips –
the sound of a single, undiluted action potential. The blip was always the same.
It never got bigger or smaller no matter how it was stimulated. The only thing
that ever changed was how frequently it fired. Pull the neuron’s muscle taut,
and the blips became frequent and numerous. Give the muscle slack, and they
slowed. When the muscle was perfectly at rest on the glass plate, there were no
blips at all. Zotterman and Adrian both realised that ‘what we now saw had
never been observed before and that we were discovering a great secret of life,
how the sensory nerves transmit information to the brain’.8 It was a massive
lightbulb moment – they were the first to finally figure out how the brain gets
information from the limbs. They had cracked the code of how these blips tell
the brain useful things about the information in the environment. Stretch:
many frequent blips! Stop stretching: no blips. Something about this encoding
sytem seemed awfully familiar.
The war, with its years of effort that involved breaking code and
intercepting transmissions, had given Adrian a new conceptual lens through
which to understand what he was seeing.9 The mechanism he had found in the
nerve’s information transmission looked like a kind of bioelectric Morse code.
Nerve impulses, and the nervous system in general, had been described in
terms of information communication since the invention of the telegraph in
the previous century. But when Adrian discovered that the nerve impulse was
just a series of variable-in-time brief pulses (Morse code without the dashes),
he was struck by the way this limited signal was able to pass on complex
information (the sensation of stretch). ‘In any one fibre the waves are all of the
same form and the message can only be varied by the changes in the frequency
and duration of the discharge. In fact, the sensory messages are scarcely more
complex than a succession of dots in the Morse code.’10 You can see a similar
change in the language of Zotterman’s accounts. Years later, recounting the
frustration of previous experiments before they were able to isolate that single
neuron, he wrote that ‘it was as if we were tapping a telegraph cable with many
lines simultaneously in transmission. It did not permit any reading of the
code.’11
Adrian’s scientific and popular writings introduced the concepts that began
to define the common perception of the nervous system and, by extension, of
the bioelectric signal and its function: messages, codes, information.
This idea of a code began to bleed from single neurons into the idea of how
whole nerve systems used action potentials to translate the outside world for
interpretation by the brain. Now that there was a code being used by the
peripheral nervous system to send messages to the brain, the next thing
Adrian wanted to understand was how the brain received those signals – how
it translated the Morse code back into a language it could understand. Was the
brain a ‘central station’ that decoded the signals into experience, as Adrian
intimated in the lecture he delivered when accepting the Nobel Prize for this
work? In which case, ‘we could tell what someone was thinking if we could
watch his brain at work’.12
Even before delivering that speech, Adrian had started combing the
literature for any explanation. And while he didn’t find it, he did discover a
possible way to find it: a new machine recently invented by the German
neurology professor Hans Berger. Its findings were of ‘exceptional interest’ to
Adrian, and he and his colleagues were shocked that no one had ever tried to
repeat them.13
Almost ten years before Augustus Waller stood his first bulldog in saltwater,
the Manchester physiologist Richard Caton was looking at similar rhythmic
readings he had obtained from putting electrodes on people’s scalps. Unlike
Waller, Caton knew the significance of what he had just found. In the years
since the electrical nature of the action potential was established, speculation
had been growing as to whether the processing machinery in the brain might
have its own electrical signature as well. In 1875, Caton discovered a ‘feeble
current’ emanating even when there was no muscle activity – not in keeping
with scientific consensus of the time, according to which muscle motion
should have been the only thing capable of generating measurable brain
activity. And yet here was Caton’s patient sitting perfectly still, emanating like a
beacon.
Nearly fifty years later, his work was exhumed by Berger, then the director
of the psychiatric clinic at the University of Jena.14 Outwardly, the man was
strict and charmless in the job.15 His heart was elsewhere; he had been toiling
in secret since the 1890s on a project of immense personal significance. It all
went back to an accident he suffered in a military training exercise as a young
man. In 1892, pulling heavy artillery on horseback, Berger was thrown off, his
head landing inches in front of the wheel of an approaching artillery gun. The
cart had stopped at the very last second – by all rights Berger should have
died. When he returned to the barracks that night, shaken by the experience,
he found a telegram from his father asking if he was all right. The reason for
the inquiry: right at the time of his accident, his older sister was overwhelmed
with an inexplicable feeling of panic and pleaded with her father to make sure
nothing had happened to Hans.
Berger couldn’t square the experience with science. What could explain
such an extraordinary coincidence? He could only conclude that the sheer
intensity of his terror had assumed a physical form external to his mind, and
had been transmitted somehow instantaneously to his sister. Berger was
determined to find the psychophysiological basis for mental telepathy.
In 1902, he discovered Caton’s work on detecting the brain’s electrical
currents with an electrometer. After twenty more years of trying to find
commensurate signatures in the brain, he finally got his hands on a string
galvanometer. His first experimental subject was a seventeen-year-old college
student named Zedel, who had been left with a large hole in his skull after the
removal of a brain tumour. Berger attached Zedel’s electrodes to a string
galvanometer he had borrowed from the university hospital, where it was
normally used to do early versions of the ECG. Suddenly, there they were –
electrical tracings like the ones Waller had got from the heart, clear as day, but
this time from the brain. Evidence of electrical brain emanations at last.
But the patterns he picked up from the brain were much more varied,
faint, and noisy than the signals the clinical device had been able to detect
from the heart, and therefore much harder to analyse for coherent motifs.
Berger ordered an even bigger galvanometer. For five obsessive years he
painstakingly tweaked the apparatus to tease out meaningful patterns from all
the other interference that obscured them: the slightest bodily movements, the
heartbeat, and even the pulsations of the brain’s own blood flow.
By 1929, he had developed his new equipment enough to produce
hundreds of recordings from patients with skull defects, epilepsy, dementia,
brain tumours and other disorders, as well as from healthy controls – himself
and his son.16 All showed consistent patterns in the waveforms; they were the
same across many kinds of people. More intriguingly, they changed in similar
ways. Their shape changed when you were paying attention versus when you
had your eyes closed, for example. They changed when someone with epilepsy
was having a seizure. It appeared that the shape of these waves did indeed tell
us something about the internal processes of the brain. Finally, he had amassed
enough proof to convince himself that he had devised a ‘brain mirror’ to
reflect the mental activity of the brain. He called his new tool an
electroencephalogram, and it was the inexact first EEG – a device that could
let you eavesdrop on the electrical activity of the brain. Almost five years after
recording, Berger finally worked up the courage to publish his results.
Perhaps he had been wise to be reticent. The reception was icy, his paper
mostly ignored. Thanks to his secrecy and the dour, uninspired reputation he
hid behind, no one could believe that this little man had found anything
groundbreaking. Many of his German contemporaries openly doubted that the
oscillating waves he claimed to have found even originated in the brain. At a
conference in Paris, while Berger explained the projections of his EEG graphs
in a darkened auditorium, half the audience simply walked out.
Adrian, however, saw potential in Berger’s work. He immediately began to
work on it in his own lab, replicating and expanding it.17 Berger found, for
example, that the resting activity of the brain forms a pattern of wavelets he
named the alpha rhythm, which hums along regularly, reliably producing
between eight to thirteen of these little sawtooths every second. Intense
mental activity changed the rhythm – the faster, more irregular waves he called
beta waves. Adrian publicised Berger’s work widely, going so far as to try to
rechristen the alpha wave ‘the Berger wave’.18 He even staged a demonstration
for the Royal Society in which he took traces of himself thinking in public –
changing the shapes of his own emitted oscillations in real time.19 No bulldogs
were involved.
Reading EEG patterns now began to allow American technicians to
distinguish sleep from waking states, focus from inattention, and even healthy
brains from those with neurological disease.
In Germany, the public imagination was beginning to enter its fever phase,
and in the late 1920s and 1930s, the EEG’s ability to make recordings of the
electrical activity of the human brain began to kindle far-reaching speculations
about the imminent deciphering of the brain – and consequently, the mind. A
German journalist wrote enthusiastically that ‘today the brain writes in secret
code, tomorrow scientists will be able to read neuropsychiatric conditions in it,
and the day after tomorrow, we will write our first authentic letters in
brainscript’.20
This enthusiasm would not last. At some point, the optimistic tone
disappeared, leaving only the worst-case scenarios. A radio programme
investigated the worrying ‘electrophysiological problems of the future’.21
Editorial cartoons captured the attitude of the average German at the time:
one speculated that the addict of the future would be addled by electricity
instead of cocaine and morphine; and another – a brutalist depiction of a
person’s exposed brain irradiated by waves that shone out through his
confused eyes – envisioned brainwashing by a surveillance state. The cartoon’s
caption: ‘Intensification of suggestive forces by the supply of electrical
oscillatory energies into the brain’.22
Then there were the enterprising opportunists who seized on EEG, whose
schtick you’ll find quite familiar by now. Berger’s discovery ushered in a
booming market of quack medical appliances. One buyer asked Berger for
advice on using EEG to assess the temperament of his new horse. The head
of a women’s clinic at Tubingen tried to use EEG to establish neural signatures
of pregnancy.23 Berger was furious about all of it.
Outside Germany, by 1938 the tool was being used all over the world. It
was particularly useful for diagnosing the patterns characteristic of epileptic
seizures, sleep stages, and drug responses. The study of EEG advanced at a
particularly breathtaking speed in the US, where wartime tech and American
open-mindedness was leading to breakthroughs in theory, devices, and
praxis.24 When new EEG labs broke ground at universities, the ceremonies
attracted luminaries from all over the country. But this was not an era known
for the open sharing of scientific knowledge between Germany and other
nations, so Berger had no idea how much his EEG was already changing the
face of neuroscience in the US. He only saw what he fulminatingly called the
‘ballyhoo’ his creation had brought about in his own country. In 1941, just as
Adrian was writing up his letter recommending Berger to the Nobel
committee, the latter, mired in despair and depression, took his own life.
After seventeen years of progress in EEG technology, the field stalled for
another four decades. During this time, we decided that we’d rather send
electricity into the brain than decipher the codes hiding in the natural kind.
It was an otherwise quiet morning in 1982, and a patient named George had
just been admitted into a psychiatry unit with a diagnosis of catatonic
schizophrenia. The designation had been applied not because it fitted, but
because nothing else did. The patient was unresponsive, but he still seemed
alert, a combination outside all the existing frameworks available at the time.
The psychiatrists were sure the patient had a neurological disorder, and the
neurologists were sure the patient had a psychiatric disorder. Eventually, the
chief resident ran to the office of the director of neurology, Joseph Langston.
Langston set aside his coffee, looked up from that morning’s EEG reports,
and began conducting his own tests and consultations. Initially, he concluded
George exhibited all of the symptoms of advanced Parkinson’s disease, a cruel
neurodegenerative disorder whose iconic symptom is a trembling so violent, a
person can’t so much as hold a cup of water, and which progresses after many
years into a rigid freeze. But Langston knew the diagnosis couldn’t be right for
two reasons. The patient was only in his early forties, which was about twenty
years too young for a diagnosis of Parkinson’s. And instead of manifesting
gradually over the course of years or even decades, his (apparently) end-stage
symptoms had appeared literally overnight.
The mystery only deepened further when they found George’s girlfriend
frozen in the same state despite being even younger – she was only thirty.
Eventually, the team located five more identical cases. It took some sleuthing
and luck, but Langston and the police eventually uncovered the common
factor: each of the patients had recently used heroin – or at least what they
thought was heroin. When Langston’s group got their hands on some samples,
what they found wasn’t heroin at all: the street chemists had mistakenly
synthesised a compound called MPTP. A search through the medical literature
revealed several existing studies on MPTP, and their findings didn’t bode well
for the couple. By destroying an area deep inside the brain called the substantia
nigra, MPTP had been found to create irreversible symptoms that mirrored
Parkinson’s – notably that rigid freeze.
The discovery of that relevant brain area was consequential. During the
1970s, a few neurosurgeons had been experimenting with implanted electrodes
for chronic pain and epilepsy. They drilled open the skull and pushed a
penetrating electrode deep into the grey matter. It was a promising solution to
the big problem that had driven psychosurgery out of business: unlike the
traditional approach of burning or cutting out bothersome bits of the brain,
‘electrical lesions’ were adjustable as well as reversible. If you were applying
too little electricity, you could dial up more; if you were putting in too much,
you could dial it back down.
As they did this, doctors began to notice two patterns among the patients
exhibiting troublesome symptoms: first, the electrical stimulation alone was
sometimes enough to blunt the symptoms. Second, the faster the electrical
stimulation pulsed, the more the patients improved.
These patterns were intriguing, but the electrodes couldn’t be implanted
for take-home care. Like Hyman’s earliest pacemaker, they were hooked up to
an unwieldy external apparatus, a large power source which was connected to
the electrodes that stuck out of the head by wires.38 Moreover, no one had
done any large clinical trials to verify that stimulating some particular brain
area worked for everyone, or if it was just bespoke to the individual patient.
The only assessment of whether it worked at all was the assurance of the
implanting surgeon.39 However, all the growing interest had led Medtronic to
work on adapting their pacemaker to make it suitable for brain implantation.
They sent their experimental devices to specialist centres, and even
trademarked the term DBS (‘deep brain stimulation’). Their devices were still
limited to small, one-off experiments. But that all changed when George’s
revelatory case study made it to the desk of Alim-Louis Benabid at the
University Hospital in Grenoble, and he connected the dots.40
Benabid was among the few psychosurgeons still using implanted
electrodes to identify the correct brain area in advance of psychosurgery. He
had become fascinated with the clear and obvious effects he saw in his
Parkinson’s patients: symptoms would quiet in real time in the operating
theatre. When he grasped the significance of George’s case study, he obtained
Medtronic’s new brain pacemakers and implanted a handful of patients. The
improvements were dramatic. Stopping the faulty neurological code emerging
from that part of the brain settled the trembling, allowing patients to once
again move their limbs as they wished. Medtronic hired Benabid to design
massive trials. Now, instead of unscientific zaps accompanied by an
unconvincing thumbs-up from a clinician, here was a disease with extremely
well-spelled-out symptoms, a relevant brain area, and a robust response to an
already approved medical device.
Medtronic had been desperate to find a way to expand its hugely
successful pacemaker business. In Benabid’s work, they saw a new opportunity.
And in trial after trial they found the same dramatic effects: start the current
flowing through those deep electrodes, and trembling subsided instantly.
People who before the surgery could not hold a cup of tea were now able to
confidently brew themselves a whole pot. EU regulators approved the implant
for Parkinson’s in 1998, and the Food and Drug Administration (FDA)
approved the treatment in the US in 2002. One of the doctors who began
implanting his patients hailed it as ‘providing a new life’. The pacemaker had
moved into the brain and deep brain stimulation was born.
More than 160,000 of these ‘brain pacemakers’ have been implanted to
mitigate the disabling muscle spasms of people with Parkinson’s, essential
tremor, and dystonia.41 As brain surgeries go, it is actually pretty
straightforward. First, drill two holes in the skull. Next, push two metal
electrodes, each about the dimension of dry spaghetti, into the region of the
brain responsible for the symptoms. Finally, snake the wire through the head
and down the neck until it reaches, implanted under the skin near the
collarbone, an object about the size of a stopwatch. That’s the pacemaker! But
now it’s sending a current whose pulse and amplitude, over the following
couple of weeks, a technician will adjust until the symptoms subside.
In big clinical trials with many participants, knowing which brain area to
overwhelm with electricity has allowed surgeons to successfully short-circuit
faulty signals in these broken regions. What other brain areas were amenable
to freezing in this way, to widen the net of conditions controllable by a
pacemaker? Many small trials offered clues to the next big ailment.
In 1999, researchers at the Catholic University of Leuven (KUL) in
Belgium implanted DBS electrodes into an area called the internal capsule in
four people with severe obsessive-compulsive disorder. Symptoms improved
for three.42 More trials on other ailments followed in rapid succession – again
small, investigational studies, often only with ten or fewer participants. But
despite their small size, these trials generated dramatic headlines, as my
colleague Andy Ridgway noted in 2015 in New Scientist .43 DBS allowed a
thirteen-year-old autistic teenager to speak for the first time.44 It freed people
with Tourette’s from bone-breaking physical tics. It allegedly stopped obese
people from overeating and anorexic people from undereating.45 The small
trials proliferated – what else would yield to a brain pacemaker? Anxiety?
Tinnitus? Addiction? Paedophilia?46
Medtronic bet on depression. They weren’t alone in thinking this was
promising. The idea had been germinating since 2001, when neuroscientist
Helen Mayberg had the idea to investigate DBS for intractable depression (the
kind that refuses to budge no matter the treatment). DBS, she told me when I
met her at the International Neuroethics Society symposium in San Diego in
2018,47 ‘seemed to block abnormal brain function in Parkinson’s, so we
wanted to block our own depression-specific area’. Mayberg focused on a
brain region known as Brodmann’s area 25, which has been dubbed the brain’s
‘sadness centre’. Too much activity here, Mayberg and her colleagues thought,
was driving symptoms like negative mood and that characteristic lack of a will
to live. What would happen if you froze those neurons? Four of her first six
patients saw dramatic improvement.48 Twenty more small trials recorded
improvement rates as high as 60 or 70 per cent. ‘People come out of that very
dangerous state, and stay well,’ Mayberg told Ridgway. ‘They just get back on
the bus.’ In other trials all over the world, depression lifted similarly.
After enough such intriguing results had piled up, St. Jude Medical – a
Medtronic rival – took the plunge and bankrolled a major trial. It seemed all
but guaranteed to culminate in the first new commercial application of DBS
since Parkinson’s. Two hundred participants at more than a dozen medical
centres got the implant. The buzz was immense. And then, after six months,
the trial stopped. Gossip spread among industry insiders that it had failed an
FDA futility analysis, meant to make sure spendy trials get kneecapped if they
are clearly wasting time and money. There were stories of terrible side effects
and a suicide attempt.49 The implication – bad for the future of the technology
– was that there was no difference between a placebo and an implant.50
When all the drama and recriminations settled, the story ended up being a
lot weirder and less straightforward than the first reports suggested: as the
journalist David Dobbs concluded in a deeply reported post-mortem analysis
published in The Atlantic in 2018, it appeared to be a case of a treatment that
actually seemed to work being sabotaged by a trial that didn’t. For the people
in whom it worked, it was a gift, yielding immediate results that were so
dramatic as to be nearly magical. ‘What did you do?’ would be the response of
the awake patient, still in the operating room, the moment the stimulator was
turned on. And when that happened, the results were enduring. ‘If you got
better, you stayed better, given continued stimulation,’ Mayberg told several
media outlets in the wake of the trial. The same pattern held true for
obsessive-compulsive disorder: those patients who had responded well in a
brain stimulation trial at the Catholic University of Leuven still had their
obsessive-compulsive disorder under control after fifteen years. ‘It was like
someone spring-cleaned out my brain and took out all the unnecessary
thoughts,’ another participant told Alix Spiegel, host of the National Public
Radio show Invisibilia.51
But it was just not possible to predict who would have a miracle and who
wouldn’t. There were also some strange side effects.52 The deep, ancient areas
of the brain targeted for electrification in depression and Parkinson’s are
involved in much more than motor and mood control. They are implicated in
learning, emotion, and reward – which is to say, addiction. Interfering with
these had unpredictable consequences. This was the case for one Dutch man
being treated for severe obsessive-compulsive disorder, anonymised by his
doctors at the University of Amsterdam as Mr B. His new brain implant had
been working for just a few weeks when he chanced upon a recording of
Johnny Cash’s ‘Ring of Fire’. In the five decades before the twin electrodes
penetrated his deep brain, he had never been especially moved by music – he
was the kind of guy who claimed to like both The Beatles and the Rolling
Stones. The day Johnny Cash’s voice hit his newly electrified pleasure centres,
however, all that changed. From then on, no other music was allowed. Mr B
bought every Johnny Cash CD and DVD he could get his hands on. But when
the electrical stimulator was off, he could not for the life of him recall what
was so important about Johnny Cash.53
Not all side effects were as endearing, though. People with Parkinson’s
implants have reported an increase in impulse control disorders, such as
excessive gambling and hypersexuality.54
This reflects a somewhat uncomfortable open secret about DBS: despite
all the complicated talk about the function of precise areas of the brain, no
one is sure exactly how DBS works.55 As recently as 2018, academic reports
described DBS as an effective but ‘poorly understood’ treatment, even in
Parkinson’s and the other motor diseases it has been approved to treat for
decades.56 ‘If you think of neurons executing the neural code as playing a
melody on a piano,’ says Kip Ludwig, a former director at the US National
Institutes of Health (NIH), ‘then DBS is like playing that piano with a mallet.’
There’s a limit to this approach. Electrifying specific brain areas could
broadly control some maladies, but it was impossible to get truly granular
enough to reliably hit a target as ephemeral as depression. We needed to know
exactly what was happening in the brain’s code as a response to those zaps.
For that, we needed to decipher the neural code.
By the 1970s, Francis Crick had grown bored of molecular biology, even
though he’d essentially invented it. He was looking to solve his next great
mystery. If cracking the blueprint for life had been exciting, how about
cracking the secret of consciousness? So in 1977, he left Cambridge for the
Salk Institute in California, where he turned his attention to what he
considered a deeply unpromising approach to neuroscience. He demanded
new ‘theories dealing directly with the processing of information’ and ways to
tie behaviours and actions to the neural firings that accompanied them.
In 1994, he summarised his research in The Astonishing Hypothesis, a slim
volume that would have an explosive impact on neuroscience and philosophy.
‘One may conclude,’ he wrote, ‘that to understand the various forms of
consciousness we need to know their neural correlates’.57 He further argued
that all those things we think or feel or see ‘are, in fact, no more than the
behaviour of a vast assembly of nerve cells and their associated molecules’.58
(He did not address how this was materially different from our identity being
no more than the behaviour of a vast assembly of genes.) The subtitle – The
Scientific Search for the Soul – made clear the book’s ambition.
In the whole two decades before Crick’s book was published, fewer than
ten peer-reviewed scientific papers referred to the term ‘neural code’. After the
release of The Astonishing Hypothesis, however, neuroscientists increasingly
turned their attention to trying to find the neural signatures of a vast array of
behaviours and thoughts. For students of the sensorium, neural code was the
new black.
Not that they knew what the term meant. Even as Crick was writing his
book, the definition of the term was the subject of a contentious spat in
neuroscience. Adrian’s ideas that information could be encoded in the Morse
code dots passed by individual neurons still had its adherents, but now there
was a fresher idea. Brain plasticity – summarised by the axiom ‘neurons that
fire together, wire together’ – became ubiquitous thanks to its succinct
explanation of how different neurons learn to work together as you learn
different skills, from language to ballet. A real neural code couldn’t possibly
focus on the firing of single neurons, representatives of the new guard wrote
in 1997, but had to take into account the way vast collections of different
neurons fired together in synchrony to form a coherent pattern over time and
space.59
It would be one heck of a hard thing to measure. By then, we had begun to
grasp the sheer size of the brain – 86 billion neurons. No tool was, or is (or
possibly will ever be), capable of reading the activity of all those neurons at the
same time. But as the twenty-first century approached, we had options.
The trusty EEG was still in use, a workhorse that had given us the
different waves that could reveal focus and inattention, but also much more.
Scientists spent decades using these readings to advance our understanding of
sleep. Because EEG doesn’t require opening the skull, just some electrodes on
the scalp, scientists were able to obtain a lot of data from a lot of people. The
EEG evolved from its humble origins in Hans Berger’s lab into skullcaps
encrusted with dozens of electrodes that could read subtle variations in the
chorus of the brain’s billions of denizens. That helped drill down into the
brainwaves, and the subsequent discovery of delta and gamma waves (in
addition to Adrian’s alpha and beta) helped researchers identify the different
stages of sleep, yielding the familiar, ever-deeper stages of I–IV and dreaming
sleep. Other work tied characteristic disruptions in these waveforms to sleep
disorders and neurological disorders – even helped identify the location of
brain tumours. Thanks to increasingly powerful computer processing power
and better signal-processing algorithms, EEG could more finely analyse the
brain’s patterns. Depression was correlated with an overabundance of alpha
waves in the EEG. In Parkinson’s disease, there was a paucity of beta waves.
Alzheimer’s patients have been found to have a deficit of high-amplitude
gamma waves. Various research papers reported a rainbow of emotions
correlated to waveforms Berger couldn’t have dreamed of.60
Another tool, electrocorticography (ECoG), could get deeper into the
brain, but it would be amenable to a smaller population. It looked like a mat of
electrodes placed directly onto the exposed folds of the brain, a bit like a doily
on a side table, to record electrical activity from the cortex. It does require
opening the skull, which is why it is rare to obtain these sorts of traces. The
only human volunteers for this kind of brain reading have already had their
skull opened for unrelated investigational purposes. Sometimes these
individuals gave researchers permission to put the mesh on their brains and
read the neural correlates of certain specific thoughts, but it still didn’t let the
researchers get next to any specific neurons.
For that, you needed an invasive brain-penetrating electrode. The first of
these was approved to stick into human brains in the 1990s. It was called the
Utah array, and it looked like a small metal square with ninety-six electrodes
sticking out of it, a bit like a bed of nails for a ladybird. Nestled into brain
folds, it could record the squabbling of many neurons talking to each other, or
hone in on one in particular. But this reading was the most invasive of all: it
required not only opening the skull (or drilling a hole in it), but also jamming
the penetrating electrode through the blood–brain barrier, and running a lead
out of the skull to power and listen to the array. The only subjects on whom it
was considered ethical to test this device were animals – and later, people who
had suffered the kind of irreversible physiological trauma that made this the
last faint hope for the possibility of a research breakthrough that would help
them.
By 2004, the theoretical neuroscientist Christoph Koch – a buddy of
Crick’s who profoundly influenced his ideas about the neural correlates of
consciousness – predicted that thanks to these and other new tools, it would
soon be possible to decipher the workings of the neural code to understand
consciousness, language, and intentionality.
Around the turn of the twenty-first century, the evidence for this
optimistic reading of the future was all around us in media reports. In 1993, an
invasive electrode placed into the cortex of a woman who had been paralysed
by a stroke allowed a computer to determine where exactly on a square of
letters she was focusing her attention. ECoGs detected the electrical signatures
of people thinking in whole words: ‘yes’, ‘no’, ‘hot’, ‘cold’, ‘thirsty’, ‘hungry’,
‘hello’, and ‘goodbye’.61
So it seemed that, in line with Koch’s predictions, you could indeed use the
brain’s electrical signals to peer into people’s minds. By 2022, at least fifty peer-
reviewed papers every year were invoking the term ‘neuron code’. Many of
these papers investigated what actions, thoughts, and feelings could be traced
back to bioelectric signals in the brain.
That raised a new question: if you could read the brain’s state by examining
its electrical signals, what would happen if you changed them? Could you
reprogram the brain?
On 22 June 2004, a little metal pincushion was pressed into Matt Nagle’s
motor cortex, specifically, into the region that controlled his dominant left
hand and arm. After he had been paralysed from the neck down in an
accident, the neuroscientist John Donoghue had signed him up for a clinical
trial called BrainGate, and implanted him with a Utah array. Eventually, Nagle
was able to move a computer cursor with just his intentions. If he wanted to
move the cursor to the left, the motor neurons in his brain fired how they
normally would have to control his fingers. The Utah array picked up that
signal, translated it into machine language to control the cursor, and the cursor
moved left. That’s how, in 2005, Nagle beat a Wired magazine reporter at the
computer game Pong.62
Donoghue had much bigger plans. If these signals could drive a robot arm,
why couldn’t researchers find a way to take control of a real arm – namely
Nagle’s own? In 2005, he told Wired that his eventual plan was to ‘hook
BrainGate up to stimulators that can activate muscle tissue, bypassing a
damaged nervous system entirely’.63 It was ambitious and very exciting (if a bit
Frankenstein): instead of trying to heal the spinal cord injury that had
disconnected the limbs from the brain, the BrainGate implant would beam the
electrical signals that drove intent to their intended endpoint directly, and so
reanimate the limbs.
The idea was called a neural bypass, and within a decade, it was being
demonstrated in a TED talk.64 ‘The idea is to take signals from a certain part
of the brain and reroute them around the injury – whether that injury is to the
brain or the spinal cord – and then reinsert those signals back into the muscles
to allow them to regain movement,’ Chad Bouton told the audience, pacing
the stage like a TV-handsome talk show host. Bouton was the engineer who
had developed the signal processing algorithms for the original BrainGate
project. ‘But we still had not given [our subjects] movement,’ he said,
frustrating his vision of helping people with life-altering spinal injuries to walk
again. When, in 2008, Cyberkinetics, the company that owned BrainGate
failed, Bouton moved to the Feinstein Institute in Manhasset, New York, to
begin work on his neural bypass. Under the auspices of a project funded by the
US Defense Department, he joined a research supergroup with the science
institute Battelle and Ohio State University, and in 2014 they implanted a
computer chip into the motor cortex of a young man named Ian Burkhardt,
who had become quadriplegic after a diving accident.
For Bouton and the others working on restoring fine motor control,
deciphering the neural code was not a matter of counting the action potentials
generated by every nerve the way Edgar Adrian did it. In a brain full of 86
billion neurons, you can’t possibly pick out and analyse the spiking behaviour
of the billion or so involved in every motion. Bouton thought the thing to
focus on instead was how groups of neurons synchronise the timings of their
firing when any particular intention was recorded. He called it a ‘spatio-
temporal’ relationship. After harvesting this pattern, they would re-encode it
into machine language, and this would animate a cuff of electrodes around
Burkhardt’s wrists. Instead of actuating motors on a robot limb (the way
BrainGate had done), each electrode would stimulate tiny swatches of muscle
in his own arm.
It wasn’t exactly the way a brain signal would innervate the muscle, but the
tortured mathematical transformations worked. With the help of the device,
Burkhardt picked up a mug of water and lifted it to his own lips to take a sip.
Ian Burkhardt had become the first person to use a chip to ‘reanimate’ his own
living muscles with the neural code harvested from his own brain.65 The
signals were precise enough to let him play Guitar Hero.66
But Bouton still wasn’t totally satisfied. What good is it to be able to move
if you have no sense of what you’re touching? This was a practical question. ‘It
seems so simple to you and me, but if you have no sense of touch in your
hands, no pressure or slip awareness, you don’t know if your grip is tight
enough,’ Bouton told me when I visited his lab at Feinstein a couple of years
later. Without that grip awareness you’d be just as likely to drop the cup as
suddenly crush it and spill hot coffee all over yourself (and your lack of pain
awareness would make you unaware that you’ve just given yourself second-
degree burns that need a doctor). To avoid this, an implantee must train their
full attention exclusively on gripping that cup from the moment they pick it up
to the moment they let it go. ‘There was one guy with the implant,’ Bouton
said, ‘he could pick stuff up, but as soon as he wanted to do or think about
literally anything else, he’d drop whatever he was holding.’ Imagine if you had
to do that every time you wanted a sip of coffee. And now imagine it’s not a
cup of coffee you’re holding, but your kid’s hand. All these activities of daily
life would be meaningless without sensation. A half measure, thought Bouton.
The sensorimotor cortex, where sensation lives, is right near the motor
area where intention lives. That’s the good news. The bad news was that
writing the correct pattern of electrical spikes into the brain to replicate the
experience of sensation would be a much harder problem than reading existing
signals as they fired.
Almost exactly six months later, a paralysed volunteer called Nathan
Copeland – working with a different research group at the University of
Pittsburgh – lay blindfolded next to a five-fingered robotic arm. Each time a
researcher prodded one of the robot’s ‘fingers’, Copeland identified where on
his own hand he was feeling the touch. ‘Index finger,’ he noted as the
researcher touched the machine’s index finger. ‘Middle finger. Ring finger,’ and
so on, and thus followed a long series of correct localisations.67 In addition to
the usual BrainGate-style motor cortex implants, Copeland had had two
electrode arrays implanted into areas of his brain whose neurons respond to
sensation in the fingers (each implant was about the size of a sesame seed).
Every time the researcher jabbed the robot’s finger, these little seeds sent
electricity patterns into the correct neurons.68
This mechanism interested Theodore Berger (no relation to Hans), but
instead of implanting sensations, his electrodes would spark artificial
memories.
So when can you expect to buy your own exocortex? The Utah array – whose
design has remained largely unchanged since its invention – is the single FDA-
approved device and remains the only game in town for those looking to read
or write the neural code. Just to be clear: it’s approved for research. Not for
you and me. Regulatory hurdles have kept more advanced implants from
advancing enough to crack the language of the brain. Many devices produce
intriguing results in rats (or sometimes monkeys), newspapers go bananas, but
then they stall before hitting the market. Why? The answer is always the same.
There is a huge difference between the hardware that can read complex signals
in lab animals and the kind that you are allowed to experimentally put into the
brain of a human volunteer.
But as a brain–computer interface, this postage stamp-sized miniature
pincushion leaves much to be desired. It can only read signals from at most a
couple of hundred neurons, and only the kind that populate the topmost
millimetre of the brain. And no, you can’t just fill the brain with a whole bunch
of them. Any more than a couple, and the wires that connect the chip to the
signal processing apparatus outside the skull pose an increasingly serious
infection risk. Not to mention the prohibitive amount of data this would
generate – more than can be reasonably stored by today’s machines.76
While dramatic implants like BrainGate got a lot of publicity, after the TV
lights and cameras went away, some participants found their devices stopped
working. After years spent learning to use her implant with a robot arm, Jan
Scheuermann, another tetraplegic volunteer in Donoghue’s experiments at
BrainGate, gradually lost her dexterity, which felt an awful lot like descending
into paralysis for the second time. As the researchers explained to her, this was
due to a predictable immune response.77 It shouldn’t come as a surprise that
the brain experiences a metal pincushion as a foreign invader and mounts a
spirited defence, walling off the implant in a protective sheath. As a result, the
Utah array is unlikely to be the foundation of any future brain chip.
There was once a design that ostensibly addressed this problem – in the
1990s, a neuroscientist named Phil Kennedy designed an alternative to the
Utah array. Instead of 100 pins sticking their noses into the neurons to
eavesdrop on their conversation, his ‘neurotrophic electrode’ worked on the
opposite principle – make them come to you. The electrode was a glass cone
that housed a gold wire impregnated with growth factors and other tempting
treats for neurons. Instead of mounting an immune response, they would grow
into and entwine themselves with the electrode, which in theory should keep
the electrode functioning for years. Oh, and it was wireless.
In 1998, Kennedy put one of these into a Vietnam veteran named Johnny
Ray, who had been rendered unable to move or speak by a stroke; he was fully
aware but locked in. Kennedy’s electrode was able to pick up Johnny Ray’s
brain signals well enough to allow him to move a cursor across a keyboard to
slowly put together words. The media compared Kennedy to Alexander
Graham Bell, but the accolades didn’t last. After another one or two locked-in
subjects didn’t respond as well, and Kennedy couldn’t find new volunteers, the
FDA rescinded approval for the neurotrophic electrode for human volunteers.
Kennedy wouldn’t provide clear data on what kinds of things he was putting in
the electrodes he was implanting into his volunteers. And, now that we had the
Utah array, there was not a lot of urgency. By the 2010s, Kennedy was
desperate. In a last effort to get enough data to convince the FDA to re-
approve his implant, he chose the only patient he could. In 2014, Kennedy
flew to Belize to have his own (banned for human use) electrode implanted
into his own (perfectly healthy) brain by an increasingly nervous neurosurgeon,
for $30,000. The procedure would have been illegal in the US.
Kennedy survived the eleven-and-a-half-hour surgery, and despite a few
scary post-operative days during which his state resembled the locked-in
patients he used to treat, a few years later he seems to have come through the
experience largely intact. Unfortunately, the electrode only stayed in his brain
for a few months before there was a problem. A second surgery excised the
recording and transmission equipment, but not the electrodes – they were in
too deep to safely get out.78
After the stunt, the FDA is unlikely to ever take another look at Kennedy’s
paperwork. Kennedy insists he got good enough data to inform future papers,
and apparently doesn’t have lingering effects from the things that are in his
brain. However, now there are several other new designs in the works –
another electrode called Neuropixels is already being used to record data in
patients undergoing DBS implantation.79 It’s not yet approved but has a
similar design to Kennedy’s neurotrophic electrode, able to record deeper in
the brain. And then there’s neural dust – micron-size piezoelectric sensors that
would be scattered throughout the brain and use reflected sound waves to
capture electrical discharges from nearby neurons.80 The one you have
probably heard of is neural lace, the stuff Elon Musk sewed into a pig using a
robot sewing machine. The most recent entrant is neurograins; salt grain-sized
sprinkles unveiled in 2021 to make a better ECoG.81 They are proliferating,
largely because money has been pouring in. BlackRock, the investment fund
that has financed the neurograins, is on record as stating that they want brain
chips to become more common than pacemakers.82
There are three essential problems facing the further development of brain
interfaces. One that sometimes gets overlooked is that when it comes down to
it we don’t actually understand much about how the brain really works. ‘What
gets forgotten in a lot of these conversations is how little we know about the
brain,’ says Flavio Frohlich, a neuroscientist at the University of North
Carolina. ‘There are very few facts that have been independently verified – I
mean like basic fundamental stuff, including visual processing.’ This is where
the device Johnson pivoted to may help. Kernel is now working on a new kind
of brain-reading headgear – no brain surgery required – that combines the best
of both EEG and fMRI: magnetoencephalography (MEG). This provides a
kind of Google street view for your brain, showing where electrical activity is
happening. MEG used to be feasible only with superconductors, which have to
be cooled with liquid nitrogen, so the machine was about the size of one of
Einthoven’s early ECG contraptions. Johnson’s design uses laser cooling; the
only remaining problem is that like the early galvanometers Nobili was
working to improve, the MEG is overwhelmed by the Earth’s magnetic field.
‘It is wildly bigger than the magnetic field of your brain,’ says Marblestone.
Which is why for now, the helmet looks like a large white plastic mushroom,
Mario Kart by way of Spaceballs. But at least it’s useful.
There’s a lot of open road between current brain implants and any Silicon
Valley applications. Complex and subjective functions like memory would
require reading the output and then manipulating so many neurons, it is
unlikely that we will ever be able to influence them through this method.
There is also the problem of how many pins you can actually stick into
someone’s brain before the brain begins to fight back. This all sounds abstract
until you think about the fate of people like Ian Burkhardt, whose paralysis is
only temporarily abated every time he volunteers to be experimented on in the
lab.83 Jan Scheuermann, the woman who gradually lost her ability to use her
robotic arm, told MIT Technology Review reporter Antonio Regalado that she
once had her care aides put rat ears and a tail on her in a darkly funny nod to
how she sometimes thinks scientists might perceive her.84 Further advances in
brain implants will require many more people like Burkhardt and
Scheuermann.
However, until we have the answers to the first two problems, no
government oversight will responsibly allow trials on large enough groups of
human volunteers to constitute a proper trial. And yet, these are not the
messages that dominate about the future of brain implants. That’s because not
a lot of people feel empowered to call bullshit on the more extreme claims.
Few areas contain subject matter more opaque and that requires more cross-
disciplinary knowledge than neuroengineering. Unscrupulous start-ups
capitalise on the confusion to provide air cover for claims that don’t stand up.
Kernel’s story is a rare exception in that the company followed the science
where it wanted to go, not where they wanted it to go.
And these challenges don’t just apply to the brain.
The neural code is only one aspect of a much larger system of body-wide
bioelectrical signalling.
CHAPTER 6
I n 2007, Brandon Ingram reached out for his walker, cantilevered himself
up from his wheelchair, and, once he had righted himself, began to take
small steps across the carpeted living room. It took great effort and some help,
but he was ultimately able to control his own legs by using his abdominal
muscles.1
This should not have been possible. Five years earlier, Ingram had been
thrown from a car in a highway accident and the injury to his spinal cord was
final. Doctors told him that he would not walk again.
And yet here he was, walking. It was a bit of a technicality, as he still
needed a wheelchair for most things, but he had regained other capacities that
are far more important when you have a spinal injury: shifting his position,
and feeling some sensation. ‘I’m very fortunate,’ Ingram told the Boston Globe.2
His good fortune was that his accident took place just as a new clinical trial
was recruiting spinal injury patients at Purdue University in Indiana. Several
days after his injury, a neurosurgeon placed electrodes between the vertebrae
of his crushed spine, where they emitted an electric field. This field, the
researchers hoped, would coax opposing ends of the severed motor and
sensory nerves in Ingram’s spine to slowly crawl towards each other over the
damaged site and knit themselves back together, allowing the brain’s signals to
flow through once more unimpeded. The implant was removed after a few
months. When the researchers followed up with Ingram and his cohort a year
later, most trial participants reported some improvements.
In 2019 – twelve years after Ingram’s tentative steps – the scientist who
invented the device that mitigated Ingram’s grim prognosis died, and with him,
much of the expertise around the oscillating field stimulator. Though it had
been deemed safe and appeared to do what no other drug or technology had
ever managed, and a larger trial had already been tentatively approved by the
US regulatory authorities, soon after Ingram’s comments to the Globe, the
research stopped cold.3 Only fourteen people have ever benefited from it, and
after years of having its development blocked at every turn, the company
tasked with bringing it into the world went bankrupt. The device was
mothballed.
There are still people who get very upset about the circumstances around
its demise. ‘I think this set back spinal injury research by ten years,’ says James
Cavuoto, who runs the influential industry publication Neurotech Reports.
‘Where would we be today if they hadn’t scared off the researchers and
investors who wanted to pursue this line of research?’ Sidelined by an
establishment that didn’t understand the principles behind its function,
attacked by point-scoring competitors whose animus was more personal than
professional, the oscillating field stimulator, Cavuoto reckons, was simply too
far ahead of its time and too unfamiliar to succeed – too much of a departure
from how people were thinking about interfacing biology and electricity at the
time.
That’s because this implant wasn’t targeting action potentials: it aimed to
harness a more fundamental electrical field, whose existence hadn’t been
officially acknowledged until the 1970s. The same electrical signature is
emitted and used by skin, bones, eyes, by every organ in your body. New
research and tools have begun to shed light on the physiological underpinnings
of this bioelectric field, illuminating its inner workings and medical potential.
The 2020s will bring more devices and techniques designed to manipulate it.
As usual, though, to understand this tale in full, we have to go back (very
briefly!) to the beginning.
It all started with those old studies that showed everything produces its own
electric field – brainless organisms like hydroids, algae, oat seedlings. Lionel
Jaffe began trying to unpick the mystery in the 1960s, when few
electrophysiologists wanted to study anything but the nervous system. But
Jaffe, a Harvard-trained botanist with the soul of a physicist, was in pursuit of
bigger, more unifying theories.
A good place to start was with brown algae (that’s marine fucus to you, if
you’re nasty). Fun fact: fucus contains up to eight times more sodium than
cheddar cheese and eleven times more potassium than bananas. Maybe we’ll all
be eating it in the future. But the reason biologists love the stuff is that it’s a
sexual organism that discharges its sperm and eggs directly into seawater
(you’re welcome). That makes it possible to study its entire developmental
procession from day one, without having to navigate any tricky uteruses. The
algae grow differently on one side versus the other, based on which end of
them is exposed to sunlight.
To investigate their electrical characteristics close up, Jaffe got a bunch of
the fucus fronds and put them into a Purdue hot tub to commingle their
efflux. Once he had his new developing embryos, he put them into a neat row
in a narrow tube, shone some light on one end to mimic sunshine, and
checked whether there was any measurable electrical field as the embryos
began to grow. And boy was there. Positive up, negative down. It was like a
battery. Now he needed some smart kids to help him investigate why.
Purdue was one of the premier electrophysiology institutions in the world,
so talent was plentiful. Jaffe decided to poach the most promising students
from the physics department. His first catch was Ken Robinson, who
abandoned vacuum physics after attending his first Jaffe class. He was in awe.
‘He had the most accurate and intuitive understanding of physics and
mathematics of anybody that I ever knew,’ Robinson told me. ‘I was just blown
away’.
Next, Jaffe turned Richard Nuccitelli’s head away from solid state
engineering. ‘Who ever would have thought cells could make electrical
currents?’ he marvels, fifty years on. He left physics and crammed entire
semesters of biology to try to catch up with the rest of his labmates. They
were glad to have him. ‘He was the most gifted technician I have ever laid eyes
on,’ said Robinson. In 1974, Nuccitelli built Jaffe a brand-new electrical
measurement device called a vibrating probe. It was a hundred times more
sensitive and powerful than anything that had come before. With this, the
newly assembled team could start to properly investigate the minuscule
electrical currents that swirled around the surface of fertilised fucus eggs.
These currents were much, much smaller than the ones that make an action
potential go pop. The team christened them ‘physiological currents’. In
addition to being weak, they were also steady: where action potentials oscillate
like strobe lights, the physiological currents shone from the organisms, steady
as a lightbulb.
The field seemed to orient the fucus, enabling it to grow correctly towards
the sun. What would it do for other creatures? Jaffe decided to make his own
weak electric field, being careful to exactly mimic the faint strength of the
physiological fields that emitted from brown algae eggs naturally, and apply it
to other living things.
First up: the spinal neurons of frogs. These had been chosen by Mu-ming
Poo, the biophysicist on the team, in keeping with the centuries-old tradition
that had been inaugurated with Galvani’s preparation. Poo placed the cells into
a petri dish, put them under the physiological field, and watched. A curious
behaviour emerged. As the neuron grew its neurites (the broad name for the
outgrowths of neurons that includes axons and dendrites), they extended more
quickly towards the positive electrode. The neurites just seemed to prefer that
end of the electric field.4
To go back to Faraday’s ions for a minute – it’s not just ions that wander
towards their preferred electrical ‘side’. Turns out, whole cells can wander too.
Jaffe’s group hadn’t been the first to observe this phenomenon – people had
been seeing electrotaxis (the migration of cells under electricity) since the
1920s.5 It genuinely freaked people out. There was simply no plausible
explanation for a bunch of cells crawling around a petri dish chasing an electric
field. People chalked it up to a poorly understood chemical effect and did their
best to ignore it. What was different now, in Jaffe’s lab, was that for the first
time, there was an instrument and new knowledge to study this phenomenon
properly.
The experiments and theories that came out of Jaffe’s lab united a whole
branch of cell electrophysiology, which because it took place outside
neuroscience, had previously been littered across a collection of separate
disciplines. Jaffe’s lab was considered a second home by many of his students.
He was incredibly dedicated to his science and to the scientists who worked
under him. Robinson was inspired by his fearless approach to seeking the
truth. ‘He never let data follow a hypothesis – only the opposite,’ he told me.
‘If you got a result that didn’t match the data, he didn’t get upset about it,’ said
Nuccitelli. ‘He would drop everything and say “we have got to pursue that and
find out what it’s telling us”.’ Mu-ming Poo is now one of the towering giants
of neuroscience, with joint appointments at the University of California
Berkeley and the Chinese Academy of Science. Jaffe’s inner sanctum was his
lab family. Then Richard Borgens came to town.
The Texan
The apostate
Ken Robinson was worried. He had wondered, reading Borgens and Shapiro’s
paper, what accounted for the decision to choose exactly fifteen minutes
between switching between cathode and anode. But when he poked around in
the references for the justification – he found himself faced with his own
name. This was a big surprise for Robinson, as the paper in question had in no
way addressed this idea. ‘It misrepresented my work,’ he says.
Robinson had never seen any mammalian neurons respond to
physiological fields the way his amphibian neurons did. To get any effects at all
required one or two orders of magnitude higher fields. So Robinson tried to
repeat the experiment in zebrafish. It should have been a formality, a box-
ticking exercise. Instead, his zebrafish neurons were unmoved by any
interaction with the ‘physiologic’ currents. ‘We were stopped cold,’ he says.
‘You could not extrapolate from amphibians to other animals and assume they
were the same, especially mammals. And that caused me to start looking at the
whole picture.’
In 2007, Robinson brought his misgivings to Shapiro in a long letter,
asking whether the team had ever actually directly observed the vaunted
bidirectional growth in any of the frogs. ‘Well, I got no answer whatsoever,’ he
says. Robinson began to worry that the experiments had been unethical. ‘They
just did not have sufficient grounds for doing it.’
How could a scientist make any claims for an intervention when there was
no one in the study who was not receiving the treatment, as was the case for
this safety trial, by design? How could they account for the placebo effect?
There was no way to check the volunteers’ self-reported improvements against
spinal neurite growth – it’s not like you could cut them up for dissection. The
comparison groups Shapiro and Borgens used were case studies from other
experiments unrelated to theirs. Sure, none of the ten subjects were harmed,
but ‘those experiments were unethical even if they didn’t harm anyone,’
Robinson insists. Skipping the necessary foundational work, he says, meant the
design of the stimulator was utterly arbitrary, and this alone rendered the
experiments unethical. Founding a company and selling the device
compounded the offence.
This argument formed the rough gist of a review article, which Robinson
and his colleague Peter Cormie published in 2007, after deeming that they had
waited long enough for a response to their letter.30 It eviscerated Borgens’
work. Its most immediate effect was to alienate Robinson from the rest of
Jaffe’s descendants. The break was so immediate and complete that today,
retired in Oregon, he still refers to himself as ‘the apostate’. This term is
usually reserved for religious acolytes who turn their back on their beliefs. The
acerbic review was the first drop in a hailstorm.
Against expectations, the acquisition by Cyberkinetics had not cleared a
path for trials. In fact, nobody told Shapiro about the acquisition. ‘I was
unaware,’ he says. He had been plugging away at his fastidious roadmap,
having implanted another two patients, and things were going well. ‘All of a
sudden, this company came in, took all my study documents and devices, and
shut me down.’
What no one knew was that, by 2007, Cyberkinetics was near bankruptcy
and desperate for a commercial product. ‘They tried to ram the device through
the FDA on a compassionate use basis on the basis of twelve patients,’ Shapiro
says. ‘I knew that would fail.’ At some point – in addition to failing to grant the
humanitarian exemption – the FDA also appears to have withdrawn its
approval for any more implanted patients in a phase two trial.
But none of this was ever made clear to Borgens. The FDA just dragged
its feet on approvals, in a move that seemed calculated to run out the clock
until no one had any money left, and the project withered and died.
Quick aside for a moment in defense of the FDA: the Food and Drug
Administration might be the most underfunded, overworked, and unfairly
maligned regulatory agency in the US. They are tasked with making sure every
drug and device meets its claims and doesn’t kill people. Business-friendly
political administrations like to starve it of funds, believing that the agency
really enjoys getting in the way of innovation. But when the FDA can’t do its
job properly, you get things like vaginal mesh implant disasters and breast
implants that leak. It’s thanks to the FDA that faulty ventilators were recalled
during the Covid-19 pandemic before they could kill someone.
However, as the oscillating field stimulator was going through its
tribulations, the FDA was very different from the agency it is today, and this
played a part in the device’s demise.
Like James Cavuoto, Jennifer French was frustrated by the way the agency
handled the Andara device’s approval. French, a patient advocate for the FDA,
saw the entire process from the inside. She knows a thing or two about spinal
injuries. In 1998, after a snow-boarding accident permanently damaged her
spine, she became tetraplegic. A year later, French volunteered to become one
of the first people in the world to test a cutting-edge new electrical implant
called the Implanted Neural Prosthesis, which temporarily restores to
paralysed people the ability to stand and move. It does so by injecting pulses
of current into the muscles and nerves through precisely placed electrodes.
Serving as a test pilot for this cutting-edge neuroengineering gave her
unparalleled insight into the gap between what people needed and what
researchers were providing. She quickly became involved in advocating for
people with neurological conditions, specifically to help the agencies tasked
with sorting the promising advances from the snake oil.
For French, the OFS’s ability to restore sensation had been the most
compelling and statistically significant outcome. Sensation is an absolutely vital
priority for people with spinal injuries. It is crucial to warding off pressure
sores – breaks in the skin. When you can’t feel your skin, these can go
unnoticed and then become infected and turn septic, poisoning the blood.
Sepsis is one of the top two causes of death among people with spinal cord
injury.
However, when it came to evaluating evidence of a product’s efficacy, the
FDA was less interested in what patients in trials had to say about a device’s
impact on their life than in what it considered more ‘objective’ measures,
which is why, in 2007, the measurements the FDA used to evaluate a device
didn’t include sensation. ‘It’s considered a black box,’ she explains. The sort of
evidence they wanted could be evaluated independently by a clinician with a
focus on motor activity. Today, thanks to the advocacy of people like Jen
French, that has changed, and the FDA takes patient-reported outcomes much
more seriously than they used to.
But at the time, this meant that the FDA didn’t really see what the fuss was
about. This device wasn’t really getting people on their feet again, so why rush
through more trials or exemptions? The other issue was that, unlike today, the
agency hadn’t yet established the programmes that help guide companies
through the reams of paperwork necessary to gather the safety and efficacy
data needed for approval. They just sort of let them figure it out for
themselves. Some did; others didn’t.
All the while, Cyberkinetics continued to bank on the exemption, which
kept being promised and delayed, month after month. ‘Back then the FDA
was taking a long time to make decisions,’ says Cavuoto. Even as they passed
over patient reports, they attended to reams of other evidence in their
deliberations. That probably included Robinson’s review. But it would have
certainly included the public statements made by a world-famous neuro-
scientist in a leading newspaper. In 2007, Miguel Nicolelis at Duke was
interviewed about the OFS by the Boston Globe.31 ‘I have nothing good to say
about this company,’ he fumed. ‘I see no solid science behind their latest
attempt to make some quick revenue or save their stock price from collapsing
altogether.’
What did Miguel Nicolelis know about physiological fields? Not much, it
turns out. ‘This had nothing to do with Andara,’ says Cavuoto – it had
everything to do with John Donoghue, one of the founders of Cyberkinetics.
Nicolelis hated Donoghue, he recalls. Both men were pioneers in brain–
computer interfaces, but Donoghue was the media darling who got column
inches in The New York Times, and Nicolelis wasn’t – to his ever-present
consternation. ‘Nicolelis didn’t know anything at all about the technology – all
he knew was that it was John Donoghue’s company.’
After the disparaging comments were published, Cavuoto wrote a pleading
editorial – aimed at the FDA – begging them to not listen to Nicolelis. But it
was too late. Cavuoto reckons the article helped sink the company. ‘The FDA
took their sweet time and [Cyberkinetics] ran out of money, and the investors
pulled the plug. And that was that,’ he says. Then the 2008 recession hit.
Cyberkinetics, Andara, the humanitarian device exemption – all of it was gone.
Fifteen years later, Cavuoto is still sore about the way things went down,
and not just about the device itself. ‘When you forced Cyberkinetics out of
business – which is what the FDA did – you were sending a clear message to
the research and the investment community,’ he says: work on this, and your
career will go nowhere. The effects of its actions, he claims, ripple out more
than a decade later. ‘In my opinion, it set the field back maybe ten years.’
Meanwhile, the Purdue lab could never get their phase two trial properly
underway. ‘The FDA wouldn’t allow it to start,’ says Bohnert. ‘They just kept
requesting more information. We never knew why. I just remember saying to
Richard, “Who did you piss off ?”’ But true to his personality, after all the
setbacks, Borgens still refused to give up. He tried several times to reassemble
what pieces they had left. Both he and Shapiro – and later, his post-doc
Jianming Li – applied herculean efforts to get the OFS restarted or at least
prevent it from being memory-holed. In 2012, Shapiro wrote a post-review
publication to report the OFS results that included the four additional
participants he had managed to cobble together. He published another similar
review in a European journal in 2014, part of the same effort to keep the work
academically relevant.32
But eventually, it was too much even for Borgens. ‘The FDA just buried
him under so much paperwork that he eventually gave up,’ says Ann Rajnicek,
who earned her PhD under Robinson but cut ties after the events. ‘He used to
put his hand up, you know, extend his arm as high as it can go, and he would
say, “I have filled in paperwork, literally this high for the FDA, to try and make
this happen.” And he said, “I just don’t have the will for it any more.”’
Li, now a research professor, tried to take over. He modernised the OFS
electronics and tinkered with optimising the electrode placements. The
technological advances since 2001 offered staggering upgrades: the ability to
alter device settings, new algorithms, control of the device using an app. But
Borgens had already turned his focus away from technology and towards
drugs that could fuse neurons.33
Then, in 2018, he was diagnosed with prostate cancer. That’s when Purdue
cleaned house. Bohnert says one of the deans forced Borgens to retire. The
others in the department who weren’t laid off either retired early or left.
Borgens passed away at the end of 2019.
Even then, Li tried to carry on his old mentor’s work, trying to keep both
the Center for Paralysis research and the OFS alive.34 As the original patent
had expired, Li put together a new patent filing and published some of the
advancements.35 A tentative collaboration with Case Western was on the verge
of testing the new version of OFS in humans. Then Covid struck.
In the chaos, Li was let go and replaced with a new director, who changed
the mission of the centre in a direction that ruled out further work on the
OFS. ‘It was just so sad,’ says Bohnert. ‘He had a way of helping people and
they wouldn’t let it go forward.’ Shapiro retired from Indiana University in
2021. All that remains of Richard Borgens at Purdue is an office door painted
to look like the Texas flag.
Decades later, Andara is a whole garden of what-ifs. Would it have
succeeded if it hadn’t gotten caught up with Cyberkinetics, hadn’t been
torpedoed by the recession? Was Robinson right that Borgens skipped crucial
steps at the beginning that would have made themselves known in later trials?
Or was the idea simply too far ahead of its time?
Here was a device that worked on neurons but had nothing to do with
familiar concepts like the neural code and action potentials. It was a wound-
healing mechanism that used electricity, which didn’t make any sense to
biologists and dredged up old memories of electroquackery. ‘The stuff he was
doing was truly on the cutting edge,’ says Richard Nuccitelli. ‘Trying to
regenerate the spinal cord – guide new growth – that’s something standard
electrophysiologists know nothing about. They aren’t interested in that –
they’re only interested in action potentials’.
Today, spinal stimulation is back in the news.36 But it’s action potentials
that are the focus of these new research efforts, which approach spinal
connectivity from the traditional perspective. Instead of trying to reconnect
severed axons, they apply intense bursts of electricity to any remaining intact
axons in the spinal cord to force them to carry action potentials that drive
motor function. These few remaining undamaged pathways, it turns out, can
exhibit plasticity like that most commonly associated with the brain. This – not
fusing broken neurons back together – seems to be how electricity is popularly
used in spinal injury research now. And there is some success. A handful of
people are walking who, before technological intervention, did not. ‘Maybe if
Andara had been approved, that all would have happened years earlier with
more different approaches,’ Cavuoto grouses. ‘Maybe more people would be
walking around now after spinal injuries.’
Was Borgens’ device actually working on these principles? Or are today’s
devices actually working because some of the fields are reconnecting axons the
way the OFS did? The problem is that there are not a lot of ways to test
whether Borgens was right about exactly what mechanism restored movement
to Brandon Ingram. As with dogs, you can’t cut people open to check.
However, the work emerging from other areas of bioelectricity in the body
could settle that question soon. That’s because it’s becoming clear that Richard
Borgens was tapping into bioelectric properties of cells that are just now
beginning to be understood in full. When it comes to the physiological fields
Borgens’ OFS was recruiting, those were absolutely real, and they are not
unique to cells in the spine. The same electrical properties are common to
every living cell in your body. Borgens might have recruited them in a way that
put the cart before the horse, but it is clear that he was tapping into something
elemental. And as this research finally begins to mature, it is cohering into
theories of how physiological electric fields mend the body whichever way it
breaks down, and how to create new devices to help it do better.
Borgens’ work continues to be replicated in small trials, most recently by a
Slovakian group in 2018, which recreated the OFS precisely. They tested it in
rats, and with the improved imaging and analysis available more than thirty
years after Borgens did his own rat trials, were able to see exactly what the
OFS was doing. Under the guidance of the electric field, the mangled axons
successfully clasped on to their estranged partners over the injury site. We may
not have seen the last of the oscillating field stimulator.
Borgens’ instincts, it turns out, had been right.
It was becoming clear that you could disrupt the body’s natural repair
processes just by interfering with the electrics. Researchers in Scotland found
that if they used channel-blocking drugs to inhibit sodium ions, thereby
interrupting the electrical signals sent by the wound current in rats, their
wounds took longer to heal.42
But was the opposite true? Could we also speed up the healing process by
amplifying our body’s natural electricity? A spate of clinical trials over the past
decade increasingly suggests that the answer is yes. Perhaps the most
harrowing kinds of wounds are severe bed sores, which can take months to
years to heal (if they heal at all) and attack tissue, muscle, and bone deep
beneath the skin. Most of the research using electrical stimulation to heal
wounds in humans has been done in these kinds of wounds – like deep brain
stimulation, this is a method of last resort when nothing else seems to help.
After many years of these kinds of experiments, two groups of scientists
conducted meta-analyses and concluded that amplifying the natural wound
current with electrical stimulation almost doubles their healing rate.
Nor was the effect limited to skin; since the 1980s, a growing body of
evidence hinted that the same kind of small electrical currents could accelerate
healing in bone fractures, and some suggested it might even help treat
osteoporosis.43 It helps new blood vessels grow into wounds faster and is also
beginning to be seriously examined for eyes. Electrical stimulation has even
been shown to be effective at aiding skin transplants – it seems to help the
new skin take.
There’s a catch: the results of all these types of experiments have been
broadly positive – but also inconsistent and unpredictable. ‘The problem here
is that it’s not optimised,’ says Mark Messerli, who is working on bioelectric
wound dressings at the University of North Dakota. Because we don’t
understand the mechanism by which the electricity is speeding the healing of
the wound, we can’t do anything targeted to enhance or improve – or even
standardise – the stimulation. And that makes things hard for any doctors
hoping to use electrical stimulation on their patients. ‘To optimise wound
healing, we need to understand how it works.’
Min Zhao was able to vastly advance this understanding in 2006, when he
and the geneticist Josef Penninger undertook the first-ever controlled
experiment aimed at pinning down some of the genes that get switched on by
electric fields on wounds.44 This work was reported widely in the news – it
finally brought electricity into the legible zone of genes. This was some of the
earliest, most robust and tantalising evidence for the epigenetic power of the
electrome.
The next thing to do was find a way to measure the actual electrical field of
human wounds. Existing electrotherapy devices apply current with no insight
as to what effect it has on a person’s own bioelectricity. To change that, you
need a device that can help identify whether a person has an abnormal or
malfunctioning wound current. No tool had ever been capable of measuring
the electric field in the air next to dry mammalian skin – it had always been
done on wet frog skin in the controlled conditions of a lab. In 2011, Richard
Nuccitelli created a non-invasive device that could deal with human skin,
allowing our injury currents to be closely observed. The Dermacorder could
sense whatever the nearest voltage is. Hold it up to the skin and it maps the
voltage on its surface and correlates it with the depth of the wound.45 That
gives you a topographical, three-dimensional electrical map of the wound.
‘This was the first tool a physician could actually hold and use on a person,’
says Rajnicek.
It vastly deepened the understanding of how electricity works in wound
healing. Nuccitelli found a strong correlation between the magnitude of the
wound’s electric field and the progression of the healing – it peaks at injury,
slowly decreases as the wound heals, and returns to undetectable when healing
is complete. More interesting, though, was the relationship between the
strength of a person’s wound current and their ability to heal. People with
weak injury current healed more slowly than those whose injury current was
‘louder’. Most interesting of all: wound current strength wanes with age,
emitting a signal that is only half as strong in people over the age of sixty-five
as it is in those under twenty-five.46
With better measurements came better experimental results. In 2015,
Nuccitelli and Christine Pullar applied electrical stimulation to wounds and,
mapping it with the Dermacorder, were able to coax new blood vessels to
form, accelerating the healing in all their patients.
Electric healing
In the beginning:
The electricity that builds and rebuilds
you
Schrödinger’s finger
F or the past decade, Michael Levin’s conference talks and papers have
included a finely detailed line drawing of a little white mouse sitting up
on its hind legs. The expression on its face can only be described as a Mona
Lisa smile.1 Another source of ambiguity is its left foreleg, which is encased in
a small box. The paw in the box may have five fingers, or it might have four.
There are several actual mice in Levin’s lab at Tufts University, and they’re
each wearing one of the little boxes. All have had a single finger amputated.
The box is called a bioreactor, and it is placed on the stump after amputation,
along with something patented to manipulate the electrical communications in
the remaining tissue. It’s possible that one of the boxes contains a once-again
complete set of five fingers. The results are not yet in but this Schrödinger’s
appendage could change the future of an entire scientific field.
‘Regenerative medicine’ is an umbrella term that was only invented about
thirty years ago to cover the wide variety of ways people have tried to replace
what has been lost to trauma or age.2 The discipline was stitched together, a
bit like Frankenstein’s monster, from a disparate collection of other sub-
disciplines that included implant and transplant medicine, prosthetics, and
tissue engineering. What united them all into a coherent framework was the
discovery, and the galvanising promise, of stem cells.
The reason you’re always hearing about stem cells is because of their
unique ability to turn into many other kinds of cells. They’re a bit like kids:
initially infinitely malleable, but as they mature and grow into their eventual
vocation, they specialise into specific adult roles like muscle or nerve or bone.
When you were a three-to-five-day-old blastocyst, you were all stem cells
(about 150 of them, in fact). By the time you’re an adult, you don’t have many
of them left, and what few you have are mostly generated in your bone
marrow.
When, in 1998, it became possible to derive these magic materials from
human embryos and transform them into any other cell in a lab, it was
suddenly conceivable that we could use them to repair or replace any organ or
body part, instead of what we had done before: swapping in metal or plastic,
or a donated organ that required suppressing the immune system. Whether
they were old, damaged or diseased, soon stem cells would rejuvenate livers,
joints, hearts, kidneys, eyes and anything else you could want.3
Amid controversies (people didn’t like the idea of using foetal tissue as
building blocks for medicine) and new hope (it turned out other cells in the
adult body could be recruited for similar purposes!) the headlines never
stopped. Stem cells would cure neurological disorders. They’d cure lower back
pain. Heck, they’d cure anything. They were biological miracles.
Despite thirty years of dramatic headlines, however, most of these goals
remain perpetually out of reach. ‘There’s no injury, no disease, no anything,
where stem cell therapy is better than the other things we’re doing, after all
these years,’ says Stephen Badylak, who runs the McGowan Institute for
Regenerative Medicine in Pittsburgh. So Levin is trying something completely
different. Instead of trying to micromanage the wildly complicated universe of
molecular and chemical interactions involved in building an appendage out of
individual cells, he thinks it’s possible to instead turn on the bioelectric
switches that shaped that mouse (and all its fingers) in the first place. He’s
banking on the idea that the ability to regrow anything you’ve lost to injury or
illness is not written in your genes, but can be controlled by the electrical
language the body uses to talk to itself about what shape it is. Figure out that
code, and you can just get nature to build you a new one. The first hints of
these electric switches’ existence go back nearly a century, long before we
knew what to do with them.
We used to think that only some animals could regenerate themselves: hydras,
salamanders, crabs . . . nothing as interesting as a mammal. But in the
twentieth century, the formal study of regeneration revealed just how
widespread the phenomenon actually is in the animal kingdom.
In nature, there seems to be no theoretical limit to what you can cut off
and expect to get back, if you find the right animal: hydras – tiny freshwater
organisms – can be cut to absolute ribbons, and the little shred will rebuild
itself again into a fully functioning animal. The same is true of that freshwater
flatworm we met before, called the planarian.
In fact, this is how they reproduce – they tear themselves in half (you
thought you had problems).36 If you had this capability, someone could throw
a segment of your finger into the sea and a week later, it would have grown
into an extra you. You can see for yourself, in fact: chop a hydra in half and the
tail end will sprout a new head and the head end will sprout a new tail.
Sea stars combine the abilities of hydras and planarians. In addition to
being able to regenerate a new body from a severed arm, some species can
regrow their entire central nervous system from scratch. They’ve been known
to tear themselves in half on purpose to start a family,37 and they’ve also been
known to use their own severed leg to beat off their enemies.
Then there are salamanders, which can regenerate a remarkable number of
their tissues and organs, including their limbs, tails, jaws, spinal cords, and
hearts. A frilly red version called an axolotl can heal anything on its body
without scarring, including its brain. Frogs can regenerate entire limbs and tails
(and even eyes) when they are tadpoles, but they lose this ability after their
metamorphosis into a frog.
Same goes for humans – at least until you exit the womb. To riff off a
famous phrase often attributed to Abraham Lincoln: we can regenerate all our
tissues some of the time, and some of our tissues all of the time, but we can’t
regenerate all our tissues all the time. Our regenerative ability follows a
schedule that is strictly dependent on age and body part.
A zygote is the regenerative equivalent of a planarian. Someone could slice
it in two and the two cells would continue developing into identical twins.38
That ability falls off quickly, but a foetus has impressive regenerative ability
even so. Most foetal injuries don’t leave scars, an insight obtained in the late
1980s when foetal surgery became routine.39 After birth, however, the
superpower disappears fast, with one exception. Until between the ages of
seven and eleven (for obvious reasons there hasn’t been a lot of experimental
evidence to pin this down exactly), if you lose the tip of your finger, you’ll
probably regenerate it in full.
This phenomenon is not extensively documented in the scientific literature
– and not for the pinky-decapitating reasons you might think. Ai-Sun Tseng, a
professor at the University of Las Vegas who leads a lab that specialises in
regeneration, recalls describing her work to a class. One of her students ‘totally
lit up. He was like, “Yeah! Look at my fingers!” He grew up in the Philipines
and at some point he’d had four of his fingers chopped off above the knuckle,’
she says. Because he was not yet eleven when it happened, they all grew back
perfectly. But his age wasn’t the only factor. His family had been too poor to
afford a doctor, so they kept the wounds wrapped and wet and clean – and
eventually all four fingers regenerated perfectly, nails and all. By the time Tseng
inspected them decades later, they were indistinguishable from fingers that had
never been maimed. At a conference a few years later, Tseng recounted the
story to a group of colleagues, one of whom was a paediatric surgeon. He
pointed out that, faced with a similar situation, most parents actually refuse to
take advantage of this last vestige of regenerative ability. ‘They’re way too
scared of leaving an open wound,’ he told her. ‘They worry it’ll get infected.’
So they ask the surgeon to suture together the surrounding skin, which
protects the wound with fibrous scar tissue that forecloses any hope of the
finger regenerating according to its potential. ‘Part of the reason we know
about childhood regeneration at all is because of children in developing or
poorer countries without healthcare,’ she recalls him telling her.
Our regeneration schedule is age-dependent – but it also depends on the
body part. The liver renews about once every two months. Your intestinal
lining sheds completely and renews again every seven days; what you eat next
Saturday will be processed by a completely different set of cells than the ones
working on today’s breakfast.40 A small population of stem cells in your lungs
regularly undergo cell division. Even the lens of your eye regenerates. As you
age, however, all these tissues lose their ability to rise from the dead,
exemplified by the skin, whose outer layer renews every fourteen days while
you’re in your teens, but slows to twenty-eight or even forty-two days by late
midlife. And of course, most of our tissue doesn’t do it at all. Cut off a nose or
a hand and they stay gone.
But why, when we obviously carry the genetic instructions for
regenerating? Why can children regrow a fingertip but not a nose? Over the
past couple of decades, the consensus across multiple disciplines has become
that this latent ability actually lurks in every animal – and with it, our ability to
regrow lost limbs or regenerate other organs. But how to unlock it? Once
again, we turn to electricity.
Lionel Jaffe had found big differences between the electrical currents emitted
by animals that regenerate limbs and the ones that just put a scar on a wound
and call it a day.41 In the early 2000s, Betty Sisken at the University of
Kentucky painstakingly copied the exact qualities of the electrical fields that
had been observed in regenerating animals and inscribed these onto the tissues
of animals that don’t regenerate. After amputation, a range of her animals –
including amphibians, chick embryos, and rats – began to form limb buds.
These had complex tissues like cartilage, vasculature, all the things you need
for a functioning limb.42 But alas, not an actual functional limb. Then Ai-Sun
Tseng, who at the time was part of Levin’s lab, manipulated the membrane
voltages with ion-channel tweaks, and now we were cooking with gas.
She and Levin had been kicking around an idea – instead of
micromanaging the process of regeneration, could it be possible to tweak the
bioelectrics to kickstart the development processes that had built these
appendages in the first place? Tseng started looking for ion channels that
might be amenable to tweaking. She discovered one kind of sodium channel
that was crucial to regeneration. Better yet, an ion-channel drug had already
been developed that could act on these. Called Monensin, it was able to ferry
extra sodium into a cell. Tseng had a hunch that flooding the cell with sodium
– mimicking the electrical differences Jaffe had identified all those years ago –
might restart regeneration in an animal that normally doesn’t: a tadpole. Not
only did it work, it worked shockingly quickly. Soaking for a single hour in the
sodium channel drug bath drove eight days’ worth of tail regeneration. When
she told Levin about it, even he was sceptical. An hour seemed so short. But
Tseng had been right. That little soak was enough to give the cells the idea, he
says: ‘Grow back whatever belongs there.’43
This was literally what he had envisioned being possible with the
bioelectric code. Tseng had shown that all the persnickety chemical gradients,
transcriptional networks, and force cues needed to orchestrate individual cells
into complicated tissues could be harnessed with a comparatively simple set of
electrical instructions. The genes were hardware, and they could be controlled
by manipulating ion flows – the instructions from the software. Tseng and
Levin soon published the seminal paper introducing their new idea: ‘Cracking
the bioelectric code’.44
Subsequent research has yielded multi-limbed frogs and other evidence of
bioelectricity’s role in regeneration. Among the most startling of these, it was
possible to use bioelectric interventions to make planarians that had been
chopped in half grow a second head instead of a tail. And as the press loves
nothing more than a mutant, all the resulting media attention translated to
money. First, DARPA came calling with enough money to build the little
regenerative boxes that are now on the mice in Levin’s lab. They’ve extended
out to frogs, too, growing a new leg on an adult frog. The new leg wasn’t
perfect, but it worked – the frog used it to swim around, and after a few
months it even regrew toes. In 2016, Microsoft billionaire Paul Allen added
nearly $10 million to Levin’s coffers.
The open question now: when will it jump to humans?
At the end:
The electricity that breaks you back
down
I n the late 1940s, the zoologist Sylvan Meryl Rose toiled in his lab at Smith
College, creating cancer chimeras. He would cultivate rapidly growing
kidney tumours in frogs, excise them from their hosts, and then carefully graft
the growths onto the legs of salamanders, tucking them just under the skin.
(As we learned in the prior chapter, except for some brief periods during
development, frogs can’t regenerate, but salamanders can regrow whole limbs.)
After the tumour transplant, the poor salamanders usually died of the resultant
malignancies, with one exception: if Rose cut off the leg onto which he had
grafted the tumour, precisely bisecting the implanted tumour, the animal
regrew its leg, every time. The regenerating limb bud conscripted what was left
of the tumour, transforming its cancerous cells into the normal cellular
building blocks of biological tissue.1 The regenerating leg essentially absorbed
the cancer.
His experiment was among the first to identify the strange link between
regeneration and cancer, but it was not the last.2 Among the strangest of these
is the discovery of the naked mole rat’s trifecta of superpowers: not only does
this rodent rarely get cancer, it seems to heal without scarring3 – and it defies
known biological laws of aging.4 These animals can live up to thirty years in
captivity (a standard non-naked, non-mole rat clocks in at about a year). Naked
mole rats had been known for a long time to be almost totally impervious to
tumours, but in 2018 it emerged that they don’t die of old age the way other
mammals do. Emerging evidence also suggests that they can heal better than
other mammals.
This odd story is one of many strange links between healing wounds,
regeneration, and cancer. We have known since before Jaffe and Borgens that
differences in bioelectric signalling are a critical component of both wound
healing and limb regeneration – but instead of creating more of what we
needed, what if it could also create more of what we don’t? It would be a long
time, however, before the proper study of the complicated relationship
between electricity and cancer could be undertaken. The first scientists who
tried to investigate the electricity of cancer faced an uphill journey, thanks to
the a long parade of Victorian con artists who had poisoned the well with their
electrical cancer cures.
Around the time Sylvan Rose was chopping off salamander legs, Harold
Saxton Burr and his colleagues got a visit from Louis Langman, an obstetrician
at Bellevue Hospital in Manhattan. Langman hoped Burr’s electrical ovulation-
detecting technique might help bolster his success rates at artificial
insemination, as one has to be ovulating for the procedure to work.5 Burr –
having just emerged from his bruising fight with Catholic doctor John Rock
over the electrical signals in ovulation – was happy to help and instructed
Langman on proper use of the device. It turned out well; electrometric
measures improved the rate at which Langman was able to help women
conceive. But it soon became clear that this wasn’t the sole reason he had
approached Burr. What he really wanted to know was if this technique might
also help him identify cancers in his clients’ reproductive systems.
Burr was in. He enthusiastically sent one of his contraptions back with
Langman to his wards, where, in an initial group of 100 women, he strapped
one electrode to the lower abdomen above the pubis, and the other either on
or alongside the cervix.6 Women whose troubles turned out to be caused by
ovarian cysts or other non-cancerous medical issues almost always had a
positive reading. Women with malignant tumours, however, showed an
electrical ‘marked negativity’ of the cervical region every time.7 Langman
confirmed their diagnosis with a pathological examination. Cancerous tissues,
it appeared, emitted an unmistakable electrical signature.
Langman repeated the technique in about a thousand women to see
whether his results stood up. They did: 102 of his patients exhibited the
characteristic voltage reversals. When Langman operated on them, he
confirmed that 95 of the 102 had cancer.8 Even more remarkably, often the
masses had not even progressed to the point where the symptoms would have
driven them to visit the doctor, never mind obtain a correct diagnosis. After
removing these cancers, the electrical polarity shown on the electrometer
would normally flip back to a ‘healthy’ positive indicator – but it did not
always. When it stayed negative, Burr and Langman suspected that this
indicated that they either hadn’t got it all, or the cells had metastasised.
Somewhere in the body, a cancerous mass was still sending its nefarious
signals.
What struck them as especially strange was that the electrode inside the
genital tract did not have to be placed directly on, or even particularly near to,
the malignant tissue for the anomaly to be detectable. It was like a distress
signal was being sent over distances through the body’s healthy tissue.
It’s hard to evaluate any of these experiments nearly eighty years after the
fact. But to all appearances, it sure seems like a potentially reliable, non-
surgical way to detect malignancy was discovered in the 1940s – and then got
memory-holed. Langman and Burr were happy to acknowledge that ‘the
method employed in this study is obviously an adjunct to other diagnostic
procedures, and in no sense should be considered as a substitute for them’.9
However, it was something – and they wrote rather plaintively that they hoped
others would refine this fledgling technique to aid early diagnosis. In his
memoir, published twenty-five years later, Burr noted with evident
disappointment that no one ever followed up their literature or did any
replications.
In hindsight, it’s quite easy to see why. No one had any idea what could
possibly account for a voltage difference in cancerous tissue. Langman and
Burr’s findings were poorly understood, and like most bioelectrical phenomena
outside neuroscience, were ignored. And then, of course, four short years later,
studies of electrical signals in biology became moot with James Watson and
Francis Crick’s announcement that they had discovered the double helix
structure of DNA. Oncology began to reorganise itself around genes. Not
long after DNA was determined to be the sole arbiter of inheritance, it
became canon that anything that damaged DNA and caused mutations in it
may also cause cancer. In the 1970s and 1980s, a vigorous search for the
abnormal genes ensued.10 It was not a good time to go against the grain of
science.
In the 1940s, while Langman and Burr had been investigating the
electrometric cancer diagnostic techniques, Björn Nordenström was furrowing
his eyebrows, puzzling over the subtle anomalies he kept finding in the X-rays
of his lung and breast cancer patients. As a diagnostic radiologist at the
Karolinska Institute in Stockholm, he had used X-ray imaging to inspect the
blood vessels inside lung cancer tissues. It was during these examinations that
he started to wonder about the persistent and puzzling irregularities that kept
showing up in his images.11
The images in the X-rays looked like spiky flares around the tumours and
lesions.12 Colleagues dismissed these as artifacts of the imaging method, but
that explanation didn’t satisfy Nordenström. By 1983, he had devised a theory.
Like Burr and Langman, Nordenström had found mysterious electrical
differences between normal tissue and tumours, and concluded that these were
the result of differences in the way ions flowed around them, thereby the
source of the flares he had found, which he named ‘corona structures’. He
believed that both coronas and the flow of ions that caused them were part of
a body-wide electrical circulatory system that existed alongside our traditional
vasculature, like an additional blood stream. This system transported the ions
in our ‘conductive media and cables’ (including the blood) like little weather
systems in coherent circuits around the body. Our electrical circulatory system
was not only as complex as the circulation of the blood, but also similarly
implicated in all the body’s other physiological activities. But because it was
invisible, until now we had missed it.
Controversially, instead of publishing this hypothesis as a series of small
articles in highly ranked journals, which is the way scientific theories are
typically disseminated, Nordenström decided to skip all that. In 1983, he self-
published the whole thing in the form of a book – an oversized, 358-page
colossus which he named Biologically Closed Electric Circuits: Clinical, Experimental
and Theoretical Evidence for an Additional Circulatory System.13 No publisher would
touch it.
Some researchers would, though. The book’s introduction sported not one
but three forewords – and a preface – from four scientists willing to stake their
reputation on this unusual idea. ‘I feel little need for lengthy considerations of
its scientific merit,’ harrumphed Universite d’Aix-Marseille biochemist Jacques
Hauton with unsatirisable French bof-ery. ‘Its full importance is today
impossible to appreciate,’ he continued, representing ‘no less than a major
point in the evolution of our understanding of biologic science.’14 The other
contributors were similarly dazzled.
It had only been seven years since the first ion channel had been observed
ferrying sodium in and out of cellular nightclubs. In the eyes of science, that’s
a millisecond, so the idea of bioelectricity in cancer was inconceivable. ‘The
theory sounds flawed,’ a National Cancer Institute deputy director, Gregory
Curt, told the Los Angeles Times in 1986. ‘Based on what we know about cancer
biology, there is no evidence that changing electrical fields have any impact on
a tumor.’15
Nordenström, however, had already started treating patients using the
principles of his biologically closed electrical circuits to (he claimed) interrupt
the electrical signals that promoted cancer. He would place a positively charged
electrode needle into the tumour and another negatively charged one into
healthy tissue, and send ten volts of direct current through the tissue for
several hours. This was repeated until the tumour began to shrink.
Nordenström told the Los Angeles Times that the patients on whom he was
experimenting ‘have been rejected by surgeons and other physicians as having
cancer too advanced to be treated’.16 Between 1978 and 1981, he treated
twenty such hopeless cases. Thirteen died despite his interventions. But
Nordenström insisted that many of the tumours shrank and even disappeared.
A short description of those first twenty cases appeared in 1984 in the Journal
of Bioelectricity.17 He insisted that he had been too busy to publish detailed
accounts in mainstream journals – and, in a move that was sure to make him
no friends, told the Los Angeles Times that what he was doing was too complex
for many of his colleagues to comprehend. ‘People say it’s controversial
because it’s another way to say they don’t understand.’
Here, surely, was the pseudoscience trifecta: a theory completely out of
step with current scientific thinking; a standoffish refusal to publish in
appropriate venues; and insistence on treatment being administered before the
method has been properly validated. Nordenström was showing all the
hallmarks of being a quack. And yet! Very smart researchers just couldn’t
agree. ‘It doesn’t follow the usual medical logic but it fits in with a number of
scientific facts in many disciplines,’ Morton Glickman told the Los Angeles
Times.18 It had taken Glickman, a radiology professor at Yale School of
Medicine, a full year to inch his way through the migraine-inducing
explanations of the biologically closed electrical circuits. He had come out the
other side a believer. ‘My feeling is there is a very good chance that it will [turn
out to be] true,’ he said.
While Western science held its nose, Nordenström’s mentions of unseen
forces travelling around the body piqued considerable interest in the People’s
Republic of China. In 1987, he was invited to Beijing to demonstrate his
technique at the Ministry of Public Health.19 There’s not much information
about that meeting, but afterwards the ministry wasted no time in mounting an
aggressive education campaign to get Nordenström’s technique taught at
hospitals. Between 1988 and 1993, forty-two courses were convened to teach
Nordenström’s methods, attended by 1,336 doctors from 969 hospitals.20 By
1993, they had treated nearly 5,000 patients. By 2012, the technique had
treated over 10,000 malignant and benign tumours.21
The media was sceptical, if not hopeful. On 21 October 1988, the highly
regarded TV news programme 20/20 ran a segment about a surprising new
approach to cancer.22 ‘Reporting on exciting medical breakthroughs is a role
20/20 has carried out for years,’ host Barbara Walters prefaced, before
describing the vetting process the show goes through ‘to make sure we don’t
give credence to frauds’. It was an unusual bit of deflection from an otherwise
impeccably confident anchor. She then went on to introduce ‘a story that
sounds almost like science fiction. It involves the theory that electricity plays a
very major role in the human body. It could revolutionise medical science. It
might even provide a new way of treating cancer.’
So what do we make of this? As Walters’ unease reveals, it can be really
difficult in the moment to distinguish the quack from the revolutionary, but
clarity tends to set in within a couple of decades. Not so with Nordenström.
He disappeared. He may have moved to China to continue his work, and by
one arcane account he died in 2006.23 Several of the researchers who staked
their careers on his claims have also passed away. Most people have forgotten
him . . . but only most. Some of the researchers I interviewed have quietly
squirrelled away copies of his book, which are a rare find. They were kind
enough to send me photocopies of certain sections, because like Glickman
they believe – off the record – he will be vindicated.
Whatever you make of his theory – if you can understand it – there were
some basics we didn’t comprehend then which have since been put on solid
foundations. One of these is that ion channels are present in all cells. Their
activity determines the membrane voltage of the cell and the tissue, and
thereby the behaviour of these cells and tissues. Even cancer.
Mustafa Djamgoz was in the middle of his third pint down at the pub when he
got the overwhelming urge to put a cancer cell into a patch clamp. He had
never heard of either Burr or Nordenström. He wasn’t even a cancer
researcher (yet) – at the time, he was a neurobiologist at Imperial College
London. It was the early 1990s and he had met up with some of his old
colleagues one evening after a conference to knock a couple back.
They were pondering cancer’s electrical behaviour and getting nowhere
when Djamgoz, who had spent his life studying ion channels, had his eureka
moment. ‘Suddenly this big penny dropped in my mind: “My God, no one has
looked at electrical signals in cancer cells!”’ He asked his friends for some cells
and got to work. Djamgoz didn’t know it at the time, but he was about to
embark on the most complicated and frustrating seven years of his career.
Good thing, then, that he was no stranger to complexity and frustration.
Djamgoz grew up in Cyprus, whose Greek and Turkish residents have long
been engaged in various territorial feuds. The island seethed under British
colonial rule from 1878 to 1960, so when Djamgoz was born, every corner of
his neighbourhood was still festooned with characteristic British red telephone
and post boxes. Throughout his childhood he dreamed of attending Imperial
College, and as a teenager taught himself to build a radio transmitter from
scratch. He would shock himself up to fifty times a day in the process – and
not always by accident, as he was quickly developing a fascination with the way
electricity is interpreted by human biology. This unusual child was soon lured
from sunny Cyprus by a scholarship to study physics at a soggy boarding
school in Kent, a springboard to Imperial. The university had an established
reputation in visual psychophysics, the branch of vision research that probes
the way animals transform physical stimuli like photons into our subjective
sensory experience of the world, like the colour blue. Djamgoz built an
electrophysiology lab for his mentor from scratch, down to the amplifiers, and
in return received his doctorate.
He spent the next two decades studying the electrophysiological response
of the retina. ‘The retina is a beautiful model of the central nervous system,’ he
says. ‘You peel it out of the eye, you put electrodes in, you flash lights and you
can see all those individual cells responding.’ He still remembers the first time
he stuck an electrode into a retinal cell and flashed a red light at it. The cell
obliged immediately, flopping down into a state of flabby depolarisation, its
inner voltage becoming the same as its surrounding milieu, letting ions drift in
and out of the cell as they please. Then he flashed it some blue, and the cell
responded in the opposite direction, hyperpolarising – re-establishing the big
electrical difference between its inside and outside, which meant the ion
movements were once again tightly controlled. ‘This one cell knew the colour
it was seeing,’ he marvels. ‘And you knew because you could see the potential
on the oscilloscope just go up and down.’
Djamgoz was doing these experiments in the mid-1990s, at the dawn of
scientific investigation into adult synaptic plasticity – the idea that the brain’s
ability to change its connections doesn’t end with childhood, but persists into
later life.24 Indeed, Djamgoz’s work provided evidence for the idea, using the
retina as his model to assemble proof that adult retinal cells can change their
connections and adapt to different conditions. The work earned him a
professorship in neurobiology, and that’s where he likely would have spent the
rest of his career if it hadn’t been for that fateful night at the pub.
Now cancer seized his attention. One of his interlocutors from that
evening gave him a batch of cells from prostate tumours in rats. Back in his
lab, Djamgoz subjected these to the same electrophysiological prodding usually
reserved for the retina. He found them teeming with electrical activity – but
not the kind he was used to seeing in healthy cells.
It had long been known that as healthy cells turn cancerous, they de-
differentiate: this means that they leave behind their prior identities as bone or
skin or muscle cells, and return to a primordial state resembling that of a stem
cell. But unlike stem cells, which often dutifully transform into new identities
and go where they are needed, cancer cells refuse to ‘grow up’. They simply
drift, proliferating and consuming madly, never contributing to the society of
healthy cells around them. This de-differentiation was reflected perfectly in the
electrical activity Djamgoz observed in the cancer cells. The cancer cells had
traded their strongly negative electrical identities (-70mV) for the permanently
depolarised ‘zero’ existence of a perpetual stem cell. (This observation of his
was not unique, but in line with decades of previous observations.)
But that wasn’t the only electrical artifact that grabbed his attention. They
were doing something else too, something far more perplexing. These
depolarised cancer cells were somehow . . . spiking. ‘These were bog standard
action potentials,’ says Djamgoz – but what business did these cells have with
action potentials? These cells came from gut or skin cells, not nerve cells. And
yet, the aggressive cancer cells had somehow gained this ability to spike like a
neuron during their transformation from healthy cells. But the spikes they sent
were not the dependable, decisive spikes of nervous signalling. They were
much more chaotic than that, waving and flickering and exhibiting an
incoherent pattern Djamgoz had previously seen only in epileptic episodes.
What were these weird action potentials doing in cancer cells?
Djamgoz knew they were unmistakably the work of a voltagegated sodium
channel, the same family of sodium channels that lets nerves send action
potentials. No one had ever investigated whether changes in these ion
channels’ behaviour could be related to a cell’s transformation into cancer.
Could these aberrant spiking channels be the reason tumours became
aggressive and metastasised? This was the question Djamgoz asked in his first
paper. He and his colleagues submitted it to Nature, the premier science
journal in the UK. It was rejected instantly by editors who dismissed the
observations as an epiphenomenon. But Djamgoz and his co-authors
eventually managed to present their work at an obscure urinary-tract
conference. It was enough to get their paper published in a minor but
respectable journal.25 It was 1993, and Mustafa Djamgoz was done with
neurobiology. The retina was out. Djamgoz only had eyes for cancer.
The next seven years were spent on what Djamgoz calls a charm offensive:
publishing a barrage of incremental advances, climbing up the ladder of minor
journals to arrive at middling journals, and talking to anyone who would listen
about electrophysiology, bioelectricity, and basic physiology. An ever-
increasing list of diseases were being accounted for by pathological mutations
of the various ion channels – including cystic fibrosis, epilepsy, heart
arrhythmias, and even gastrointestinal diseases – why should cancer be
exempt? ‘Your electricity is what helps you get up and move around,’ he
remembers shouting at his oncology associates. ‘Well, it helps cancer cells get
up and move around too!’ He continued to harangue his colleagues as he
worked to build a careful foundation to understand the precise role these
channels played in metastasis.
The broad consensus on cancer is that it results from abnormal expression
of genes – or at least, a cell’s initial flip from healthy to cancerous is usually
chalked up to genetic defects and mutations. However, that is not what ends
up killing you. It’s widely accepted that most cancer deaths happen when the
cells invade the rest of the body.26 This incursion is facilitated by a repertoire
of basic cell behaviours in which ion channels are known to be crucial:
moving, multiplying, attaching, and more. It is not always possible to look at
the genes in someone’s prostate tumour and conclude from the DNA whether
that tumour will sit there not bothering anyone, or whether it will start
roaming around your body. But Djamgoz and his team were starting to
wonder if maybe there was a clue in the action potentials. Could their spikiness
correlate with the aggressiveness of the cancer? That would be a hugely
valuable diagnostic tool.
By the turn of the century, people stopped dismissing these ideas. Other
researchers had already been making the connection between ion channels and
cancer, notably the Italian pathologist Annarosa Arcangeli, who had spent
decades at the vanguard of linking cancer’s electricity to specific genes.27 At
the University of Florence, she established the cancer-causing relevance of a
gene called hERG, which is already familiar to many biologists in an electrical
context: the ion channel it encodes plays a well-known role in coordinating
your heartbeat by controlling its potassium current.28 Arcangeli and Djamgoz
were careful and talented scientists, and as more researchers began to join their
investigation, overwhelming evidence accumulated that ion channels were key
players in cancer progression.29 Suddenly it wasn’t just an interesting academic
finding or even a new diagnostic: here was a promising avenue for new
treatments.
Ion channel drugs were now a plausible way forward for cancer treatment.
About 20 per cent of drugs on the market target ion channels in some way,
variously blocking them or prying them open.30 If ion channels were turning
out to be important in letting cancer proliferate, could blocking the right one
help stop it? Could one of the existing ion channel drugs hold the key to
stopping these aggressive cancers?
There was just one problem: the property Djamgoz identified as making
cancer more aggressive was controlled by the same voltage-gated sodium
channels in charge of the action potential. You couldn’t block them. Sure, you
might stop a person’s cancer metastasising, but you’d also stop their nervous
system; bad news for their heart and their brain.
This is one of the hardest, most bedevilling problems in cancer treatment:
finding some unique target that exists exclusively in a cancer cell but that will
not also mess up a normal, healthy cell. ‘There’s a huge history in cancer of
people identifying some property of cancer cells,’ Mel Greaves told me. ‘But
when you dig deeper, you often find those properties are not cancer-specific at
all; it is only that cancer cells are exploiting a perfectly normal property.’
Greaves, at the Institute of Cancer Research in London, is cancer-research
royalty – he received a knighthood in 2018 for his investigations into what
triggers leukaemia in children.31 This is the guy journalists call when they want
to know if something is legit in oncology.
But Djamgoz dug deeper. When he did, he found that the guilty cells were
using a special type of ion channel that normally only existed in the cells of
developing foetuses. There, they supercharged cell multiplication and the other
processes needed to quickly form a whole human from nothing. By the time a
baby is born, though, this turbo version should have been shut down, deleted,
and replaced with the normal, ‘adult’ version of the channel, the one that only
does approved activities like sending action potentials.
Djamgoz’s prostate cancer cells were teeming with these pre-birth ion
channels, which he called an ‘embryonic splice variant’. Something had woken
them up again as the previously healthy tissue turned cancerous.
Now that Djamgoz knew how the aggressive splice variant differed from
the normal and life-sustaining regular sodium channel, he had a target whose
removal would not harm normal bodily operations. Over the next few years,
he searched for the same variant in other metastatic cancers, trawling through
biopsies from human cancer patients, and reliably found his splice variants (or
their counterparts) in colon, skin, ovarian, and prostate malignancies.32 This
time, it did not take much convincing to get a grant from Cancer Research UK
to work on an antibody to specifically inhibit these variants.
Mustafa Djamgoz and Annarosa Arcangeli are no longer struggling to get
people to accept their ideas. More than two decades after Djamgoz’s ion
channel was dismissed as a coincidence, the field exploring ion channels and
cancer has exploded.33 Researchers around the world34 are busy digging for
hidden treasure in that big catalogue of existing drugs.35 What’s more, sodium
and potassium channels are no longer the only game in town. People are also
looking at chloride and calcium. The picture that is emerging is of many
different types of channels all working together in complicated synchrony, like
an orchestra, as Djamgoz put it in a 2018 interview. The sodium channel
‘could be the lead violinist, but to be able to create the full symphony we must
understand the other players as well’.36 For example, Arcangeli’s hERG
channel is now an object of great interest among pharmaceutical companies.
At a roundtable of Bioelectricity editors in 2019, she predicted that novel
therapies targeting ion channels would be a future cancer treatment.37
Djamgoz has his own company now, and they were starting to put
together a human clinical trial when – as with so much science – the pandemic
put everything on ice. Neither this nor the fact that he is not a clinical
oncologist have stopped forlorn people from calling him at all hours of the
day. ‘They are desperate,’ he says. People diagnosed with cancer need new
options.
The efficacy of the most common treatments depends on the cancer being
caught early, while it is still a tumour sheltering in place. Once the cancer has
spread its tendrils elsewhere into the body, survival rates start to go down. Mel
Greaves outlined his theory explaining why in the journal BMC Biology in 2018:
successfully destroy a tumour with radiation or chemotherapy, and in theory,
you’ve won. If no cells remain, for the moment, you are cancer-free. However,
should even a single cell survive, then by definition, it is now immune to
anything you threw at the tumour before. That cell is the mother of your
future tumour, and as it proliferates, all its progeny will be equipped with that
same resistance. (The same logic governs drug resistance.38) And there’s
evidence that this new batch of cells will be not only more tenacious, but more
aggressive than the original tumour. ‘We are battling natural selection, one of
the fundamental laws of the universe,’ Charles Swanton, an oncologist at the
Francis Crick Institute in London, told New Scientist.39
To start working out the new battle plan, in 2013 Mel Greaves set up the
Centre for the Study of Evolution in Cancer. He gave a talk at London’s
Science Media Centre, where he floated a new idea to address the resistance
problem: for some advanced cancers – particularly in older patients – instead
of hunting down every last cancer cell in the aim for a cure, perhaps we should
approach it more like a chronic disease. ‘Most cancers hit people when they
are past the age of sixty,’ he explained to me, recounting the talk he gave there.
‘If you treat cancer as a chronic disease, and keep it from becoming aggressive,
you might get ten or twenty more good years.’ This would be a vast
improvement over the mere months some treatments add to people’s lives in
pursuit of curing their late-stage cancer (not to mention the impoverishing
expense, and the toxic medications that more often destroy their quality of life
than their cancer). Not everyone was convinced. ‘I got a lot of hassle for that,’
he recalled in our conversation. An editor from The Times told him that it was
the worst idea he had ever heard. The published responses were similarly
unkind. ‘Let’s stop trying to cure cancer, says cancer professor,’ sneered the
Daily Telegraph.
But time has been on Greaves’s side. A lot of scientists today agree: catch
it early, but if you can’t, ‘control is a much more realistic objective’.
Genomics has revolutionised cancer treatments and has vastly enhanced
our in-depth understanding of cancer. It has yielded powerful new diagnostic
and therapeutic tools that have been incredibly effective in some cases,
including a game-changing treatment for adult leukaemia.
But there is a gulf between these successes and the contention that cancer
is a disease of the genome. ‘Cancer is not purely a disease of the genome, just
like evolution is not just about genes,’ Greaves says. A cell can change many of
its attributes on the fly, in response to its environment, in a way that their
genomes can’t fully account for. ‘So saying it’s all genomics is wrong,’ Greaves
told me.
So the question is: if the electrome affects cancer, what can we do with
that information?
In the decades since Harold Saxton Burr and Louis Langman first suggested
using cancer’s electric properties to detect it, many research efforts have
discovered that you can use bioelectric properties to distinguish cancer cells
from their healthy counterparts, owing to the way they disrupt the flow of
electrical currents through the body. This was an unfamiliar concept when
Burr and Langman were doing their work, but is now widely known as
bioimpedance.40 You might recognise the word from those fancy scales they
have at gyms and spas that measure body composition (though most people
who use them are primarily interested in the precise ratio of body fat to muscle
they reveal). These work on the principle that current can’t travel through fat
cells – fat having a higher ‘impedance’ – but can travel through lean tissue like
muscle. Cancer also has its own bioelectric signature.
When removing a cancerous tumour from any area of the body, the
surgeon’s goal in the operating theatre is to leave nothing behind. But she is
cutting blind, unable to see the difference between cancerous and healthy
tissue. While imaging techniques and other technologies give a location and
map of the mass, when it comes to the actual act of cutting tumour out of
flesh, it’s highly educated guesswork. To raise the odds that the whole mass is
cleanly removed, the surgeon aims to carve out not just the tumour but also a
generous rind of normal tissue around it, often several centimetres’ worth.
After surgery, that carved-out lump of flesh is sent to a pathologist. The
pathologist examines it – specifically, the rim of healthy flesh around the
tumour, which is known as the surgical ‘margin’ – to be sure that margin is
clear of any cancer cells. The problem is that the results can take several days
to come back, and if the analysis finds a positive margin – cancer cells are
present in the rind – it will mean the patient needs a second or third surgery,
and more treatments to augment them.41
Several new technologies, in various stages of clinical trials, aim to help
surgeons get the whole tumour the first time. One promising candidate,
ClearEdge, developed by a start-up in San Francisco, used bioimpedance for
breast cancer margin detection. It integrated the technology into a device
called a ‘margin probe’. This is used by the surgeon while the patient is still
under anaesthesia, after the surgery, to measure the bioelectric properties of
the area around the tumour that has just been removed. A ‘traffic light’
bioimpedance map helps the surgeon see where she missed a spot: red for
cancer, yellow for uncertain, green for clear. It was clinically evaluated in
several hospitals in the UK. In 2016, surgeons at the University of Edinburgh
Medical School and Western General Hospital, in Edinburgh, successfully used
the device to identify cancer in areas around the excision and reported that it
can reduce the need for repeat surgeries.42 It compared favourably to existing,
more time-consuming ways of testing for cancer.
So, where’s ClearEdge? Why haven’t you heard of it? Mike Dixon, one of
the surgeons who trialled the device, told me that while the technology was
easy to use and its results were pretty good, the follow-up studies never
happened. ‘The company was reliant on venture funding,’ he said. ‘The
technology sounds great,’ he says, but so have many other margin probes that
their team has been involved with. Some proved too elaborate, others not
accurate enough, and some just disappeared.
Dany Spencer Adams is working on a way to make an affordable, accurate
version that she says anyone can use, based on the same kind of bioelectric dye
that helped her visualise the ghostly frog face. It tattles on the peculiar
electricity of cancer cells in a different way – by lighting them up according to
their membrane voltage, so cancer cells appear to be a different colour than
healthy cells. They don’t do this in a live, open patient though – they do it with
the mass they’ve already removed, and a very fancy piece of blotting paper.
After excising the tumour, a surgeon will press this special paper to the margin
of the mass to transfer the cells, put the paper in the dye, photograph it, and
upload the results to a computer program. Within ten minutes, you have a heat
map of the entire surgical margin – a paint-by-numbers landscape that tells you
where you missed a spot. If they did, they can go back in while the patient is
still on the table.
That’s the idea, anyway. After testing it in many cells in petri dishes, and
watching the voltage dye cause the cancer cells to light up dramatically, testing
has begun on live tissue with promising results. However, it’s not available yet.
Clinical trials are always expensive, and sometimes investor goals can work
against new devices, when cashing out of your start-up is more important than
making it into a surgeon’s hands. We’re therefore still a way off this new wave
of bioelectric diagnostics making it into the operating theatre, where it could
make surgery for cancer much more effective and reduce recurrence, not to
mention the trauma and infection risk that comes from more surgeries.
Further out, it may become possible to check your cancer’s bioelectric
properties to find out whether you even need surgery to take the tumour out
at all. Remember, genetic defects may have caused the initial cancer, but
whether it grows or goes on walkabout is up to your body’s bioelectrics. Not
all tumours are aggressive – some are slow, and may go away on their own. In
an as yet unpublished study, Djamgoz and his colleagues have collected more
evidence that their sodium channel could in and of itself be a diagnostic
marker for a cancer’s aggression levels.43 When his channels whip up ion
currents, survival rates drop, he told an ion channel modulation symposium in
2019. This could help people make difficult treatment decisions, for example
when evaluating the need for radical and life-altering surgeries and other
treatments. ‘We have never seen metastasis where the channel was not present,’
he told me. Djamgoz’s sodium-channel findings are also opening up some
really unexpected new options for how we might treat the cancer we find.
Communications blackout
To prevent seizures, some people with epilepsy take drugs that close the
sodium channels that spark abnormal action potentials in nerves. This calms
the electrically overactive brain action potentials, making them less likely to
cascade. Such drugs don’t just treat epilepsy symptoms; they have a wide range
of uses, such as for heart arrhythmias and some types of antidepressants.44
A little over a decade ago, anecdotes at clinics and occasional reports to
the FDA began to hint that people who took these sodium-channel-blocking
drugs seemed to have a lower risk of getting some kinds of cancer, and were
more likely to survive if they did get it.45 According to follow-up reviews,
those kinds of epilepsy drugs seemed to be associated with lower incidences of
colorectal cancer, lung cancer, gastric cancer, and blood cancers.46 (To be clear:
these are early signs, not smoking guns. None of this is enough data for
anyone to start taking anti-epileptic drugs if they’re not needed!)
The very preliminary story of these sodium-channel-blocker drugs,
however, happens to fit very neatly into Djamgoz’s theory. Added to this
mosaic, Djamgoz’s research suggests a mechanism for the mystery of how the
sodium channel blockers might be keeping cancer at bay. The erratic action
potentials sent by his splice variant create a way for the tumour cells to
establish contact with each other and the cells in the immediate vicinity. ‘They
are communicating with each other,’ he says. Blocking them would block this
communication.
These trials are all in their earliest days, but if they pan out, there’s good
news: the process to approve these drugs to treat cancer could be very short.
Djamgoz, Huang, and Arcangeli are among many researchers repurposing the
troves of existing ion-channel drugs to keep cancer cells from communicating
and acting on their environment. One big draw of repurposing existing ion-
channel drugs is that you don’t have to start from scratch with drug
development – which can take decades – and that can dramatically accelerate
how soon you see them in the clinic.
If such drugs can remove a cancer’s ability to metastasise, Djamgoz thinks
you can turn the disease into a chronic and manageable condition – exactly in
line with Greaves’s position that cancer should be treated as a chronic illness.
‘We advocate “living with cancer”, rather like we can live chronically with
diabetes and the AIDS virus,’ Djamgoz said in his 2018 interview. ‘Living with
cancer means suppressing metastasis, since this is the main cause of death in
cancer patients.’47
But ion-channel drugs could do even more than that. Some very early
studies have raised the possibility that, just as Sylvan Rose’s regenerating
animals were able to hit the undo button on a growing tumour, messing with
the right bioelectric parameters could help us do the same.
The society of cells
In the last few years, an unambiguous general consensus has emerged that the
solution to the cancer problem likely resides in new theories of cancer. In
1999, Ana Soto and Carlos Sonnenschein at Tufts University School of
Medicine suggested exactly such a novel paradigm: what if we started looking
at cancer not as a breakdown of individual cells but as a breakdown of cellular
society? When individual cells get together, they form tissue, and that tissue is
a kind of society. Proliferation is a cell’s default state, they argued. So cancer is
not sparked by one rogue cell gone wrong so much as a failure of the local
environment to keep the cell’s ‘natural instincts’ in check.
Cancer, in this view, becomes a disorder of organisation in the human
body rather than a defect of individual cells. It was a beguiling metaphor,
especially as it squared so well with the way cancer cells stop contributing to
the body and decide to live on their own radically individualistic terms. Nor
was it as radical a supposition as it first appeared.
A spate of more recent work has begun to more closely examine the
significance of non-genetic factors on cancer’s spread: things like tensile forces
and biomechanics in the micro-environment, and their contribution to a
tumour’s ability to expand and invade its surroundings. In 2013, researchers at
Memorial Sloan Kettering Cancer Center in New York wrote that ‘many
studies have shown that the microenvironment is capable of normalizing
tumor cells’, that re-education of the cells around the tumour, rather than
trying to get rid of them, ‘may be an effective strategy for treating cancer.’48 In
other words, the healthy cells around the tumour are just as important as the
tumour in determining whether the thing can spread. It’s not just the cells
themselves but something in their environment (the society) that is falling
down on the job of regulating their behaviour.
In particular, the evidence has recently started to point to the importance
of the bioelectric signals that cells use to process information. The same kinds
of weak electrical fields that coaxed healthy cells to crawl across a petri dish
have also convinced cells from brain, prostate, and lung tumours to make the
trek.49 Such fields, of course, also exist inside the body – they’re the
consequence of the currents swirling around the cytoplasm and the membrane
voltage of all our cells.
To sum it all up, the interactions between cancer cells and the surrounding
bioelectric fields are increasingly recognised as an overlooked but crucial
aspect of how cells make decisions based on the state of their neighbours. In
this framework, cancer can be viewed as a failure of communication – a fault
in the field of information that coordinates individual cells’ ability to be part of
a normal living system.
So if that’s the case, is it possible to re-establish the communication
protocols? This is fairly unconventional thinking when it comes to cancer, and
yet has a growing number of adherents.50 But as we get a better view of the
varied roles bioelectric signals play in cancer, new possibilities are emerging.
The new suite of tools enabled by a focus on the bioelectricity of cancer could
lead to earlier diagnosis, its transformation into a chronic disease – and maybe
even a way to convince cancer cells to hit the ‘undo’ button.
If you recall, the cell’s membrane voltage is closely related to (and can
determine) its identity, from stem to fat to bone.51 Manipulating this voltage
made many remarkable changes to an organism: like that eye made to grow on
a frog’s butt. Well, it turns out the same factor that could sculpt an eye on a
frog’s butt could also tamp down a cell’s will to become cancerous.
If the body’s ‘societal’ control over its cells is mediated by the signal of
membrane voltage, then a good way to test this bold theory would be to see if,
simply by changing the electrical voltage of a cell, you could cause a healthy
cell to turn cancerous, or convince a cancerous cell to return to a healthy state.
Those were precisely the experiments researchers undertook at Michael
Levin’s lab at Tufts University in 2012. If bioelectric signalling was an
important part of how cells communicate to work on pattern and coherence,
they reasoned, and cancer represented a break in this multicellular contract,
then interfering with cells’ ability to send bioelectric signals should lead to
cancer. After Levin’s doctoral student Maria Lobikin depolarised normal cells,
the depolarised cells began to act malignant.52 It was evidence that
bioelectricity is the informational glue that holds big multicellular structures
together. Membrane voltage was ‘an epigenetic initiator of widespread
metastatic behavior in the absence of a centralized tumor,’ she and her co-
authors wrote.
The next year, another member of Levin’s team, Brook Chernet, went a
step further: could you use membrane voltage alone to predict whether cells
would become cancerous? They tested the hypothesis on frog embryos that
had been laced with human cancer genes to cause them to form tumours.
Using the same fluorescent voltage-reporter dye that Dany Spencer Adams
had used to watch the electrical development of the frog face, they were able
to observe depolarised membrane potential in the tumours. And just as Adams
had been able to predict facial features, the electric signal change just by itself
could predict which cells would turn cancerous.53 This experiment, they wrote,
not only implicated bioelectric signalling in tumour formation, but suggested
new approaches for anti-cancer therapies. That’s because when they
repolarised (and strengthened) the low-voltage cancerous membrane, the cells
stayed connected to the society and ignored their own mutated genes’ efforts
to turn them cancerous. In other words, Chernet and Levin reduced the
number of tumours just by repolarising the depolarised cancer cells.54 Chalk
up another win for the bioelectric code.
By 2016, Chernet could not only stop new tumours from forming – he
was able to ‘reprogramme’ existing ones back into normal tissue in tadpoles.
Their tumours were advanced: they had already spread and formed their own
blood supply. But when Chernet used light-activated channels (a technique
known as optogenetics) to modulate the cells’ resting potential, they stopped
acting like cancer. ‘You can turn on the light . . . and the tumor goes away,’
Adams, who was one of the co-authors of the paper, told Reuters.55
Electrically reminding the cells of their role in the rest of the tissue, Levin told
me, appeared to snap them out of their midlife crisis and help them re-enter
the society of cells. Bioelectricity overrides genetics. As these and other
experiments showed, the voltage changes were not merely a sign of cancer.
They controlled it.56
This is all fascinating, but it, too, is an extremely long way from your
doctor’s consulting room. Like all the recent results on bioelectricity, those in
cancer are still early. Tadpoles are really, really not like us. Furthermore,
repeating some of the experiments has flagged inconsistencies.57 There’s a lot
left to do.
Like regeneration, however, it offers a very tantalising prize: a control
switch for more complicated biological processes. ‘The electrical
communication amongst cells is really important for tumour suppression,’
Levin says. What’s more, this control switch might also be amenable to existing
pharmacological interventions. Like Djamgoz and Arcangeli, Levin is also
looking at ion-channel drugs.58
F rogs have been through a lot over these past 200 years of
electrophysiology, from Galvani’s grotesque puppeteering to Matteucci’s
body horror power source. But no one could have anticipated the next role
they would play in the quest to unite biology and electricity. In 2020, frogs
became the raw material for a class of organism that had never before existed
in the evolutionary history of the world.
Well, their cells did anyway. A few thousand of them were scraped off frog
embryos and then reconstituted into groups of roughly 2,000. Under some
cleverly programmed tutelage, the clumps began to cooperate, moving and
acting of their own accord, becoming – in the verbiage of their creators –
‘xenobots’, literally ‘frog’ (from Xenopus) ‘robots’. This wasn’t the average
person’s idea of a robot, but neither were these frogs, not any more. They did
not have brains or nervous systems so their ability to move and decide was
outside the traditional accounting for how animals do this. They didn’t have
mouths or stomachs so they couldn’t eat. No reproductive organs meant they
couldn’t make more. Joshua Bongard, the University of Vermont roboticist
who had helped create them, called them the only thing he could: ‘these are
novel living machines’.1
Wait – roboticist? Why is a roboticist creating frog cell bots?
Robotics is changing. Where they used to be thought of as hard-edged
appliances that occasionally (as in The Terminator) take on biological forms,
now the line is blurring between biology and robotics as we learn more about
both. After all, a robot is a programmable device that can manage information
– and a cell is turning out to be that too. The xenobots’ creators speculate that
the tiny organisms might someday deliver drugs into targeted areas of the
body, scrape plaque out of arteries, or clean up plastic waste in oceans. But
perhaps the most important thing they offer is a rare glimpse into the possible
future of the materials we use for robotics, electronics – and implants.
For years, researchers have toiled to find new and better ways to interface
with our nervous system, but have been thwarted by the mechanical, chemical,
and electrical properties of existing devices and their fundamental mismatch
with our brains. Compared to the signals they are tasked with manipulating,
these metal devices are rigid and bulky. ‘It’s like playing the piano with a
mallet,’ Andrew Jackson complained when I went to his neural interfaces lab at
Newcastle University to get a better understanding of the future of brain
implants. (His phrasing echoes Kip Ludwig’s on deep brain stimulation – two
researchers using this phrase is an interesting coincidence; if a third uses it, I
might cry conspiracy.)
For the past decade or more, the limitations of metal devices have
motivated an enormous project to create squishier, stretchier, more
biocompatible materials that let our bodies electrically communicate with the
foreign bodies inserted into them. This trend extends from tissue engineering
to robots, which are increasingly being augmented by, or made entirely out of,
synthetic materials such as hydrogel, a squishy polymer that is popular for soft
robotics.2 In the future, such nano-gloop-bots will supposedly swim through
our bodies, making adjustments to errant tissues.3
As we gain a better understanding of the electrical instructions of biology
itself, a sizeable contingent of scientists is beginning to wonder if the ultimate
biocompatible material isn’t just . . . literal biology. That’s why researchers are
now studying the properties of sea creatures, frogs, and fungi for their
programmability and biological compatibility.
To interact with the electrical signals of the body, either by reading or writing
them, you need to use an electrical device. In the brain and heart, implants like
pacemakers and deep brain stimulators have traditionally been made from
materials deployed in the semiconductor industry, like silicon or metals that
control the flow of electricity, among them platinum and gold.
But (unfortunately) your body isn’t made of gold. There’s no love lost
between these kinds of implants and biology, which is likely to mount a
healthy campaign of resistance to the invader. That is especially true with brain
implants, which create an inflammatory defence response in brains. You can’t
blame the brain, because during insertion, ‘the microelectrode tears blood
vessels, mechanically damages the membrane of neuronal and [other] cells, and
breaches the blood brain barrier’, per the authors of a widely cited 2019 study
of ways to calm the resulting inflammatory response.10 Things don’t get much
better from there.
For people who have no other options – some of whose stories I told in
Chapter 5 – an electrode can alleviate acute symptoms. But there are trade-offs
and problems. For one thing, metals tend to be the wrong thing to stick in a
brain. The two materials have a mismatched Young’s modulus, which is a way
of quantifying a material’s ‘bend or break’ quotient. For the brain, the Young’s
modulus describes not just its give but also its ability to deform and then
return to its prior shape. Say you had a bowl of jelly, and you stuck a pencil
into it, and carried it around your house. Initially, you’d see no gaps between
the jelly and the pencil: they’re in perfect contact and the join looks seamless.
But after a bit of walking around, you would soon see the jelly de-adhere from
the pencil. Worse for the wobbly dessert, apart from the big gaps between jelly
and pencil, there would start to be some indirect structural damage in the jelly
caused by the destabilising effects of the intrusion – sideways clefts that split
off from the pencil gap. The jelly starts to lose its structural integrity.
You don’t want any of this happening to your brain. Once neurons die,
they don’t regenerate. To try and protect them, the brain relies on supporting
characters called glia. They are traditionally considered fighters and janitors
that help defend and protect the neurons in their care and keep them running
optimally. After an electrode implant, these cells flood in to try to seal the rest
of the brain off against the wound made by the rigid, bulky electrode and the
dead neurons. To protect the brain’s integrity, they envelop the implant in a
thick sheath of proteins and cells. This creates a spatial and mechanical barrier
that, as it grows thicker, mutes any electrical signals the electrode can both
send and receive. Over time, the signals will degrade in crispness and the
implant will eventually stop working entirely. At that point it will have to be
replaced, which requires yet another brain surgery, another implant, more dead
neurons, and more angry glia.
Meanwhile, things aren’t going too great for the pencil in our extended
metaphor, either. Being sealed off from the electrical signals is not the only
trouble for the implant. Biology is hostile to things like silicon and metal.
Instead of a harmless tasty flavour, imagine your jelly is a corrosive brine of
salt and vinegar. That pencil may look okay for a while, but leave it in the
mixture long enough and it will start to take some damage – all right for a £1
pencil, less so for your extremely expensive, sensitive, experimental electrode.
Engineers test implant materials for longevity by bathing the devices in
warm salt water for a few weeks to try to recapitulate a couple of years in the
environment of a human body.11 But we have little idea what would happen to
an implant you want in your head for thirty years, as the scope for testing is
limited; mice only live for, at most, three to five years.
Are you thinking about those so-called telepathic AI-brain implants a little
differently now?
There’s an enormous research effort to mitigate these problems, with
many projects at various stages of maturity. Different rules will apply for
neural implants, tissue engineering, and materials for wound healing. But
broadly there are two rules, Chris Bettinger tells me, and he knows these rules,
because his laboratory at Carnegie Mellon University in Pittsburgh works on
creating the materials that need to obey them: ‘The main ways to make an
implant that evades the immune response is either make it very small or
camouflage it.’
The first rule explains why there has been an enormous effort to make
everything at the nanoscale. Tiny wires or grains will be so infinitesimal, so the
theory goes, that the brain won’t notice the interloper and therefore won’t
mount an immune response. The problem with that is that you can only do so
much listening or talking with a tiny device. The smaller the electrodes get, the
less suited they will be to recording from the brain due to basic physics.12
You’d have to make up for it by putting in many, many tiny devices. And then
your brain will probably notice, and you’re back to square one with an immune
response.
The other option addresses the problem a bit more elegantly: cover the
electrical interloper in something the body mistakes for familiar. Many people
are trying to come up with a material the body is happy to see hanging out
inside its environment and use it to disguise the silicon or metal beneath.13
The winning material needs to be able to conduct electricity without messing
with your brain’s structure or otherwise catching the attention of the glia. But
what material conducts electrons apart from metals? Well, as it turns out,
plastics.
We used to think polymers were insulators – and they are, which is why
they are used for, well, insulation. But in 1977, Alan J. Heeger, Alan G.
MacDiarmid, and Hideki Shirakawa found out that some kinds of plastic can
conduct a current when they discovered a synthetic polymer called
polyacetylene. Their fabrication of this ‘conductive plastic’ with metallic-like
electroactivity was a major breakthrough in the field – in 2000, it earned the
trio the Nobel in chemistry.14 It’s to them we owe the existence of flat-screen
TVs and anti-static coatings and all kinds of trappings of modern life. Their
discovery also kicked off a new field of research called organic electronics, and
twenty-five types of conductive polymers have been developed since.
One major goal for organic electronics has been to solve the Young’s
constant problem and thereby create ever squishier, more flexible electronics.
Some organic semiconductors fit the bill, and one that is getting a lot of
attention right now has an unspeakable name typical of the genre: poly(3,4-
ethylenedioxythiophene). The material (nickname PEDOT) is so promising it
even found its way into the Independent: ‘Scientists have discovered a ground-
breaking bio-synthetic material that they claim can be used to merge artificial
intelligence with the human brain,’ they reported in 2020. ‘The breakthrough is
a major step towards integrating electronics with the body to create part
human, part robotic “cyborg” beings’.15
And PEDOT is indeed nice – squishy, stable, and kinder to cells. But is it
going to help you become a cyborg? Kip Ludwig, ever jaundiced after his many
years in the industry, can contain his enthusiasm: ‘This is not a game-changer
by any stretch.’ Though PEDOT has been approved for devices like catheters,
like the other polymers vying to usher in our cyborg future, PEDOT has some
obstacles to overcome before the FDA or other bodies will let you pop it in
someone’s brain. Yes, it may be the least offensive implant material we have
ever made, and yes, it conducts electrons with the best of the rigid metal
implants. There’s just one problem: we don’t speak electron.
Lost in translation
A transistor is a little piece of silicon in your laptop that can switch on or off
the electrical current flowing through it. I don’t want to talk about transistors
any more than I have to, so please trust me that it is the fundamental unit of
modern computing, and that billions of these little guys are stuffed into your
laptop and your phone and all your other digital electronics, and they are
responsible for the astonishing capabilities of these machines.
Rolandi’s transistor looked nothing like the highly sophisticated, exquisitely
etched devices that sit in your laptop. It was neither processed nor exquisite,
just a few soggy-looking nanofibres of chitosan, a material derived from squid
pen, which is a vestigial internal hard bit descended from the animal’s ancestral
mollusc shell. It’s soft and pliable enough that a brain implant would likely
cause minimal scarring, but that wasn’t its main advantage. The appeal of this
transistor was that, unlike the fancy semiconductors that act as gatehouses for
electron currents, this one was able to control the flow of protons.
So why are we so excited about protons?
You might remember from Chapter 7, protons are just hydrogen ions.
Researchers understand them well, because their contributions to the reactions
that make energy in the cell have been studied to death.23 Protons are also the
main component that determines the acidity inside and outside cells. These are
among the most thoroughly picked-over mechanisms in biology.24 So far, so
boring, if I can be frank.
Here’s something about protons that is not boring: they are able to control
the membrane voltage of a cell, and thereby control sodium and potassium
and voltage, and thereby cell identity, during regeneration and cancer. ‘It
doesn’t matter which ions or ion channels you use as long as you can control
the voltage,’ says Dany Spencer Adams. ‘What’s important is the bioelectric
state they create.’ Protons were the easiest to use. You just had to borrow a
gene from yeast to make them. Adams and Levin used this insight to create
that mirror-image organ condition in frog embryos.
Controlling the flow of protons would do something that has not been
possible – combine the effectiveness of drugs with the local precision of
electrical zaps. If you could make an electrical device that could manipulate the
proton gradients the way they were altered to make frogs regenerate – but in a
more tailored way than a drug could – well, then you’d have a whole new
option for bioelectric medicine, a best of both worlds to combine the power of
ion-channel drugs and electroceuticals.
Actually, the more you learn about protons, the easier it is to understand
what Rolandi found so compelling about a device that could control their flow.
If you can manipulate protons in a cell, it becomes possible to do precision
tweaks of cellular electricity without involving electrons – or other ions. ‘It’s
really easy to use,’ Adams says. ‘A proton pump is nothing fancy – it’s just a
single protein.’ That means it is easy to get into the body. After isolating these
proteins from yeast, Adams then simply injected them into the frog embryos.
‘[The proton pump] assembles itself.’ That current changed the concentration
of protons in the cells, which changed the membrane voltage, which then
changed the identity of the cells. Soon, in Adams’ experiment, the once-non-
regenerating cells agreed to start regenerating again. The reverse was also true:
she was able to stop a regenerating frog from being able to do so by poisoning
one of its hydrogen pumps to keep it from working. ‘But it doesn’t matter how
you inject those protons or control them,’ she says. ‘The only thing that
matters is the voltage.’
In the decade or so since he first made that early smudge of chitosan,
Rolandi has honed his device and made many more. And he’s not alone.
Biological materials from cephalopods are an increasingly attractive research
area in general. Chitosan, for example, turns out to be much better at
absorbing large amounts of blood than traditional bandages, so it is widely
used in wound dressings for military applications.
But it’s their electrical properties that have drawn researchers to look more
closely at various parts of the squid. The chitosan from its pen conducts not
just protons, but other ions as well. A reflective protein in squid skin called –
of course – reflectin is also a conductor of protons. Even the ink that jets
defensively out of a squid contains eumelanin, which is capable of mixed
conduction.25
As these properties came to light, more people started tinkering with the
materials to see whether they could make a device that could control a non-
electron current. Alon Gorodetsky, a chemical engineer at the University of
California Irvine, has concluded that reflectin conducts protons fast enough to
make it a plausible material for a proton-based protonic transistor – just as the
transistor is the basic unit of computation that makes the current flow in your
electronic devices, a proton transistor might make ions flow instead.26
Gorodetsky and his group have also been testing materials from arthropods,
and they think these will form the next generation of biocompatible proton-
conducting materials and protonic devices.27 They may even be the basis of
edible batteries, which could also be useful for implants.28
But for all the advances in the field since his first foray into squidtronics,
Rolandi has moved away from cephalopods. ‘We gravitated towards the
biomaterial routes at the beginning,’ he told me, huffing through an early
morning hike near the University of California’s idyllic Santa Cruz campus,
where he is now the chair of the engineering department. ‘Back then, my
thinking hadn’t really crystallised yet.’ More than a decade after his first foray
into biological electronics, he’s agnostic about what kind of material he uses.
The real prize, he realised, was the ability – by any means – to control protons.
Rolandi started making proton devices to tweak cellular currents out of
silver chloride and palladium. The upshot was that protons might be a stopgap
until we can figure out how to interface with individual ions and individual
channels, and offer a more precise interaction and control than electrons
provide. A 2017 paper Rolandi wrote landed on Michael Levin’s desk, and he
got in touch. He knew exactly what he wanted to do with such a capability.
Levin had found that a cell’s fate (bone, neuron, fat, etc.) is tied to their
membrane voltage, as we’ve discussed. Fat cells tended to be around -50
millivolts with respect to the extracellular fluid. Bone cells were the most
polarised, at -90. Skin and neurons hovered in the middle around -70. He had
also seen that stem cells were nearly at 0, and as their membrane polarised so
did their identity develop in accordance with the amount. Now he wanted to
tune a stem cell’s voltage himself and thereby control its destiny. If you could
reliably drive it to become a fat cell, or a bone cell, or a neuron, this would be
proof that electricity could be used as a control system for a dizzying amount
of genetic and chemical processes.
But how could he keep a living cell in a constant state for enough time that
it would differentiate into something new – probably hours, possibly days?
The problem with cells is that they are homeostatic – if something perturbs
their voltage, they will quickly rebalance. In the body, that problem is solved
because the microenvironment around the cell exerts constant regulatory
signalling. No existing tool in an electrophysiologist’s repertoire was capable of
mimicking this.
Then DARPA swooped in to help. It has a long history of investing heavily
in research to advance, for example, new directions in prosthetic limbs and
neuroprosthetics. Around the time Rolandi met Levin, DARPA also developed
an interest in bioelectricity, thanks to the arrival of a new program manager
called Paul Sheehan, who had been deeply influenced by Rolandi’s proton
transistor. (In the course of a previous appointment at the US Naval Research
Laboratory, Sheehan had used proton pumps to design colour-changing
bioelectronic devices, based on squid camouflage.29)
Now that he had a purse at DARPA, Sheehan gave Rolandi and Levin
money for their stem cell fate project. With the money, Rolandi and Levin
brought Marcella Gomez onboard. Gomez is a mathematical and systems
biologist at Santa Cruz with a background in control theory and cybernetics.
She knows her way around mathematical tools that can drive biology and
realised what they needed was a machine learning system that could monitor
the constantly changing voltage of the cells and act on it in real time. So she
made one.
The team put stem cells into an array with a device of Rolandi’s design,
which injected a proton current around the cell to drive its membrane voltage
up. Whenever a cell would engage any of its channels to try to get back to a
more comfortable voltage, Gomez’s AI would notice and inject more proton
current. It was able to consistently keep the membrane voltage of living stem
cells 10 millivolts higher than the cell’s usual depolarised baseline. In 2020, the
trio published the results of Gomez’s remarkable new tool, which had
managed to continuously impose the artificial voltage for ten hours. No one
had ever done that before.
As they were working out how to extend that voltage window so that they
could watch the stem cell differentiate, however, funding ran out.
But that turned out to be okay, because by then, they had given Sheehan all
the evidence he needed to launch the much bigger project he had been
planning. At the beginning of 2020, DARPA launched the $16 million BETR
(Bioelectronics for Tissue Regeneration) programme – a pretty hefty chunk of
money even by DARPA standards – whose goal is a radically expedited healing
process for wounds.30 This has not been possible with traditional electronics
(or anything else, for that matter). While individual studies on electrical
stimulation for healing have sometimes had promising results, no one could
ever give you a specific recipe for how to make it work every time in every
patient. Sheehan had seen enough of the research to suspect that speaking to
the body in its own language might be the way out of the impasse. ‘I wanted to
pivot to bioelectricity mediated by ions, instead of just a voltage,’ he told me.
‘Right now, it is very challenging to go from electrical to biochemical signals
and vice versa. That’s what this programme is trying to do.’ He wants to
improve every aspect of wound healing, from better sensors and actuators to
creating better models of how healing actually works.
There are so many things we don’t know about wounds, and this is why no
one has come up with advances to make them heal better or faster. One
problem is that every wound is different. Sheehan ticked off the list for me.
‘The edge of a wound is different from the centre. A cut on your foot heals at
a different rate than a cut on your face. Young people heal faster than old
people.’
Rolandi’s group is using bioelectronics to control different aspects of
wound regeneration. Instead of just applying an electric field and hoping for
general improvements, the idea is to be specific. The team monitors specific
wound processes (like the inflammation stage) with sensors. Gomez’s
algorithms then process information from these sensors into actionable items,
for example to deliver ions or an electric field to the wound to calm
macrophages faster in a bid to accelerate the healing process. They couldn’t get
that granular without more diverse tools at their disposal. ‘Otherwise it would
be like, great, you’ve detected all this stuff, and you have this very complicated
algorithm, and now all you can do with the information is shoot an electron at
it,’ says Rolandi. ‘That’s just not going to cut it.’
But just as the stem cell project was for Sheehan a stepping stone to the
BETR project, so is BETR a step to something bigger. ‘Wound healing is a
great initial problem to go after,’ he said. ‘But if you look across the board,
there are many different places in medicine where you want to have control
over the delivery of a drug compound.’ One oft-cited example is delivering
specifically targeted medicines to a tumour, but a generic interface could also
choose the time of delivery not just the place. Any oncologist will tell you that
they wish cancer medications could be administered to their patients at night
when they’re asleep, because that is when the body is regenerating. What’s
more, during this resting period, some non-cancerous tissues that are most
sensitive to drugs are not dividing – administering noxious drugs then would
help reduce some of the adverse consequences. But of course you can’t
administer these drugs in the middle of the night. The doctors and nurses and
office managers are asleep too.
Hence Sheehan’s next goal: ‘What we really need is a generic interface with
biology that would enable us to deliver biological information into the body,’
he told me. ‘Cytokines, hormones, chemokines. Having a generic device that
could deliver those therapies would be like having a twenty-four-hour doctor
right there,’ says Sheehan. Or for wounds, it would act as a twenty-four-hour
surgeon on call. As it happens, that was one of the selling points of the
xenobots.
When Michael Levin first started dismantling the frog embryos, he wanted to
understand what happens to living cells when they are freed from the
constraints of the electrical signals sent by their bioelectric environment. You
remember from Chapter 7 that he and a coterie of other scientists believe that
these cues are a crucial authority that instructs cells which shape to assume
and where, and that this guidance is crucial to whether those cells cooperate in
their trillions to properly form us in the womb.
But how do you put that idea to the test? ‘The xenobots were a way of
asking: here are a bunch of cells, how do they specify what they should be
building, in the absence of any guidance?’ Levin told me. ‘The big picture is
not about having robots made of frog cells, or in fact, robots made of any
cells. The idea here is that we need to understand how collections of
competent agents work together towards large goals.’ That would have
obvious corollaries in regenerative medicine: how do cells get together and
agree on building something large, like an organ, or in fact, the whole body? It
might also yield insights into why and under what circumstances cells opt for
an ‘every man for himself ’ approach to become cancerous.
‘Everything in my lab focuses on this idea of how many become one,’ he
says. ‘How is it that lots of small competent agents get together to have a
single unified cognitive system that has a goal state?’ If we understand how
that works, then rebuilding organs, reprogramming tumours, fixing birth
defects, and reversing aging really does just become a matter of programming.
‘Everything boils down to the question of convincing cells to build one thing
rather than whatever they’re currently doing,’ he says.
So he decided to see what the cells would do in the absence of the cues.
He and his collaborators scraped a few thousand cells off a frog embryo. Then
they put them into a completely different, neutral environment and waited to
see what they would make of their newfound independence. The cells had a
lot of options. They could have just died. All the cells could have struck out on
their own. They could have formed themselves into a single layered ‘skin’ that
lay in a flat plane like a cell culture.
They did none of that.
Instead, a few thousand of them got together and made something new.
Somehow, they agreed among themselves to glom together into a new
architecture, little discrete balls. Then each of them grew cilia, which in itself
wasn’t unusual. These little hairs grow on the outer surface of normally
developing embryos to move the mucus around the body and keep it clean –
what was unusual was how they put them to use. ‘These cells basically
repurposed that genetically encoded hardware,’ Levin says. Now, instead of
using their cilia to move the mucus, they used them to move themselves –
even though they didn’t have any nervous system to either conjure intent or
act on that intent. Nonetheless, with their new equipment in place, they started
coasting around. ‘We have these amazing videos of the little clumps moving
around. Sometimes they formed little groups, interacted in various
configurations, they even went through a maze.’
And even though they were just clumps of cells with no brain or nervous
system, they even seemed to have preferences. When Levin cut them nearly in
half, they regenerated, always seeming to favour reconstituting themselves into
the little spherical shapes they had originally assumed, if a ball of 2,000 frog
cells can be said to prefer something. Xenobots indeed. ‘These are neither a
traditional robot nor a known species of animal. It’s a new class of artifact: a
living, programmable organism,’ said Joshua Bongard, the roboticist on the
team.
So far, the only thing that is strictly programmable about them is their
shape and their lifespan. The xenobots don’t have digestive systems and so
their cells are instantiated with a little yolk sac containing a fixed amount of
fuel. When that runs out, they die. That does seem to be the main advantage
of using living systems as robots – living systems die, which rules out the
horror scenario of xenobots overrunning the world.
Or maybe not. By the end of 2021, they had been programmed to
reproduce.31 They had not built themselves novel sex organs – instead, they
used their Pacman-like mouths to scoop up cells into groups of roughly the
same size as themselves, which then themselves aggregated into new lifeforms.
They made new creatures like themselves. It was a method of reproduction
that was new to the evolutionary history of the planet. Nearly five years into
working on the creatures, Levin is unequivocal: they are alive – ‘under any
reasonable definition of life’. No wonder ethicists began to worry. ‘Is this as
much of a Pandora’s Box as it sounds?’ wrote two of them not long after the
self-reproduction study was published, raising a range of possible adverse
consequences and wondering whether science needed more limits placed on it
to avoid them.32 ‘Although xenobots are not currently made from human
embryos or stem cells, it is conceivable they could be,’ they wrote.
Andrew Adamatzky thinks biology is the inevitable future of implants, but
where others are working with frogs and squids, his money is on fungus.
Adamatzky is a professor of unconventional computing at the University of
the West of England, where he has created a computer model of the mycelial
electrical activity and encoded the spikes into logical functions, a bit like
AND/OR functions transistors are able to create in traditional computing.33
Once we have these for the body, why not for the environment?
The future is not diving to the bottom of a reef to source some coral for
your hip. The future is to understand the properties of biomaterials that make
them good interfaces, and then make a steady supply of them, tuned to the
properties that can best interface with the body – synthetic coral, synthetic
squid pen, to ensure a steady supply of materials whose quality is as unerring
as the silicon crystals that now make semiconductor wafers.
While we wait for the new ion-channel drugs, the new trials, and the
biological implants (none of which are guaranteed to be with us in ten years),
there is another option for electroceuticals: non-invasive wearables that can do
it all from outside the skin.
CHAPTER 10
Around the time she was trying to steer the FDA onto the right track with the
oscillating field stimulator, Jennifer French founded Neurotech Network, a
neurotechnology advocacy group that helps people with neurological injuries
find assistive technology that can help them in their specific circumstance.
‘Technology can become a great equaliser,’ she says. ‘It gives people choices.’
However, often people who design neurotechnologies can focus on the
kind of evidence that will moisten the eyes of people who are like them, at the
expense of things that will help the actual people who need the technology.
French understands why they do it. ‘Getting people walking again is sexy,’ she
says. Behind the scenes, after the media attention has died down, the
researchers will quietly file the grants that contain the sorts of priorities that
are actually important to people who have a spinal injury: pain, and bowel and
bladder control. ‘Addressing the true needs of this population doesn’t sell in
the media.’
Instead, the public message remains on what the disability studies scholar
Stella Young calls ‘inspiration porn’, and it has wide-reaching consequences.8
For example, those research videos of paralysed people walking make the
rounds on social media and in the traditional media. Shorn of their context –
which they often are, because you know how the internet works – they give
people a deeply skewed picture of what is possible if they get injured.
‘Every time one of those news stories comes out – Hey, we’ve cured spinal
cord injury, we have people up and walking! – that leaves a false impression,’ French
says. ‘Then the advocacy groups get tons of phone calls from people living
with the condition, asking “When can I get the cure?”’ It is not of course a
cure, and it falls to groups like hers to bring people down from the hype. The
media hype is destructive in more ways than one.
The misperception it seeds also makes it hard for people to have a clear
picture of what capabilities actually exist. This makes it difficult to objectively
evaluate whether to enrol in a trial.
When Phil Kennedy underwent voluntary, extremely risky (and potentially
unethical) surgery on himself, he was widely hailed in the tech press as a self-
sacrificing hero of science. And yet, with a few exceptions, the coverage of the
volunteers in clinical trials has little of this veneration. ‘The people who test
neurotechnology are test pilots every bit as much as Chuck Yeager or Buzz
Aldrin,’ French says. Just as those men risked their lives to expand science’s
understanding of the sound barrier and space flight, people who volunteer to
test new neurotechnologies should be understood as daredevils working – at
great risk to themselves – to take science into new frontiers.
Yeager and Aldrin (and Kennedy) knew the risks inside out before they
undertook their experimental flights. But there are no standards for how
clinicians should communicate expectations to people who volunteer for trials.
As many people join trials of new invasive experimental neurotechnology for
altruistic reasons as they do hoping this trial ends up being the cure or an aid.
Sometimes a volunteer comes into a trial desperate, their head filled with
misleading ideas from the ‘inspiration porn’. French is exasperated by this
because the people who join clinical trials are not guinea pigs, and should not
be condescended to or tantalised with false hope.
Making someone a test pilot is only ethically possible when that person has
the full picture of everything that could go wrong, and precisely managed
expectations of what the technology can and can’t do. Right now, that kind of
transparency is not enforced. ‘We need to be really clear with people about
what this can do for them personally,’ French says. But there are no standards
clinicians must adhere to when advising their trial volunteers.
For one thing, anyone who designs bioelectric interfaces should look at the
ethical history of medical implants. We know about the grisly history of people
having implants against their will – but what about explants? A few people had
their experimental implants removed against their will after the companies that
made them went broke. I spent a few hours at a neuroscience conference a
couple of years ago talking through these issues with Frederic Gilbert, a
neuroscientist and philosopher at the University of Tasmania who studies
explantation.
Gilbert points in particular to a major ethical issue – potential trial
participants are often not told the whole story about the future of their
devices. A study from Rice University and Baylor College of Medicine found
that potential study participants are generally soft-shoed when it comes to
what will happen to their implants after a trial has concluded.
A typical case would involve a person with a treatment-resistant disease
that was robbing her of her quality of life. Maybe she could no longer drive or
work. As a last resort, she would join a clinical trial of an implant that
promised to change all of that. The implant would work. Soon she could drive,
make plans, and regain the fairly predictable life most of us take for granted.
But her implant was an experimental device, and when the neurotech start-
up that had implanted her found their device did not work for everyone in the
trial, the company collapsed into bankruptcy. The insolvent company could no
longer support their devices, so they needed them back. That meant another
brain surgery to remove the investigational device. She was unprepared to
return to life before the implant. She did not consent to having the device
removed, or to the brain surgery. ‘How are you supposed to recover these
devices?’ Gilbert asked me. ‘Do you hunt these people down? It becomes like
something out of Blade Runner.’9
When radical new medical technologies are successful, the tech press
reports breathlessly on paralysed people who can eat grapes, or trial results
that unlock another thing brain implants could ameliorate. But what happens
when the trials are over? The tech press is a little harder to locate.
You might be wondering: why can’t these devices stay in? Usually it’s
because they need long-term tech support a failed start-up won’t be able to
provide. Stimulator batteries need to be changed, or stimulation frequencies
adjusted. There needs to be someone in charge of medical check-ups for
people with implants bedded down in their grey matter. In rare cases, this is
possible – if the person in charge of your clinical trial happens to be Helen
Mayberg. You could call Mayberg the doyenne of DBS – after a long and
prestigious career at Emory University, the Icahn School of Medicine at New
York’s Mount Sinai Hospital created a new Center for Advanced Circuit
Therapeutics just so she could run it. When you implant patients, she says, you
own them. ‘Not in the sense that you can do what you want with them – it’s
the opposite,’ she says. You now bear a lifelong responsibility to them.
Mayberg is passionate about this and has clawed her way to letting her
participants keep their depression-busting DBS implants after a trial. But she is
also a big, powerful name in neuroscience with a lot of credentials to back her
up, institutional firepower at her behest, and university funding.
Hank Greely, a law professor at Stanford and an expert on ethics in
biosciences, thinks the answer is that before any neuro-engineering or
bioelectricity researchers run any kind of trial, the companies or universities
behind them should be forced to invest in something like a bond – ‘a common
fund that allows people to keep their devices, have them maintained and
repaired, and their batteries replaced,’ he says. ‘These people are not rats. You
don’t implant them, grab your data, and get rid of them.’
Today, French lends her expertise to several neuroethics panels and patient
advocacy panels, including the National Institutes of Health, the BRAIN
initiative, and the Institute for Electrical and Electronics Engineers, which is
working on a framework for neuroethics around medical devices and
neurotechnology devices. All the new standards aim to ensure full disclosure
for the volunteers who are trialling deep brain stimulation, spinal stimulators,
and other next-generation neurotech. This is part of the larger neuro-rights
initiative that is gaining traction, most recently having been written into law in
Chile in 2021.10
More volunteers, who are better informed, will accelerate our understanding of
neural implants, electroceuticals and other kinds of electrical intervention. But
electrical stimulation is not the only way we are impacting our normal
bioelectric functions.
We’re starting to look for future electro-drugs in the ion-channel drugs we
have been using for decades. They are ion-channel manipulators, capable of
either blocking them, prying them open, or otherwise messing with their state.
As we saw in Chapters 7 and 8, a more complete understanding of their
significance in bioelectric signalling is driving new research into how these
drugs could be repurposed for cancer therapies and regenerative medicine. But
it also opens up a more unsettling question: if we’re already taking so many of
these drugs, do we have a complete understanding of what they have been
doing to our electrome? Should we start figuring it out?
We started using drugs that acted on our ion channels long before we
actually knew about ion channels. We used them because they worked – we
figured out how they worked later.
In some, bioelectric side effects are already well understood. Most epilepsy
drugs, for example, are acknowledged to cause a range of birth abnormalities if
taken during pregnancy. As it turns out, this is because of how they muck with
our bioelectricity. Many of them suppress overactive sodium or calcium
channels, but while this helps to calm the relevant neurons and stop seizures,
increasing evidence suggests it may also disrupt the ion-channel
communications necessary to correctly pattern a foetal structure. In one
medication, the severity of the potential consequences – a significant risk of
lifelong learning and cognitive disabilities, and physical anomalies – have led to
restricting the medicine’s use in people who can get pregnant, during peak
reproductive age.
Epileptic drugs are far from the only ones with wide-ranging effects on
ion channels – and yet there has been little research into how others might
mess with the complex ways ion channels are involved in development. Like
Kip Ludwig, Emily Bates works on the nitty-gritty details of bioelectricity, but
she does it as a developmental biologist at the University of Colorado School
of Medicine. Bates had long wondered what other drugs can screw up the ion
channels and thereby result in birth defects.
A quick caveat before I go on. It’s very, very early days for some of this
research. Talk of medical effects on development is often tinged with a
particular strain of prim authoritarianism. When you’re pregnant, you aren’t
‘allowed’ to do much of anything without conjuring stern words of concern. I
would be mortified if my book were used as yet another cudgel to shame
someone at a point in their lives when everything is already quite unsettling
enough. This is why it is so important to fund research that lets us know what
is safe for a developing foetus.
Bates decided to focus on drugs for which a robust body of research
already existed, to demonstrate their ill effects on pregnancy. Smoking, for
example, has been widely shown to ‘increase the risk of health problems for
developing babies, including preterm birth, and low birth weight’, confirms the
Centre for Disease Control, along with being firmly linked to birth defects of
the mouth and lip, like cleft palate. But it has been hard to tease out which of
the 7,000 ingredients in cigarettes is the culprit, as they contain such a vast
assortment of chemicals – including ammonia and lead – many of which are
implicated in cancer. This is part of the reason vaping has been quietly
accepted as a harm reduction campaign – it’s the nicotine without all the rest
of the stuff.11 Maybe nicotine in this form is still not great, and it’s not quite
doctor-recommended, but it’s no shock that smokers will often switch to
vaping when they become pregnant. And if they vape already, they may not try
to quit when they find they are pregnant. Either way, vaping nicotine often
results in a higher dose.12
Does foetal nicotine exposure cause birth defects? Bates exposed pregnant
mice to pure nicotine by putting them into a vaping chamber (essentially a
giant walk-in bong) and found that the newborns still had several characteristic
developmental problems: they had shorter bones, notably of the humerus and
femurs (correlated with shorter stature in humans), and the nicotine exposure
had altered their lung development.13 So nicotine was not the innocent
bystander here, as it was clearly causing these defects. So vapes, which provide
nicotine, are bad for a developing baby.
It’s not yet possible to neatly tie these physical effects to a mechanism. But
there’s a lot of other research whose pieces fit intriguingly well with the new
data to form a compelling picture. It’s well established, for example, that
nicotine binds and blocks a potassium channel that’s known as ‘inwardly
rectifying’ – that is, it keeps the concentrations in the cell at the cell’s ‘happy
place’, because the potassium channel’s job is to allow more potassium ions to
enter the cell than leave the cell. Bates has spent her career on this one
channel. Alcohol, it is turning out from early evidence in her lab, may affect it
as well, which may be a culprit for the birth defects associated with foetal
alcohol syndrome.
Anaesthesia also does weird things to ion channels in ways we aren’t quite
across yet. It’s not just during pregnancy that drugs might affect bioelectric
signalling. If you’ve ever had general anaesthesia, there’s a small chance you
may be at higher risk of developing cancer later in life,14 or maybe memory
problems.15 There are even cases where people appear to be under but are not,
or present lingering, mysterious PTSD-like symptoms.16 But we don’t know,
because we actually don’t know exactly how anaesthesia does what it does.
Well, we do know some things. ‘We know what it does to neurons,’ says Patrick
Purdon, a professor of anaesthesia at Harvard. It makes them fire in ways that
are completely different from normal physiological processes, in some cases
shutting down all firing entirely for seconds at a time. The result of this is that
complete gone-ness, more complete than any sleep. We might know the
neurons stop – but we don’t have a molecular explanation for how they do.
Or to be blunt, how they turn back on again. ‘The amazing thing about
general anaesthesia is that any of us ever come back from it the same person as
who went in,’ says Michael Levin. Not everyone does. Some people have
hallucinations. Those little immortal worms, the planarians, when you give
them anaesthesia (and cut off their heads), regrow a head – of another species.
Even bacteria respond to anaesthesia.
It’s not just drugs that can catch our ion channels by surprise. In 2019, a
fifty-four-year-old construction worker collapsed and died even though he was
reported to be in perfectly good health. A year later, the New England Journal of
Medicine published an investigation into the strange case.17 In the three weeks
before he died, the man had eaten one to two large bags of liquorice a day,
every day. At Massachusetts General Hospital, where doctors spent twenty-
four hours trying to save him after his collapse, it became evident that his
heart rhythm had been irreversibly destabilised. It turns out that liquorice’s
active ingredient, glycyrrhizin, mimics a process the body uses when it needs
to retain sodium and shake off potassium. His potassium channels were
gasping for ions, but there weren’t any. Without these ions to regulate the
sodium–potassium balance of the heart’s cells, his heart was incapable of firing
regular action potentials. He hadn’t been the first to have this experience.
Enough similar incidents had piled up by 2012 to occasion a review article
entitled ‘Licorice Abuse’, whose authors darkly warned that liquorice was ‘not
just a candy’. They urged the US Food and Drug Administration to regulate
the ‘substance’ and create public health messages around its health hazards.18
Five years later, the Food and Drug Administration partly obliged, issuing a
stark warning about the dangers of liquorice in time for Halloween. Black
licorice, they asked, trick or treat?
Quite the tangential mashup I have assembled here. But I’m trying to paint
a picture of all the unexpected ways we can unwittingly affect our electrome. I
hope I am making the case for a more holistic understanding of our bioelectric
dimensions. Unfortunately, so far there has been a lot of resistance to that.
One good example was the reaction to Bates’s research.
Institutional silos
The paper shouldn’t have been controversial. It was only a review, and a pretty
dry one at that. It’s not that contentious to point out that bioelectricity seems
to play important roles in development, even though the mechanism by which
it does so remains unclear. So Bates and her co-author put together an
overview of the various mechanisms and theories that have been proposed to
account for bioelectrical involvement in foetal development. They sent this
draft article to a journal, which distributed it to several other scientists, as is
standard practice in peer review in order to publish any paper in a respected
scientific journal. The journal editor passed the feedback to Bates, along with a
breezy instruction to ‘address’ it. Bates made the mistake of reading the
document right before she went to bed.
Some of the responses were scathing in a way that seemed out of all
proportion with the paper they were commenting on. No one was talking
about methodological flaws or accusing her of fraudulent data. They were
simply scathing about the whole field. Phrases like ‘the mythology of the
membrane voltage’ suffused the commentary. The fatal blow seemed to be
Bates’s mention of the bioelectric code.
It wasn’t the first time I heard about one of these dismissive, excoriating
reviews – Ann Rajnicek (who had worked with Borgens) had told me about a
grant application turned down crisply with the single query, ‘Does anyone
really believe this shit any more?’ But as a science journalist, I know that
bruising peer review is just part of the game. As I spoke to more researchers,
however, I noticed a pattern.19 People didn’t ‘believe’ Laura Hinkle had
enacted electrotaxis on cells. Nor did they believe Dany Adams. Or Ai-Sun
Tseng. ‘No one has ever said they don’t believe our data,’ another bioelectricity
researcher told me. ‘They just don’t want to hear it.’ Now here was Bates with
another variation on the theme. The common factor seemed to be that critics
weren’t taking issue with any particulars. Rather, they were issuing sweeping
statements, contemptuous dismissals larded with emotion and belief language.
While critics often couldn’t point to specific problems with the science or
processes in the publications, they kept using phrases like ‘I don’t believe it’.
That’s literally what a colleague told Michael Levin at a conference: ‘I haven’t
read the papers and I don’t need to. I don’t believe it.’
But what is it they don’t believe? That depends on who is doing the
disbelieving. Levin is often invited to give talks across a wide range of
disciplines, from developmental biology departments to NeurIPs, the biggest
AI conference in the world. ‘Someone always gets pissed off,’ he said to me.
‘What they get pissed off about depends on which department I’m in.’
Contentions that are received as obvious by a neuroscientist are sacrilege for a
molecular geneticist. It’s no grand conspiracy driving scepticism around
bioelectricity’s relevance outside the nervous system, though. It’s merely
education.
Jose Lopez at the National Science Foundation thinks we need new ways
to start communicating across these ever-separating disciplines. ‘We need a
new department, and we need polymaths. Not the kind we used to have – that
ship has sailed. People like Alexander von Humboldt and Galvani were alive at
a time when it was still possible to know everything in science. Now a scientist
can spend their entire career on one mutation in one gene that causes one
variant of this rare disease.’ Stephen Badylak agrees that in the field of
medicine, many remain in silo.
A compelling new alternative is getting underway right now, exemplified at
MIT’s Department of Biological Engineering, where you can get a PhD in
being a polymath: students in this department are specifically trained to talk
across disciplines and focus in on the vocabulary and concepts needed to
bridge the chasms between them. They are encouraged to think in a systems
biology way about how information is flowing, rather than viewing
information as discrete chunks.
T here’s a joke that first-time book authors thank every person they have
ever met and I will be no exception. First, thanks to Simon Thorogood
for taking a chance on this! To Mollie Wiesenfeld and Georgia Frances King
for executing the most enormous trust fall. To Carrie Plitt for seeing the
outlines of the book long before I ever did.
I cannot get over the generosity of the scientists and researchers who
answered my emails and phone and zoom queries. They spent hours with me
through Covid lockdowns and electoral chaos to explain wildly difficult
concepts, talk me through controversial history, and help me keep my focus.
Alphabetically: Dany Adams patiently read draft after draft, and sent diagrams
and markups, and ironed out my misunderstandings. Debra Bohnert, thank
you so much for our wonderful conversation – I hope the story does the man
justice. Robert Campenot fielded my first terrified questions with grace and
good humour. Edward Farmer, thank you for reading many drafts of chapters
that were cut! I promise you will see them again. Flavio Frohlich, our
conversations unlocked an entire chapter. Franklin Harold, thank you for
giving me the most wonderful quote that had to be cut out of the final version
of the book (I will never not laugh about the vibrating probe at Wood’s Hole,
and I’m not sorry). Andrew Jackson, thank you for making me start thinking
seriously about ions. Nancy Kopell, thank you for an unforgettable
conversation. Michael Levin: thank you for four years of tirelessly answering
questions, sending papers, reading drafts, sending more papers, reading more
drafts, et cetera ad infinitum. Jianming Li, thank you for making me
understand the hair-raisingly difficult mechanics of die-back and for the many
hours on the phone. Kip Ludwig, thank you for sending more papers than a
single human could read in one lifetime, and for ‘fast email answers’ that went
on for multiple pages. Marco Piccolino, thank you for helping me figure out
where to start, and then making sure I ended up at the right end – thank you
in particular for sending me your amazing book. Ann Rajnicek, thank you for
giving me the oral history of Lionel Jaffe’s lab, but most of all for telling me
about Richard Borgens. Ken Robinson spent ages on the phone with me to
explain how the spinal stimulator did (and did not) work. Nigel Wallbridge, for
all the time spent explaining the bioelectric CANBUS system. Harold Zakon
and Min Zhao, thank you for explaining, respectively, ion channel motifs and
galvanotaxis.
Phone calls can’t do everything. I also trudged through some difficult
reading to try to understand the complicated history of how neuroscience
integrated electricity. Some books offered the keys that unlocked these papers
and historical documents. Among these, three stand out for their clarity and
explanatory power: Matthew Cobb’s book The Idea of the Brain was an
invaluable resource. So were Robert Campenot’s Animal Electricity, and Frances
Ashcroft’s Spark of Life. For anyone in whom my book sparked a curiosity
about the history of science of the brain and nervous system, I urge them to
drop everything and read these books.
To the people who read my early drafts, I am indebted to you forever:
Richard Panek, thank you so much for your inestimable help with my first,
unreadable attempts at ‘history of science’. Lowri Daniels and Michele Kogon,
you are rock stars – thank you for ensuring my facts were actually facts. David
Robson, Clare Wilson, Richard Fisher, thanks for patiently listening to my
spittle-flecked terror. Darryl Rambo, Lorri Lofvers and Joyce Wong, for being
a steadfast lifeboat of calm in turbulent years. Sumit Paul-Choudhury, Hal
Hodson and Will Heaven, thank you for the life-saving powers of Voltron, and
for enduring my endless yammering without exiting the chat (well, thanks to
two of you anyway). Sarita Bhatt: for understanding the subtext. Cristina
Calotta – I can’t thank you enough for the endless stream of requests for
GDPR articles I couldn’t access from their US websites or a library during
Covid. Soren, Cassie, Erin, Mike – you’re the constellations in the firmament.
Ann, you’re the North Star.
Dad, you got me curious about all the things there are to know in the
world – no good deed goes unpunished, so now you have to read this whole
book. Mom, you made sure I had a sceptical head on my shoulders while I fell
down every rabbit hole. And Nick and Daisy and Charlie: thank you for
enduring the late nights, the caffeine jitters, the endless illegible notes about
the neural code scrawled on random pieces of paper (and sometimes on
important mail), the mood rollercoasters. Thank you for your encouragement,
your grace, your patience, and your love.
NOTES
Introduction
1 Condliffe, Jamie. ‘Glaxo and Verily Join Forces to Treat Disease By Hacking Your Nervous System’,
MIT Technology Review, 1 August 2016.
<https://www.technologyreview.com/2016/08/01/158574/glaxo-and-verily-join-forces-to-treat-
disease-by-hacking-your-nervous-
system/https://www.technologyreview.com/2016/08/01/158574/glaxo-and-verily-join-forces-to-
treat-disease-by-hacking-your-nervous-system/>
2 Hutchinson, Alex. ‘For the Golden State Warriors, Brain Zapping Could Provide an Edge’, The New
Yorker, 15 June 2016. <https://www.newyorker.com/tech/annals-of-technology/for-the-golden-
state-warriors-brain-zapping-could-provide-an-edge>
3 Reardon, Sarah. ‘“Brain doping” may improve athletes’ performance’. Nature 531 (2016), pp. 283–4
4 Blackiston, Douglas J., and Micheal Levin. ‘Ectopic eyes outside the head in Xenopus tadpoles
provide sensory data for light-mediated learning’. Journal of Experimental Biology 216 (2013), pp. 1031–
40; Durant, Fallon, Junji Morokuma, Christopher Fields, Katherine Williams, Dany Spencer Adams,
and Michael Levin. ‘Long-Term, Stochastic Editing of Regenerative Anatomy via Targeting
Endogenous Bioelectric Gradients’. Biophysical Journal, vol. 112, no. 10 (2017), pp. 2231–43
1 Pancaldi, Giuliano. Volta: Science and Culture in the Age of Enlightenment. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press, 2005, p. 111
2 Galvani, Luigi. Commentary on the Effects of Electricity on Muscular Motion. Trans. Margaret Glover Foley.
Norwalk, CN: Burndy Library, 1953, p. 79
3 Pancaldi, Volta, p. 54; and Morus, Iwan Rhys. Frankenstein’s Children: Electricity, Exhibition, and
Experiment in Early-Nineteenth-Century London. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press, 1998, p. 232
4 Needham, Dorothy. Machina Carnis: The Biochemistry of Muscular Contraction in its Historical Development.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1971, pp. 1–26
5 Needham, Machina Carnis, p. 7
6 Kinneir, David. ‘A New Essay on the Nerves, and the Doctrine of the Animal Spirits Rationally
Considered’. London, 1738, pp. 21 and 66–7
<https://archive.org/details/b30525068/page/n5/mode/2up>
7 O’Reilly, Michael Francis, and James J. Walsh. Makers of Electricity. New York: Fordham University
Press, 1909, p. 81
8 Cohen, I. Bernard. Benjamin Franklin’s Science. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1990, p. 42
9 Finger, Stanley, and Marco Piccolino. The Shocking History of Electric Fishes. Oxford: Oxford University
Press, 2011, pp. 282–5
10 Bresadola, Marco, and Marco Piccolino. Shocking Frogs: Galvani, Volta, and the Electric Origins of
Neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2013, p. 27
11 Bergin, William. ‘Aloisio (Luigi) Galvani (1737–1798) and Some Other Catholic Electricians’. In: Sir
Bertram Windle (ed.), Twelve Catholic Men of Science. London: Catholic Truth Society, 1912, pp. 69–87
12 Bresadola & Piccolino, Shocking Frogs, p. 27
13 O’Reilly & Walsh, Makers of Electricity, p. 152; and Bergin, ‘Aloisio (Luigi) Galvani’, p. 75
14 Cavazza, Marta. ‘Laura Bassi and Giuseppe Veratti: an electric couple during the Enlightenment’.
Institut d’Estudis Catalans, Vol. 5, no. 1 (2009), pp. 115–24 (pp. 119–21)
15 Messbarger, R. M. The Lady Anatomist: The Life and Work of Anna Morandi Manzolini. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press, 2010, p. 157
16 Frize, Monique. Laura Bassi and Science in 18th-Century Europe. Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer, 2013; see
also Messbarger, The Lady Anatomist, pp. 171–3
17 Foccaccia, Miriam, and Raffaella Simili. ‘Luigi Galvani, Physician, Surgeon, Physicist: From Animal
Electricity to Electro-Physiology’. In: Harry Whitaker, C. U. M. Smith and Stanley Finger (eds),
Brain, Mind and Medicine: Essays in Eighteenth-Century Neuroscience Boston: Springer, 2007, pp. 145–58
(p. 154)
18 Bresadola & Piccolino, Shocking Frogs, p. 76
19 Bresadola & Piccolino, Shocking Frogs, p. 89
20 Bresadola & Piccolino, Shocking Frogs, p. 122
21 O’Reilly & Walsh, Makers of Electricity, p. 133 3
22 See Bernardi, W. ‘The controversy on animal electricity in eighteenth-century Italy. Galvani, Volta
and others’. In: F. Bevilacqua and L. Fregonese (eds), Nuova Voltiana: Studies on Volta and His Times
Vol. 1. Milan: Hoepli, 2000, pp. 101–12 (p. 102). A translation is available at
<http://www.edumed.org.br/cursos/neurociencia/controversy-bernardi.pdf; and Bresadola &
Piccolino, Shocking Frogs, p. 143, among others
23 Pancaldi, Volta, pp. 14–15
24 Pancaldi, Volta, p. 20
25 Pancaldi, Volta, p. 31
26 Pancaldi, Volta, p. 91
27 Pancaldi, Volta, p. 111
28 Pancaldi, Volta, p. 111
29 Bresadola & Piccolino, Shocking Frogs, p. 152
30 Bresadola & Piccolino, Shocking Frogs, pp. 143–4
31 Bernardi, ‘The controversy’, pp. 104–5
32 Material about the French commissions from Blondel, Christine. ‘Animal Electricity in Paris: From
Initial Support, to Its Discredit and Eventual Rehabilitation’. In: Marco Bresadola and Giuliano
Pancaldi (eds), Luigi Galvani International Workshop, 1998, pp. 187–204
33 Blondel, ‘Animal Electricity’, p. 189
34 Volta, Alessandro. ‘Memoria seconda sull’elettricita animale’ (14 May 1792). Quoted in: Pera,
Marcello. The Ambiguous Frog. Trans. Jonathan Mandelbaum. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University
Press, 1992, p. 106
35 Unless otherwise referenced, the quotes from the rash of scientific papers in this section have been
taken from Bresadola & Piccolino, Shocking Frogs and Pera, The Ambiguous Frog
36 Ashcroft, Frances. The Spark of Life. London: Penguin, 2013, p. 24
37 Blondel, ‘Animal Electricity’, p. 190
38 Bernardi, ‘The controversy’, p. 107 (fn. 26)
39 Robert Campenot provides a clear and straightforward description of this experiment. Campenot,
Robert. Animal Electricity. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 2016, p. 40
40 Bernardi, ‘The controversy’, p. 103
41 Bernardi, ‘The controversy’, p. 107
1 Aldini, Giovanni. Essai théorique et expérimental sur le galvanisme, avec une série d’expériences. Faites en présence
des commissaires de l’Institut National de France, et en divers amphithéâtres Anatomiques de Londres. Paris:
Fournier Fils, 1804. Available via the Smithsonian Libraries archive at <https://library.si.edu/digital-
library/book/essaitheyorique00aldi>
2 Some sources suggest Queen Charlotte and her son, the Prince of Wales, attended but it may have
been the younger prince, Augustus Frederick, who Aldini later dedicated a book to. It seems clear
that there was at least one royal present.
3 Tarlow, Sarah, and Emma Battell Lowman. Harnessing the Power of the Criminal Corpse. London:
Palgrave Macmillan, 2018, pp. 87–114
4 McDonald, Helen. ‘Galvanising George Foster, 1803’, The University of Melbourne Archives and
Special Collections. <https://library.unimelb.edu.au/asc/whats-on/exhibitions/dark-
imaginings/gothicresearch/galvanising-george-foster,-1803>
5 Morus, Iwan Rhys. Frankenstein’s Children: Electricity, Exhibition, and Experiment in Early-Nineteenth-
Century London. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press,1998, p. 128
6 Sleigh, Charlotte. ‘Life, Death and Galvanism’. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C: Studies
in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences, vol. 29, no. 2 (1998), pp. 219–48 (p. 223)
7 There are many accounts of this experiment – mine is drawn mainly from Morus, Iwan Rhys.
Shocking Bodies: Life, Death & Electricity in Victorian England. Stroud: The History Press, 2011, pp. 34–
7. Other sources are Aldini’s personal account and the Newgate Calendar, 22 January 1803, p. 3
8 Sleigh, ‘Life, Death and Galvanism’, p. 224
9 Parent, André. ‘Giovanni Aldini: From Animal Electricity to Human Brain Stimulation’, Canadian
Journal of Neurological Sciences / Journal Canadien des Sciences Neurologiques, vol. 31, no. 4 (2004), pp. 576–
84 (p. 578)
10 Blondel, Christine. ‘Animal Electricity in Paris: From Initial Support, to Its Discredit and Eventual
Rehabilitation’. In: Marco Bresadola and Giuliano Pancaldi (eds), Luigi Galvani International Workshop,
1998, pp. 187–204 (pp. 194–5)
11 Aldini, ‘Essai Théorique’, p. vi
12 Aldini’s most detailed account of such a treatment concerns Luigi Lanzarini.
13 Carpue, Joseph. ‘An Introduction to Electricity and Galvanism; with Cases, Shewing Their Effects in
the Cure of Diseases’. London: A. Phillips, 1803, p. 86
<https://wellcomecollection.org/works/bzaj37cs/items?canvas=100>
14 Blondel, ‘Animal Electricity’, p. 197
15 Aldini, John [sic]. ‘General Views on the Application of Galvanism to Medical Purposes, Principally
in Cases of Suspended Animation’. London: Royal Society, 1819, p. 37. When publishing abroad,
Aldini had the habit of changing his first name. In the UK he anglicised to John, and in France he
became Jean.
16 Parent, ‘Giovanni Aldini’, p. 581
17 Vassalli-Eandi said in August 1802 that Aldini ‘has been obliged to acknowledge that he had not
been able to get any contractions from the heart using the electro motor of Volta’.
18 Aldini, ‘Essai Théorique’, p. 195
19 Giulio, C. ‘Report presented to the Class of the Exact Sciences of the Academy of Turin, 15th
August 1802, in Regard to the Galvanic Experiments Made by C. Vassali-Eandi, Giulio and Rossi on
the 10th and 14th of the same Month, on the Head and Trunk of three Men a short Time after their
Decapitation’. The Philosophical Magazine, vol. 15, no. 57 (1803), pp. 39–41
20 Morus, Iwan. ‘The Victorians Bequeathed Us Their Idea of an Electric Future’. Aeon, 8 August 2016
21 Aldini, ‘Essai Théorique’, p. 143–4
22 This section draws heavily from: Bertucci, Paola. ‘Therapeutic Attractions: Early Applications of
Electricity to the Art of Healing’. In: Harry Whitaker, C. U. M. Smith, and Stanley Finger (eds),
Brain, Mind, and Medicine: Essays in Eighteenth-Century Neuroscience. Boston: Springer, 2007, pp. 271–83;
Pera, Marcello, The Ambiguous Frog. Trans. Jonathan Mandelbaum. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press, 1992; and several unbeatable details from Iwan Rhys Morus’s Frankenstein’s Children
23 Pera, The Ambiguous Frog, pp. 18–25
24 Pera, The Ambiguous Frog, p. 22
25 Ashcroft, Frances. The Spark of Life. London: Penguin, 2013, pp. 290–1
26 Bertucci, ‘Therapeutic Attractions’, p. 281
27 Calculated on 23 May 2022 using the CPI Inflation Calculator.
<https://www.officialdata.org/uk/inflation>
28 Bertucci, ‘Therapeutic Attractions’, p. 281
29 Shepherd, Francis John. ‘Medical Quacks and Quackeries’, Popular Science Monthly, vol. 23 (June
1883), p. 152
30 Morus, Shocking Bodies, p. 35
31 Ochs, Sidney. A History of Nerve Functions: From Animal Spirits to Molecular Mechanisms. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press, 2004, p. 117
32 Miller, William Snow. ‘Elisha Perkins and His Metallic Tractors’. Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine,
vol. 8, no. 1 (1935), pp. 41–57 (p. 44)
33 Lord Byron. ‘English Bards and Scotch Reviewers’. Quoted in: Miller, ‘Elisha Perkins’, p. 52
34 Finger, Stanley, Marco Piccolino, and Frank W. Stahnisch. ‘Alexander von Humboldt: Galvanism,
Animal Electricity, and Self-Experimentation Part 2: The Electric Eel, Animal Electricity, and Later
Years’. Journal of the History of the Neurosciences, vol. 22, no. 4 (2013), pp. 327–52 (p. 343)
35 Finger, Stanley, and Marco Piccolino. The Shocking History of Electric Fishes. Oxford: Oxford University
Press, 2011, p. 11
36 Finger et al., ‘Alexander von Humboldt’, p. 343
37 Otis, Laura. Müller’s Lab. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2007 p. 11; see also Finger et al.,
‘Alexander von Humboldt’, p. 345
38 A picture can be seen at ‘Nobili’s large astatic galvanometer’, Museo Galileo Virtual Museum
<https://catalogue.museogalileo.it/object/NobilisLargeAstaticGalvanometer.html>
39 Verkhratsky, Alexei, and Parpura, Vladimir. ‘History of Electrophysiology and the Patch Clamp’. In:
Marzia Martina and Stefano Taverna (eds), Methods in Molecular Biology. New York: Humana Press,
2014, pp. 1–19 (p. 7). However, much of the detail about Nobili and Matteucci’s experiments comes
from Otis’s Müller’s Lab.
40 Cobb, Matthew. The Idea of the Brain: A History. London: Profile Books, 2020, p. 71
41 Finger et al., ‘Alexander von Humboldt’, p. 347 and Otis, p. 90
42 Emil du Bois-Reymond in an 1849 letter to fellow experimental physiologist Carl Ludwig,
reproduced on p. 347 of: Finger et al., ‘Alexander von Humboldt’.
43 Finger & Piccolino, The Shocking History of Electric Fishes, p. 369
44 Bresadola, Marco, and Marco Piccolino. Shocking Frogs: Galvani, Volta, and the Electric Origins of
Neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2013, p. 21
45 Finkelstein, Gabriel. ‘Emil du Bois-Reymond vs Ludimar Hermann’. Comptes rendus biologies, vol. 329,
5-6 (2006), pp. 340-7 doi:10.1016/j.crvi.2006.03.005
Part 2: Bioelectricity and the Electrome
1 Bresadola, Marco, and Marco Piccolino. Shocking Frogs: Galvani, Volta, and the Electric Origins of
Neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2013, p. 13
1 The first mention of the word ‘electrome’ can be found in an obscure 2016 paper written by the
Belgian biologist Arnold de Loof (‘The cell’s self-generated “electrome”: The biophysical essence of
the immaterial dimension of Life?’, Communicative & Integrative Biology, vol. 9,5, e1197446). This
definition did not break into wider circulation. Even before its publication, however, other
bioelectricity researchers, including Michael Levin and Min Zhao, had begun to use the word. Zhao,
in particular, has reviewed a few manuscripts using that term ‘without [consistent] definition, and
clarification. It is an evolving understanding’. The purpose of this book is to pin the word down like
a butterfly behind glass.
2 Valenstein, Elliot. The War of the Soups and the Sparks: The Discovery of Neurotransmitters and the Dispute
over how Nerves Communicate. New York: Columbia University Press, 2005, pp. 121–34
3 James, Frank. ‘Davy, Faraday, and Italian Science’. Report presented at the IX National Conference
of ‘History and Foundations of Chemistry’ (Modena, 25–27 October 2001), pp. 149–58
<https://media.accademiaxl.it/memorie/S5-VXXV-P1-2-2001/James149-158.pdf> Accessed 22
February 2021
4 Faraday, Michael. Experimental Researches in Electricity – Volume 1 [1832]. London: Richard and John
Edward Taylor, 1849. Available at <https://www.gutenberg.org/files/14986/14986-h/14986-
h.htm>
5 Ringer, Sydney, and E. A. Morshead. ‘The Influence on the Afferent Nerves of the Frog’s Leg from
the Local Application of the Chlorides, Bromides, and Iodides of Potassium, Ammonium, and
Sodium’. Journal of Anatomy and Physiology 12 (October 1877), pp. 58–72
6 Campenot, Robert, Animal Electricity. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 2016, p. 114
7 McCormick, David A. ‘Membrane Potential and Action Potential’. In: Larry Squire et al. (eds),
Fundamental Neuroscience. Oxford: Academic Press, 2013, pp. 93–116 (p. 93)
8 Hodgkin, Alan, and Andrew F. Huxley. ‘A quantitative description of membrane current and its
application to conduction and excitation in nerve’. The Journal of Physiology, vol. 117, no. 4 (1952), pp.
500–44
9 Bresadola, Marco, and Marco Piccolino. Shocking Frogs: Galvani, Volta, and the Electric Origins of
Neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2013, p. 294
10 Ramachandran, Vilayanur S. ‘The Astonishing Francis Crick’. Francis Crick memorial lecture
delivered at the Centre for the Philosophical Foundations of Science in New Delhi, India, 17
October 2004. <http://cbc.ucsd.edu/The_Astonishing_Francis_Crick.htm>
11 Schuetze, Stephen. ‘The Discovery of the Action Potential’. Trends in Neurosciences 6 (1983), pp. 164–
8. See also Lombard, Jonathan, ‘Once upon a time the cell membranes: 175 years of cell boundary
research’. Biology Direct, vol. 9, no. 32, pp. 1–35; and Finger, Stanley, and Marco Piccolino. The
Shocking History of Electric Fishes. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2011, p. 402
12 Campenot, Animal Electricity, pp. 210–11
13 Agnew, William, et al. ‘Purification of the Tetrodotoxin-Binding Component Associated with the
Voltage-Sensitive Sodium Channel from Electrophorus Electricus Electroplax Membranes’.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol. 75, no. 6 (1978), pp. 2606–10.
14 Noda, Masaharu, et al. ‘Expression of Functional Sodium Channels from Cloned CDNA’. Nature,
vol. 322, no. 6082 (1986), pp. 826–8.
15 Brenowitz, Stephan, et al. ‘Ion Channels: History, Diversity, and Impact’. Cold Spring Harbor Protocols
7 (2017), loc. pdb.top092288
<http://cshprotocols.cshlp.org/content/2017/7/pdb.top092288.long#sec-3>
16 McCormick, ‘Membrane Potential and Action Potential’, p. 103
17 Ashcroft, Frances. The Spark of Life. London: Penguin, 2013, p. 69
18 McCormick, David A. ‘Membrane Potential and Action Potential’. In: John H. Byrne, and James L.
Roberts (eds), From Molecules to Networks: An Introduction to Cellular and Molecular Neuroscience.
Amsterdam/Boston: Academic Press, 2nd edition, 2009, pp. 133–58 (p. 151)
19 Ashcroft, The Spark of Life, p. 49 and pp. 87–9
20 Barhanin, Jacques, et al. ‘New scorpion toxins with a very high affinity for Na+ channels.
Biochemical characterization and use for the purification of Na+ channels’. Journal de Physiologie, vol.
79, no. 4 (1984), pp. 304–8
21 Kullmann, Dimitri M. ‘The Neuronal Channelopathies’. Brain, vol. 125, no. 6 (2002), pp. 1177–95
22 Fozzard, Harry. ‘Cardiac Sodium and Calcium Channels: A History of Excitatory Currents’.
Cardiovascular Research, vol. 55, no. 1 (2002), pp. 1–8
23 Sherman, Harry G., et al. ‘Mechanistic insight into heterogeneity of trans-plasma membrane electron
transport in cancer cell types’. Biochimica et Biophysica Acta – Bioenergetics, 1860/8 (2019), pp. 628–39
24 Lund, Elmer. Bioelectric Fields and Growth. Austin: University of Texas Press, 1947
25 Prindle A, Liu J, Asally M, Ly S, Garcia-Ojalvo J, Süel GM. ‘Ion channels enable electrical
communication in bacterial communities’. Nature. (2015) Nov 5;527(7576):59-63. doi:
10.1038/nature15709. Epub 2015 Oct 21. PMID: 26503040; PMCID: PMC4890463
26 Brand, Alexandra et al. ‘Hyphal Orientation of Candida albicans Is Regulated by a Calcium-
Dependent Mechanism’. Current Biology, 17, (2007), pp. 347–352
27 Davies, Paul. The Demon in the Machine. London: Allen Lane, 2019, p. 110
28 Anderson, Paul A., and Robert M. Greenberg. ‘Phylogeny of ion channels: clues to structure and
function’. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part B: Biochemistry and Molecular Biology, vol. 129, no. 1
(2001), pp. 17–28. doi: 10.1016/s1096-4959(01)00376-1
29 Liebeskind, B. J., D. M. Hillis, and H. H. Zakon. ‘Convergence of ion channel genome content in
early animal evolution’. Proceedings of the National Academies of Science 112 (2015), E846–E851
1 Besterman, Edwin, and Creese, Richard. ‘Waller – pioneer of electrocardiography’. British Heart
Journal, vol. 42, no. 1 (1979), pp. 61–4 (p. 63)
2 Acierno, Louis. ‘Augustus Desire Waller’. Clinical Cardiology, vol. 23, no. 4 (2000), pp. 307–9 (p. 308)
3 Harrington, Kat. ‘Heavy browed savants unbend’. Royal Society blogs, 14 July 2016. Retrieved from
the Internet Archive 21 September 2021
<https://web.archive.org/web/20191024235429/http://blogs.royalsociety.org/history-of-
science/2016/07/04/heavy-browed/>
4 Waller, Augustus D. ‘A Demonstration on Man of Electromotive Changes accompanying the Heart’s
Beat’. The Journal of Physiology, vol. 8 (1887), pp. 229–34
5 Campenot, Robert. Animal Electricity. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 2016, p. 269
6 Burchell, Howard. ‘A Centennial Note on Waller and the First Human Electrocardiogram’. The
American Journal of Cardiology, vol. 59, no. 9 (1987), pp. 979–83 (p. 979)
7 AlGhatrif, Majd, and Joseph Lindsay. ‘A Brief Review: History to Understand Fundamentals of
Electrocardiography’. Journal of Community Hospital Internal Medicine Perspectives, vol. 2 no. 1 (2012), loc.
14383
8 Ashcroft, Frances. The Spark of Life. London: Penguin, 2013, p. 146
9 Campenot, Animal Electricity, pp. 272–4
10 Aquilina, Oscar. ‘A brief history of cardiac pacing’. Images in Paediatric Cardiology, vol. 8, no. 2 (April
2006), pp. 17–81 (Fig. 16)
11 Rowbottom, Margaret, and Charles Susskind. Electricity and Medicine: History of Their Interaction.
London: Macmillan, 1984, p. 248
12 Rowbottom & Susskind, Electricity and Medicine, p. 249
13 Rowbottom & Susskind, Electricity and Medicine, p. 249
14 Emery, Gene. ‘Nuclear pacemaker still energized after 34 years’, Reuters, 19 December 2007
<https://www.reuters.com/article/us-heart-pacemaker-idUSN1960427320071219>
15 Norman, J. C. et al. ‘Implantable nuclear-powered cardiac pacemakers’. New England Journal of
Medicine, vol. 283, no. 22 (1970), pp. 1203–6. doi: 10.1056/NEJM197011262832206
16 Roy, O. Z., and R. W. Wehnert. ‘Keeping the heart alive with a biological battery’. Electronics, vol. 39,
no. 6 (1966), pp. 105–7. Also see: <https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/BF02629834>
17 Greatbatch, Wilson. The Making of the Pacemaker: Celebrating a Lifesaving Invention. Amherst:
Prometheus Books, 2000, p. 23
18 Tashiro, Hiroyuki, et al. ‘Direct Neural Interface’. In: Marko B. Popovic (ed.), Biomechatronics. Oxford:
Academic Press, 2019, pp. 139–74
19 Greatbatch, The Making of the Pacemaker, p. 23
1 Hamzelou, Jessica. ‘$100 million project to make intelligence-boosting brain implant’, New Scientist,
20 October 2016 <https://www.newscientist.com/article/2109868-100-million-project-to-make-
intelligence-boosting-brain-implant/>
2 McKelvey, Cynthia. ‘The Neuroscientist Who’s Building a Better Memory for Humans’, Wired, 1
December 2016 <https://www.wired.com/2016/12/neuroscientist-whos-building-better-memory-
humans/>
3 Johnson, Bryan. ‘The Urgency of Cognitive Improvement’, Medium, 14 June 2017
<https://medium.com/future-literacy/the-urgency-of-cognitive-improvement-72f5043ca1fc>
4 Campenot, Robert, Animal Electricity. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 2016, pp. 110–11
5 Finger, Stanley. Minds Behind the Brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2005, pp 243–7. See also
Ashcroft, Frances. The Spark of Life. London: Penguin, 2013, ch. 3
6 Garson, Justin. ‘The Birth of Information in the Brain: Edgar Adrian and the Vacuum Tube’. Science
in Context, vol. 28, no. 1 (2015), pp. 31–52 (pp. 40–2)
7 Finger, Minds, p. 249
8 Finger, Minds, p. 250
9 Finger, Minds, p. 250
10 Garson, ‘The Birth’, p. 46
11 Finger, Minds, p. 250
12 Adrian, E. D. The Physical Background of Perception. Quoted in Cobb, Matthew. The Idea of the Brain: A
History. London: Profile Books, 2020, p. 186
13 Borck, Cornelius. ‘Recording the Brain at Work: The Visible, the Readable, and the Invisible in
Electroencephalography’. Journal of the History of the Neurosciences 17 (2008), pp. 367–79 (p. 371)
14 Millett, David. ‘Hans Berger: From Psychic Energy to the EEG’. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine,
vol. 44, no. 4 (2001), pp. 522–42 (p. 523)
15 Ginzberg, quoted in Millet, ‘Hans Berger’, p. 524
16 Millet, ‘Hans Berger’, p. 537
17 Cobb, The Idea of the Brain, p. 170
18 Millet, ‘Hans Berger’, p. 539
19 Borck, ‘Recording’, p. 369
20 Borck, ‘Recording’, p. 368
21 Borck, Cornelius, and Ann M. Hentschel. Brainwaves: A Cultural History of Electroencephalography.
London: Routledge, 2018, p. 110
22 Borck & Hentschel, Brainwaves, p. 109
23 Borck & Hentschel, Brainwaves, p. 115
24 Collura, Thomas. ‘History and Evolution of Electroencephalo-graphic Instruments and Techniques’.
Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology, vol. 10, no. 4 (1993), pp. 476–504 (p. 498)
25 Marsh, Allison. ‘Meet the Roomba’s Ancestor: The Cybernetic Tortoise’, IEEE Spectrum, 28
February 2020 <https://spectrum.ieee.org/meet-roombas-ancestor-cybernetic-tortoise>
26 Cobb, The Idea of the Brain, p. 190
27 Hodgkin, Alan. ‘Edgar Douglas Adrian, Baron Adrian of Cambridge. 30 November 1889–4 August
1977’. Biographical Memoirs of Fellows of the Royal Society 25 (1979), pp. 1–73 (p. 19)
28 Tatu, Laurent. ‘Edgar Adrian (1889–1977) and Shell Shock Electrotherapy: A Forgotten History?’.
European Neurology, vol. 79, nos 1–2 (2018), pp. 106–7
29 Underwood, Emil. ‘A Sense of Self ’. Science, vol. 372, no. 6547 (2021), pp. 1142–5 (pp. 1142–3)
30 Olds, James. ‘Pleasure Centers in the Brain’. Scientific American, vol. 195 (1956), pp. 105–17; Olds,
James. ‘Self-Stimulation of the Brain’. Science 127 (1958), pp. 315–24
31 Moan, Charles, and Robert G. Heath. ‘Septal Stimulation for the Initiation of Heterosexual Behavior
in a Homosexual Male’. In: Wolpe, Joseph, and Leo J. Reyna (eds), Behavior Therapy in Psychiatric
Practice. New York: Pergamon Press, 1976, pp. 109–16
32 Giordano, James (ed.). Neurotechnology. Boca Raton: CRC Press, 2012, p. 151
33 Frank, Lone. ‘Maverick or monster? The controversial pioneer of brain zapping’, New Scientist, 27
March 2018 <https://www.newscientist.com/article/mg23731710-700-maverick-or-monster-the-
controversial-pioneer-of-brain-zapping/>
34 Blackwell, Barry. ‘José Manuel Rodriguez Delgado’. Neuropsychopharmacology, vol. 37, no. 13 (2012),
pp. 2883–4.
35 The photo has been widely reproduced, but can be found in Marzullo, Timothy. ‘The Missing
Manuscript of Dr. José Delgado’s Radio Controlled Bulls’. JUNE, vol. 15, no. 2 (Spring 2017), pp.
29–35
36 Osmundsen, John. ‘Matador with a radio stops wired bull: modified behavior in animals subject of
brain study’, New York Times, 17 May 1965
37 Horgan, John. ‘Tribute to José Delgado, Legendary and Slightly Scary Pioneer of Mind Control’.
Scientific American, 25 September 2017
38 Gardner, John. ‘A History of Deep Brain Stimulation: Technological Innovation and the Role of
Clinical Assessment Tools’. Social Studies of Science, vol. 43, no. 5 (2013), pp. 707–28 (p. 710)
39 Schwalb, Jason M., and Clement Hamani. ‘The History and Future of Deep Brain Stimulation’.
Neurotherapeutics, vol. 5, no. 1 (2008), pp. 3–13
40 Gardner, ‘A History’, p. 719
41 Lozano, A. M., N. Lipsman, H. Bergman, et al. ‘Deep brain stimulation: current challenges and
future directions’. Nature Reviews Neurology 15 (2019), pp. 148–60
<https://www.nature.com/articles/s41582-018-0128-2>
Nuttin, Bart et al. ‘Electrical Stimulation in Anterior Limbs of Internal Capsules in Patients with
42 Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder’. The Lancet, vol. 354, no. 9189 (1999), p. 1526
43 Ridgway, Andy. ‘Deep brain stimulation: A wonder treatment pushed too far?’, New Scientist, 21
October 2015 <https://www.newscientist.com/article/mg22830440-500-deep-brain-stimulation-a-
wonder-treatment-pushed-too-far/>
44 Sturm, V., et al. ‘DBS in the basolateral amygdala improves symptoms of autism and related self-
injurious behavior: a case report and hypothesis on the pathogenesis of the disorder’. Frontiers in
Neuroscience, vol. 6, no. 341 (2013), doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00341
45 Formolo, D. A., et al. ‘Deep Brain Stimulation for Obesity: A Review and Future Directions’.
Frontiers in Neuroscience, vol. 13, no. 323 (2019), doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00323; Wu, H., et al. ‘Deep-
brain stimulation for anorexia nervosa’. World Neurosurgery 80 (2013), doi:
10.1016/j.wneu.2012.06.039
46 Baguley, David, et al. ‘Tinnitus’. The Lancet, vol. 382, no. 9904 (2013), pp. 1600–7; Luigjes, J., van den
Brink, W., Feenstra, M., et al. ‘Deep brain stimulation in addiction: a review of potential brain
targets’. Molecular Psychiatry 17 (2012), pp. 572–83 <https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2011.114>; Fuss, J.,
et al. ‘Deep brain stimulation to reduce sexual drive’. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, vol. 40, no. 6
(2015) pp. 429–31
47 Satellite meeting of Society for Neuroscience, San Diego, 2018. Mayberg also spoke about it at the
Brain & Behaviour Research Foundation: ‘Deep Brain Stimulation for Treatment-Resistant
Depression: A Progress Report’, Brain & Behaviour Research Foundation YouTube channel, 16
October 2019 <https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=X86wBj1tjiA>
48 Mayberg, Helen, et al. ‘Deep Brain Stimulation for Treatment-Resistant Depression’. Neuron, vol. 45,
no. 5 (2005), pp. 651–60
49 Dobbs, David. ‘Why Deep-Brain Stimulation for Depression Didn’t Pass Clinical Trials’, The Atlantic,
17 April 2018 <https://www.theatlantic.com/science/archive/2018/04/zapping-peoples-brains-
didnt-cure-their-depression-until-it-did/558032/>
50 ‘BROADEN Trial of DBS for Treatment-Resistant Depression No Better than Sham’, The
Neurocritic blog, 10 October 2017 <https://neurocritic.blogspot.com/2017/10/broaden-trial-of-
dbs-for-treatment.html>
51 ‘The Remote Control Brain’, Invisibilia, NPR, first broadcast 29 March 2019
<https://www.npr.org/2019/03/28/707639854/the-remote-control-brain>
52 Cyron, Donatus. ‘Mental Side Effects of Deep Brain Stimulation (DBS) for Movement Disorders:
The Futility of Denial’. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience 10 (2016), pp. 1–4
<https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnint.2016.00017/full>
53 Mantione, Mariska, et al. ‘A Case of Musical Preference for Johnny Cash Following Deep Brain
Stimulation of the Nucleus Accumbens’. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, vol. 8, no. 152 (2014), doi:
10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00152
54 Florin, Esther, et al. ‘Subthalamic Stimulation Modulates Self-Estimation of Patients with
Parkinson’s Disease and Induces Risk-Seeking Behaviour’. Brain, vol. 136, no. 11 (2013), pp. 3271–
81.
55 Shen, Helen H., ‘Can Deep Brain Stimulation Find Success beyond Parkinson’s Disease?’. Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences, vol. 116, no. 11 (2019), pp. 4764–6
56 Müller, Eli J., and Peter A. Robinson. ‘Quantitative Theory of Deep Brain Stimulation of the
Subthalamic Nucleus for the Suppression of Pathological Rhythms in Parkinson’s Disease’, ed. by
Saad Jbabdi, PLOS Computational Biology, vol. 14, no. 5 (2018), e1006217. See also Kisely, Steve, et al.
‘A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Deep Brain Stimulation for Depression’. Depression and
Anxiety, vol. 35, no. 5 (2018), pp. 468–80
57 Crick, Francis. The Astonishing Hypothesis: The Scientific Search for the Soul. New York: Scribner; London:
Maxwell Macmillan International, 1994, p. 10, see also pp. 182-4
58 Crick, The Astonishing Hypothesis, p. 3. For more on consciousness, a wonderful resource is Chapter 15
of Matthew Cobb’s The Idea of the Brain
59 Gerstner, Wulfram, et al. ‘Neural Codes: Firing Rates and Beyond’. Proceedings of the National Academy
of Sciences, vol. 94, no. 24 (1997), pp. 12740–1
<https://www.pnas.org/doi/epdf/10.1073/pnas.94.24.12740>
60 See Buzsöki, Gyárgy. Rhythms of the Brain, New York: Oxford University Press, 2011
61 Kellis, Spencer, et al. ‘Decoding Spoken Words Using Local Field Potentials Recorded from the
Cortical Surface’. Journal of Neural Engineering, vol. 7, no. 5 (2010), 056007
62 Martin, Richard. ‘Mind Control’, Wired, 1 March 2005 <https://www.wired.com/2005/03/brain-
3/>
63 Martin, ‘Mind Control’, 2005
64 Bouton, Chad. ‘Reconnecting a paralyzed man’s brain to his body through technology’, TEDx Talks
YouTube channel, 25 November 2014 <https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=BPI7XWPSbS4>
65 Bouton, C., Shaikhouni, A., Annetta, N., et al. ‘Restoring cortical control of functional movement in
a human with quadriplegia’. Nature 533 (2016), pp. 247–50 <https://doi.org/10.1038/nature17435>
66 Geddes, Linda. ‘First paralysed person to be “reanimated” offers neuroscience insights’, Nature, 13
April 2016 <https://doi.org/10.1038/nature.2016.19749>
67 Geddes, Linda. ‘Pioneering brain implant restores paralysed man’s sense of touch’, Nature, 13
October 2016 <https://doi.org/10.1038/nature.2016>
68 Flesher, S. N., et al. ‘Intracortical microstimulation of human somatosensory cortex’. Science
Translational Medicine. vol. 8, no. 361 (2016), doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.aaf8083
69 Berger, T. W., et al. ‘A cortical neural prosthesis for restoring and enhancing memory’. Journal of
Neural Engineering, vol. 8, no. 4 (2011), doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/8/4/046017
70 Frank, Loren. ‘How to Make an Implant That Improves the Brain’, MIT Technology Review, 9 May
2013 <https://www.technologyreview.com/2013/05/09/178498/how-to-make-a-cognitive-
neuroprosthetic/>
71 Hampson, Robert E., et al. ‘Facilitation and Restoration of Cognitive Function in Primate Prefrontal
Cortex by a Neuroprosthesis That Utilizes Minicolumn-Specific Neural Firing’. Journal of Neural
Engineering, vol. 9, no. 5 (2012), 056012
72 Strickland, Eliza. ‘DARPA Project Starts Building Human Memory Prosthetics’, IEEE Spectrum, 27
August 2014 <https://spectrum.ieee.org/darpa-project-starts-building-human-memory-
prosthetics>
73 McKelvey, ‘The Neuroscientist’, 2016
74 Ganzer, Patrick, et al. ‘Restoring the Sense of Touch Using a Sensorimotor Demultiplexing Neural
Interface’. Cell, vol. 181, no. 4 (2020) pp. 763–73
75 ‘Reconnecting the Brain After Paralysis Using Machine Learning’, Medium, 21 September 2020
<https://medium.com/mathworks/reconnecting-the-brain-after-paralysis-using-machine-learning-
1a134c622c5d>
76 Bryan, Carla, and Ivan Rios (eds). Brain–machine Interfaces: Uses and Developments. New York: Novinka,
2018
77 Chad Bouton is working on the solution to the ‘take home’ problem. Bouton, Chad. ‘Brain Implants
and Wearables Let Paralyzed People Move Again’, IEEE Spectrum, 26 January 2021
<https://spectrum.ieee.org/brain-implants-and-wearables-let-paralyzed-people-move-again>
78 Engber, Daniel. ‘The Neurologist Who Hacked His Brain – And Almost Lost His Mind’. Wired, 26
January 2016
79 Jun, James J., et al. ‘Fully Integrated Silicon Probes for High-Density Recording of Neural Activity’.
Nature, vol. 551, no. 7679 (2017), pp. 232–6
80 Strickland, Eliza. ‘4 Steps to Turn “Neural Dust” Into a Medical Reality’, IEEE Spectrum, 21
October 2016 <https://spectrum.ieee.org/4-steps-to-turn-neural-dust-into-a-medical-reality>
81 Lee, Jihun, et al. ‘Neural Recording and Stimulation Using Wireless Networks of Microimplants’.
Nature Electronics, vol. 4, no. 8 (2021), pp. 604–14
82 ‘Brain chips will become “more common than pacemakers”, says investor, as startup raises $10m’,
The Stack, 19 May 2021 <https://thestack.technology/blackrock-neurotech-brain-machine-
interfaces-peter-thiel/>
83 Ghose, Carrie. ‘Ohio State researcher says Battelle brain-computer interface for paralysis could save
$7B in annual home-care costs’, Columbus Business First, 10 October 2019
<https://www.bizjournals.com/columbus/news/2019/10/10/ohio-state-researcher-saysbattelle-
brain-computer.html>
84 Regalado, Antonio. ‘Thought Experiment’, MIT Technology Review, 17 June 2014
<https://www.technologyreview.com/2014/06/17/172276/the-thought-experiment/>
1 Bowen, Chuck. ‘Nerve Repair Innovation Gives Man Hope’, Spinal Cord Injury Information Pages,
4 July 2007 <https://www.sci-info-pages.com/news/2007/07/nerve-repair-innovation-gives-man-
hope/>
2 Wallack, Todd. ‘Sense of urgency for spinal device’, Boston Globe. 18 September 2007
<http://archive.boston.com/business/globe/articles/2007/09/18/sense_of_urgency_for_spinal_d
evice/>
3 Per Debra Bohnert, Richard Borgens’ lab assistant from 1986 to 2019, in a telephone interview with
the author.
4 Jaffe, L. F., and M.-m. Poo. ‘Neurites grow faster towards the cathode than the anode in a steady
field’. Journal of Experimental Zoology 209 (1979), pp. 115–28
5 Ingvar, Sven. ‘Reaction of cells to the galvanic current in tissue cultures’. Experimental Biology and
Medicine, vol. 17, issue 8 (1920)
6 Bishop, Chris. ‘The Briks of Denton and Dallas TX’, Garage Hangover, 18 October 2007
<https://garagehangover.com/briks-denton-dallas/>
7 Pithoud, Kelsey. ‘Ex-rocker turns to research’, The Purdue Exponent, 17 September 2003
<https://web.archive.org/web/20151216205707/https://www.purdueexponent.org/campus/article
_73f34375-9059-5273-b6a8-8d9577c74b5d.html>
8 Bishop, ‘The Briks’, 2007
9 Comment by Johnny Young on Bishop, ‘The Briks’, 2007. 25 January 2019 at 11.33 a.m.
10 Kolsti, Nancy. ‘This is . . . Spinal Research’, The North Texan Online, Fall 2001
<https://northtexan.unt.edu/archives/f01/spinal.htm>
11 Hinkle, Laura, et al. ‘The direction of growth of differentiating neurones and myoblasts from frog
embryos in an applied electric field’. The Journal of Physiology, 314 (1981), pp. 121–35
12 McCaig, Colin. ‘Epithelial Physiology, Ovarian Follicles, Nerve Growth Cones, Vibrating Probes,
Wound Healing, and Cluster Headache: Staggering Steps on a Route Map to Bioelectricity’.
Bioelectricity, vol. 2, no. 4 (2020), pp. 411–17 (p. 412)
13 Borgens, Richard, et al. ‘Bioelectricity and Regeneration’. BioScience, vol. 29, no. 8 (1979), pp. 468–74
14 Borgens, Richard, et al. ‘Large and persistent electrical currents enter the transected lamprey spinal
cord’. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol. 77, no. 2 (1980), pp. 1209–13
15 Borgens, Richard B., Andrew R. Blight and M. E. McGinnis. ‘Behavioral Recovery Induced by
Applied Electric Fields After Spinal Cord Hemisection in Guinea Pig’. Science, vol. 238, no. 4825
(1987), pp. 366–9
16 Kleitman, Naomi. ‘Under one roof: the Miami Project to Cure Paralysis model for spinal cord injury
research’. Neuroscientist, vol. 7, no. 3 (2001), pp. 192–201
Borgens, Richard B., et al. ‘Effects of Applied Electric Fields on Clinical Cases of Complete
17
Paraplegia in Dogs’. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, vol. 5, no.5-6 (1993), pp. 305–22
18 ‘Electrical stimulation helps dogs with spinal injuries’, Purdue News, 21 July 1993
<https://www.purdue.edu/uns/html3month/1990-95/930721.Borgens.dogstudy.html>
19 Orr, Richard. ‘Research On Dogs’ Spinal Cord Injuries May Lead To Help For Humans’, Chicago
Tribune, 20 November 1995 <https://www.chicagotribune.com/news/ct-xpm-1995-11-20-
9511200137-story.html>
20 ‘Purdue/IU partnership in paralysis research’, Purdue News Service, 28 July 1999
<https://www.purdue.edu/uns/html4ever/1999/990730.Borgens.institute.html>
21 ‘Human Trial for Spinal Injury Treatment Launched by Purdue, IU’, Purdue News Service,
December 2000 <https://www.purdue.edu/uns/html4ever/001120.Borgens.SpinalTrial.html>
22 Callahan, Rick. ‘Two universities launch clinical trial for paralysis patients’, Middletown Press, 12
December 2000 <https://www.middletownpress.com/news/article/Two-universities-launch-
clinical-trial-for-11940807.php>
23 This quote is taken from an edition of Purdue School of Veterinary Medicine’s self-published
newsletter seen by the author: ‘Tales from the Vet Clinic: Yukon overcomes his chilling ordeal!’,
Synapses, Fall 2020
24 ‘Device to Aid Paralysis Victims to Get Test’, Los Angeles Times, 13 December 2000
25 Bowen, C. ‘Nerve Repair Innovation Gives Man Hope’, Indianapolis Star, 4 July 2007
<http://www.indystar.com/apps/pbcs.dll/article?
AID=/20070703/BUSINESS/707030350/1003/BUSINESS>
26 ravn, Karen. ‘In spinal research, pets lead the way’, Los Angeles Times, 9 April 2007
<https://www.latimes.com/archives/la-xpm-2007-apr-09-he-labside9-story.html>
27 ‘Implanted device offers new sensation’, The Engineer, 11 January 2005
<https://www.theengineer.co.uk/implanted-device-offers-new-sensation/>
28 ‘Cyberkinetics to acquire Andara Life Science for $4.5M’, Boston Business Journal, 13 February 2006
<https://www.bizjournals.com/boston/blog/mass-high-tech/2006/02/cyberkinetics-to-acquire-
andara-life-science.html>
29 Cyberkinetics press release, 28 September 2006
<https://www.purdue.edu/uns/html3month/2006/060928CyberkineticsAward.pdf>
30 Robinson, Kenneth, and Peter Cormie. ‘Electric Field Effects on Human Spinal Injury: Is There a
Basis in the In Vitro Studies?’. Developmental Neurobiology, vol. 68, no. 2 (2008), pp. 274–80
31 Wallack, ‘Sense of urgency’, 2007
32 Shapiro, Scott. ‘A Review of Oscillating Field Stimulation to Treat Human Spinal Cord Injury’. World
Neurosurgery, vol. 81/5–6 (2014), pp. 830–5
33 Bowman, Lee. ‘Study on dogs yields hope in human paralysis treatment’, Seattle Post-Intelligencer, 3
August 2004
34 Li, Jianming. ‘Oscillating Field Electrical Stimulator (OFS) for Regeneration of the Spinal Cord’,
2017 entry to the Create the Future Design Contest
<https://contest.techbriefs.com/2017/entries/medical/8251>
35 Li, Jianming. ‘Weak Direct Current (DC) Electric Fields as a Therapy for Spinal Cord Injuries:
Review and Advancement of the Oscillating Field Stimulator (OFS)’. Neurosurgical Review, vol. 42, no.
4 (2019), pp. 825–34
36 Willyard, Cassandra. ‘How a Revolutionary Technique Got People with Spinal-Cord Injuries Back on
Their Feet’. Nature, vol. 572, no. 7767 (2019), pp. 20–5
37 Even chemical and physical factors like contact inhibition release and population pressure.
38 McCaig, Colin D., et al. ‘Controlling Cell Behavior Electrically: Current Views and Future Potential’.
Physiological Reviews, vol. 85, no. 3 (2005), pp. 943–78
39 ‘Direct-current (DC) electric fields are present in all developing and regenerating animal tissues, yet
their existence and potential impact on tissue repair and development are largely ignored,’ they wrote
in ‘Controlling Cell Behavior Electrically’.
40 Reid, Brian, et al. ‘Wound Healing in Rat Cornea: The Role of Electric Currents’. The FASEB Journal,
vol. 19, no. 3 (2005), pp. 379–86
41 Hagins, W.A., et al. ‘Dark Current and Photocurrent in Retinal Rods’. Biophysical Journal, vol. 10, no. 5
(1970), pp. 380–412
42 Song, Bing, et al. ‘Electrical Cues Regulate the Orientation and Frequency of Cell Division and the
Rate of Wound Healing in Vivo’. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol. 99, no. 21 (2002),
pp. 13577–82
43 Leppik, Liudmila, et al. ‘Electrical Stimulation in Bone Tissue Engineering Treatments’. European
Journal of Trauma and Emergency Surgery, vol. 46, no. 2 (2020), pp. 231–44
44 Zhao, Min, et al. ‘Electrical Signals Control Wound Healing through Phosphatidylinositol-3-OH
Kinase-γ and PTEN’. Nature, vol. 442, no. 7101 (2006), pp. 457–60.
45 See National Institutes for Health, ‘A Clinical Trial of Dermacorder for Detecting Malignant Skin
Lesions’, 17 November 2009 <https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT01014819>
46 Nuccitelli, R., et al. ‘The electric field near human skin wounds declines with age and provides a
noninvasive indicator of wound healing’. Wound Repair and Regeneration, vol. 19, no. 5 (2011), pp. 645–
55
47 Stephens, Tim. ‘Bioelectronic device achieves unprecedented control of cell membrane voltage’, UC
Santa Cruz News Center, 24 September 2020
<https://news.ucsc.edu/2020/09/bioelectronics.html>
48 Ershad, F., A. Thukral., J. Yue, et al. ‘Ultra-conformal drawn-on-skin electronics for multifunctional
motion artifact-free sensing and point-of-care treatment’. Nature Communications, vol. 11, no. 3823
(2020), doi: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-17619-1
1 Levin, Michael. ‘What Bodies Think About: Bioelectric Computation Beyond the Nervous System as
Inspiration for New Machine Learning Platforms’. The Thirty-second Annual Conference on
Neural Information Processing Systems (NIPS). Palais des Congrès de Montréal, Montréal, Canada.
4 December 2018, slide 49
<https://media.neurips.cc/Conferences/NIPS2018/Slides/Levin_bioelectric_computation.pdf>;
see also Pullar, Christine E. (ed.). The Physiology of Bioelectricity in Development, Tissue Regeneration and
Cancer. Boca Raton: CRC Press, 2011, p. 69
2 Sampogna, Gianluca, et al. ‘Regenerative Medicine: Historical Roots and Potential Strategies in
Modern Medicine’. Journal of Microscopy and Ultrastructure, vol. 3, no. 3 (2015), pp. 101–7 (p. 101)
3 Power, Carl, and John E. J. Rasko. ‘The stem cell revolution isn’t what you think it is’, New Scientist,
29 September 2021 <https://www.newscientist.com/article/mg25133542-600-the-stem-cell-
revolution-isnt-what-you-think-it-is>
4 Burr, Harold Saxton, et al. ‘A Vacuum Tube Micro-Voltmeter for the Measurement of Bio-Electric
Phenomena’. The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine, vol. 9, no. 1 (1936), pp. 65–76. It is pictured on
the journal’s website alongside the article:
<https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2601500/figure/F1/>
5 Burr, Harold Saxton. Blueprint for Immortality: The Electric Patterns of Life. Essex: Neville Spearman
Publishers, 1972, p. 48
6 Burr, Harold Saxton, L. K. Musselman, Dorothy Barton, and Naomi B. Kelly. ‘Bio-Electric
Correlates of Human Ovulation’. The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine, vol. 10, no. 2 (1937), pp. 155–
60
7 Burr, Harold Saxton, R. T. Hill, and E. Allen. ‘Detection of Ovulation in the Intact Rabbit’.
Proceedings of the Society for Experimental Biology and Medicine, vol. 33, no. 1 (1935), pp. 109–11
8 Burr, Blueprint, p. 50
9 Burr, Blueprint, p. 51
10 Langman, Louis, and H. S. Burr. ‘Electrometric Timing of Human Ovulation’. American Journal of
Obstetrics and Gynecology, vol. 44, no. 2 (1942), pp. 223–9
11 ‘Medicine: Yale Proof ’, Time, 11 October 1937
<http://content.time.com/time/subscriber/article/0,33009,770949-1,00.html>
12 There is a diagram on p. 156 of Burr et al., ‘Bio-Electric Correlates’.
<https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2601785/?page=2>
13 Altmann, Margaret. ‘Interrelations of the Sex Cycle and the Behavior of the Sow’. Journal of
Comparative Psychology, vol. 31, no. 3 (1941), pp. 481–98
14 ‘Dr. John Rock (1890–1984)’, PBS American Experience
<https://www.pbs.org/wgbh/americanexperience/features/pill-dr-john-rock-1890-1984/>
15 Snodgrass, James, et al. ‘The Validity Of “Ovulation Potentials”’. American Journal of Physiology – Legacy
Content, vol. 140, no. 3 (1943), pp. 394–415
16 Su, Hsiu-Wei, et al. ‘Detection of Ovulation, a Review of Currently Available Methods’. Bioengineering
& Translational Medicine, vol. 2, no. 3 (2017), pp. 238–46
17 Herzberg, M., et al. ‘The Cyclic Variation of Sodium Chloride Content in the Mucus of the Cervix
Uteri’. Fertility and Sterility, vol. 15, no. 6 (1964), pp. 684–94
18 Burr, Harold Saxton, and L. K. Musselman. ‘Bio-Electric Phenomena Associated with
Menstruation’. The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine, vol. 9, no. 2 (1936), pp. 155–8
19 Tosti, Elisabetta. ‘Electrical Events during Gamete Maturation and Fertilization in Animals and
Humans’. Human Reproduction Update, vol. 10, no. 1 (2004), pp. 53–65
20 Van Blerkom, J. ‘Domains of High-Polarized and Low-Polarized Mitochondria May Occur in Mouse
and Human Oocytes and Early Embryos’. Human Reproduction, vol. 17, no. 2 (2002), pp. 393–406
21 Trebichalská, Zuzana and Zuzana Holubcová. ‘Perfect Date—the Review of Current Research into
Molecular Bases of Mammalian Fertilization’. Journal of Assisted Reproduction and Genetics, vol. 37, no. 2
(2020), pp. 243–56
22 Stein, Paula, et al. ‘Modulators of Calcium Signalling at Fertilization’. Open Biology, vol. 10, no. 7
(2020), loc. 200118
23 Campbell, Keith H., et al. ‘Sheep cloned by nuclear transfer from a cultured cell line’. Nature, vol.
380, article 6569 (1996), pp. 64–6 (p. 64)
24 Zimmer, Carl. ‘Growing Left, Growing Right’, The New York Times, 3 June 2013
<https://www.nytimes.com/2013/06/04/science/growing-left-growing-right-how-a-body-breaks-
symmetry.html>
25 Some have problems with breathing normally and fertility.
26 See Nuccitelli, Richard, Ionic Currents In Development. New York: International Society of
Developmental Biologists, 1986
27 Tosti, E., R. Boni, and A. Gallo. ‘Ion currents in embryo development’. Birth Defects Research Part C
108 (2016), pp. 6–18, doi: 10.1002/bdrc.21125
28 Adams, Dany S., and Michael Levin. ‘General Principles for Measuring Resting Membrane Potential
and Ion Concentration Using Fluorescent Bioelectricity Reporters’. Cold Spring Harbor Protocols,
2012/4 (2012)
29 Cone, Clarence, and Charlotte M. Cone. ‘Induction of Mitosis in Mature Neurons in Central
Nervous System by Sustained Depolarization’. Science, vol. 192, no. 4235 (1976), pp. 155–8
30 Knight, Kalimah Redd, and Patrick Collins, ‘The Face of a Frog: Time-lapse Video Reveals Never-
Before-Seen Bioelectric Pattern’, Tufts University press release, 18 July 2011
<https://now.tufts.edu/2011/07/18/face-frog-time-lapse-video-reveals-never-seen-bioelectric-
pattern>
31 Vandenberg, Laura N., et al. ‘V-ATPase-Dependent Ectodermal Voltage and Ph Regionalization Are
Required for Craniofacial Morphogenesis’. Developmental Dynamics, vol. 240, no. 8 (2011), pp. 1889–
904
32 Adams, Dany Spencer, et al. ‘Bioelectric Signalling via Potassium Channels: A Mechanism for
Craniofacial Dysmorphogenesis in KCNJ2-Associated Andersen-Tawil Syndrome: K + -Channels in
Craniofacial Development’. The Journal of Physiology, vol. 594, no. 12 (2016), pp. 3245–70
33 Moody, William J., et al. ‘Development of ion channels in early embryos’. Journal of Neurobiology 22
(1991) pp. 674–84
34 Rovner, Sophie. ‘Recipes for Limb Renewal’, Chemical & Engineering News, 2 August 2010
<https://pubsapp.acs.org/cen/science/88/8831sci1.html>
35 Pai, Vaibhav P., et al. ‘Transmembrane Voltage Potential Controls Embryonic Eye Patterning in
Xenopus Laevis’. Development, vol. 139, no. 2 (2012), pp. 313–23
36 Malinowski, Paul T., et al. ‘Mechanics dictate where and how freshwater planarians fission’. PNAS,
vol. 114, no. 41 (2017), pp. 10888–93 <www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1700762114>
37 Hall, Danielle. ‘Brittle Star Splits’, Smithsonian Ocean, January 2020 <https://ocean.si.edu/ocean-
life/invertebrates/brittle-star-splits>
38 Levin, Michael. ‘Reading and Writing the Morphogenetic Code: Foundational White Paper of the
Allen Discovery Center at Tufts University’, p. 2 <https://allencenter.tufts.edu/wp-
content/uploads/Whitepaper.pdf>
39 Kolata, Gina. ‘Surgery on Fetuses Reveals They Heal Without Scars’, The New York Times, 16 August
1988 <https://www.nytimes.com/1988/08/16/science/surgery-on-fetuses-reveals-they-heal-
without-scars.html>
40 Barbuzano, Javier. ‘Understanding How the Intestine Replaces and Repairs Itself ’, Harvard Gazette,
14 July 2017 <https://news.harvard.edu/gazette/story/2017/07/understanding-how-the-intestine-
replaces-and-repairs-itself/>
41 Vanable, Joseph. ‘A history of bioelectricity in development and and regeneration’. In: Charles E.
Dinsmore (ed.), A History of Regeneration Research. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1991, pp.
151–78 (p. 163)
42 Sisken, Betty. ‘Enhancement of Nerve Regeneration by Selected Electromagnetic Signals’. In: Marko
Markov (ed.), Dosimetry in Bioelectromagnetics, Boca Raton: CRC Press, 2017, pp. 383–98
43 Tseng A.-S., et al. ‘Induction of Vertebrate Regeneration by a Transient Sodium Current’. Journal of
Neuroscience, vol. 30, no. 39 (2010), pp. 13192–13200
44 Tseng, Ai-sun, and Michael Levin. ‘Cracking the bioelectric code: Probing endogenous ionic controls
of pattern formation’. Communicative & Integrative Biology, vol. 6,1 (2013): e22595
45 Eskova, Anastasia, et al. ‘Gain-of-Function Mutations of Mau / DrAqp3a Influence Zebrafish
Pigment Pattern Formation through the Tissue Environment’. Development 144 (2017),
doi:10.1242/dev.143495
46 Dlouhy, Brian J., et al. ‘Autograft-Derived Spinal Cord Mass Following Olfactory Mucosal Cell
Transplantation in a Spinal Cord Injury Patient: Case Report’. Journal of Neurosurgery: Spine, vol. 21,
no. 4 (2014), pp. 618–22
47 Jabr, Ferris. ‘In the Flesh: The Embedded Dangers of Untested Stem Cell Cosmetics’, Scientific
American, 17 December 2012 <https://www.scientificamerican.com/article/stem-cell-cosmetics/>
48 Aldhous, Peter. ‘An Experiment That Blinded Three Women Unearths the Murky World of Stem
Cell Clinics’, BuzzFeed News, 21 March 2017 <https://www.buzzfeednews.com/article/peter-
aldhous/stem-cell-tragedy-in-florida>
49 Coghlan, Andy. ‘How “stem cell” clinics became a Wild West for dodgy treatments’, New Scientist, 17
January 2018 <https://www.newscientist.com/article/mg23731610-100-how-stem-cell-clinics-
became-a-wild-west-for-dodgy-treatments/>
50 Feng J. F., et al. ‘Electrical Guidance of Human Stem Cells in the Rat Brain’. Stem Cell Reports, vol. 9,
no. 1 (2017), pp. 177–89
1 Rose, Sylvan Meryl, and H. M. Wallingford. ‘Transformation of renal tumors of frogs to normal
tissues in regenerating limbs of salamanders’. Science, vol. 107, no. 2784 (1948), p. 457
2 Oviedo, Néstor J., and Wendy S. Beane. ‘Regeneration: The origin of cancer or a possible cure?’.
Seminars in Cell & Developmental Biology, vol. 20, no. 5 (2009), pp. 557–64
3 Fatima, Iqra, et al. ‘Skin Aging in Long-Lived Naked Mole-Rats is Accompanied by Increased
Expression of Longevity-Associated and Tumor Suppressor Genes’. Journal of Investigative Dermatology,
9 June 2022, doi: 10.1016/j.jid.2022.04.028
4 Ruby, J. Graham, et al. ‘Naked mole-rat mortality rates defy Gompertzian laws by not increasing with
age’. eLife 7:e31157 (2018), doi: 10.7554/eLife.31157
5 Burr, Harold Saxton. Blueprint for Immortality: The Electric Patterns of Life. Essex: Neville Spearman
Publishers, 1972, p. 53
6 Burr, Harold Saxton. Blueprint for Immortality: The Electric Patterns of Life. Essex: Neville Spearman
Publishers, 1972, p. 54
7 Langman, Louis, and Burr, H. S. ‘Electrometric Studies in Women with Malignancy of Cervix Uteri’.
Science, vol. 105, no. 2721 (1947), pp. 209–10
8 Langman, Louis, and Burr, H.S. ‘A technique to aid in the detection of malignancy of the female
genital tract’. Journal of the American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology, vol. 57, issue 2 (1949), pp. 274–
281
9 Langman & Burr, ‘Electrometric’, p. 210
10 Stratton, M. R. (2009). ‘The cancer genome’. Nature, vol. 458, article 7239 (2009), pp. 719–24, doi:
10.1038/nature07943
11 Nordenström, Björn ‘Biologically closed electric circuits: Activation of vascular-interstitial closed
electric circuits for treatment of inoperable cancers’. Journal of Bioelectricity 3 (1984), pp. 137–53
12 Nordenström, Björn. Biologically Closed Electric Circuits: Clinical, Experimental, and Theoretical Evidence for
an Additional Circulatory System. Stockholm: Nordic Medical Publications, 1983
13 Nordenström, Biologically closed
14 Nordenström, Biologically closed, p. vii
15 Parachini, Allan. ‘Cancer-Treatment Theory an Enigma to Scientific World’, Los Angeles Times, 30
September 1986 <https://www.latimes.com/archives/la-xpm-1986-09-30-vw-10015-story.html>
16 Parachini, ‘Cancer-Treatment’, 1986
17 Nordenström, ‘Biologically closed’
18 Parachini, ‘Cancer-Treatment’, 1986
19 Nilsson E., et al. ‘Electrochemical treatment of tumours’. Bioelectrochemistry, vol. 51, no. 1 (2000), pp.
1–11
20 All statistics from ‘Proceedings of the International Association for Biologically Closed Electric
Circuits’. European Journal of Surgery 1994 Supplement 574, pp. 7–23
21 ‘Activation of BCEC-channels for Electrochemical Therapy (ECT) of Cancer’. Proceedings of the IABC
International Association for Biologically-Closed Electric Circuits (BCEC) in Medicine and Biology. Stockholm,
September 12–15, 1993 (1994), pp. 25–9 <https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/7531011/>
22 ‘Björn Nordenström’, 20/20, ABC News, first broadcast 21 October 1988. Available on YouTube:
<https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=OmqTKh-CP88>
23 Moss, ralph W. ‘Bjorn E. W. Nordenström, MD’. Townsend Letter, The Examiner of Alternative
Medicine 285 (2007), p. 156 <link.gale.com/apps/doc/A162234818/AONE?
u=anon~51eea7d2&sid=bookmark-AONE&xid=8719a268>. Accessed 5 August 2021
24 Lois, Carlos, and Arturo Alvarez-Buylla. ‘Long-distance neuronal migration in the adult mammalian
brain’. Science 264 (1994), pp. 1145–8, doi: 10.1126/science.8178174
25 Grimes, J. A., et al. ‘Differential expression of voltage-activated Na + currents in two prostatic
tumour cell lines: contribution to invasiveness in vitro’. FEBS Letters 369 (1995), pp. 290–4
<https://febs.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1016/0014-5793%2895%2900772-2>
26 Reported ubiquitously, including in Pullar, Christine E. (ed.). The Physiology of Bioelectricity in
Development, Tissue Regeneration and Cancer. Boca Raton: CRC Press, 2011, p. 271
27 Arcangeli, Annarosa, and Andrea Becchetti. ‘New Trends in Cancer Therapy: Targeting Ion
Channels and Transporters’. Pharmaceuticals, vol. 3, no. 4 (2010), pp. 1202–24
28 Bianchi, Laura, et al. ‘hERG Encodes a K+ Current Highly Conserved in Tumors of Different
Histogenesis: A Selective Advantage for Cancer Cells?’. Cancer Research, vol. 58, no. 4 (1998), pp.
815–22
29 Kunzelmann, 2005; Fiske, et al, 2006; Stuhmer, et al, 2006; Prevarskaya, et al, 2010; Becchetti, 2011;
Brackenbury, 2012, collected in Yang Ming and William Brackenbury. ‘Membrane potential and
cancer progression’. Frontiers in Physiology, vol. 4, article 185 (2013), doi:
https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2013.00185
30 Santos, Rita, et al. ‘A comprehensive map of molecular drug targets’. Nature Reviews Drug Discovery,
vol. 16, no. 1 (2017), pp. 19–34
31 McKie, Robin. ‘For 30 years I’ve been obsessed by why children get leukaemia. Now we have an
answer’, The Guardian, 30 December 2018
<https://www.theguardian.com/science/2018/dec/30/children-leukaemia-mel-greaves-microbes-
protection-against-disease>
32 Djamgoz, Mustafa, S. P. Fraser, and W. J. Brackenbury. (2019). ‘In Vivo Evidence for Voltage-Gated
Sodium Channel Expression in Carcinomas and Potentiation of Metastasis’. Cancers, vol. 11, no. 11
(2019), p. 1675
33 Leanza, Luigi, Antonella Managò, Mario Zoratti, Erich Gulbins, and Ildiko Szabo. ‘Pharmacological
targeting of ion channels for cancer therapy: In vivo evidences’. Biochimica et Biophysica Acta (BBA) –
Molecular Cell Research, vol. 1863, no. 6, Part B (2016), pp. 1385–97
34 In 2019, a Chinese multicentre preclinical trial tested an antibody that was effective against
Djamgoz’s variant in mice. They claimed this was able to suppress metastasis. Gao, R., et al. ‘Nav1.5-
E3 antibody inhibits cancer progression’. Translational Cancer Research, vol. 8, no. 1 (2019), pp. 44-50,
doi: 10.21037/tcr.2018.12.23
35 Lang, F., and C. Stournaras. ‘Ion channels in cancer: future perspectives and clinical potential’.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences, vol. 369, article 1638
(2014), 20130108
<https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3917362/pdf/rstb20130108.pdf>
36 ‘An interview with Professor Mustafa Djamgoz’, External Speaker Series presentation, Metrion
BioSciences, Cambridge 2018
37 ‘The Bioelectricity Revolution: A Discussion Among the Founding Associate Editors’. Bioelectricity,
vol. 1, no. 1 (2019), pp. 8–15
38 Greaves, Mel. ‘Nothing in cancer makes sense except . . .’. BMC Biology, vol. 16, no. 22 (2018)
39 Wilson, Clare. ‘The secret to killing cancer may lie in its deadly power to evolve’, New Scientist, 4
March 2020 <https://www.newscientist.com/article/mg24532720-800-the-secret-to-killing-cancer-
may-lie-in-its-deadly-power-to-evolve/>
40 Hope, Tyna, and Siân Iles. ‘Technology review: The use of electrical impedance scanning in the
detection of breast cancer’. Breast Cancer Research, vol. 6, no. 69 (2004), pp. 69–74
41 Wilke, Lee, et al. ‘Repeat surgery after breast conservation for the treatment of stage 0 to II breast
carcinoma: a report from the National Cancer Data Base, 2004–2010’. JAMA Surgery, vol. 149, no.
12 (2014), pp. 1296–305.
42 Dixon, J. Michael, et al. ‘Intra-operative assessment of excised breast tumour margins using
ClearEdge imaging device’. European Journal of Surgical Oncology 42 (2016), pp. 1834–40, doi:
10.1016/j. ejso.2016.07.141
43 Djamgoz, Mustafa. ‘In vivo evidence for expression of voltage-gated sodium channels in cancer and
potentiation of metastasis’, Sophion Bioscience YouTube channel, 18 July 2019
<https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=bkKewfmCW6A>. The relevant section of the lecture begins
around sixteen minutes in.
44 Dokken, Kaylinn, and Patrick Fairley. ‘Sodium Channel Blocker Toxicity’ [Updated 30 April 2022].
In: StatPearls [Internet]. Treasure Island, FL: StatPearls Publishing, 2022
<https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK534844/>
45 Reddy, Jay P., et al. ‘Antiepileptic drug use improves overall survival in breast cancer patients with
brain metastases in the setting of whole brain radiotherapy’. Radiotherapy and Oncology, vol. 117, no. 2
(2015), pp. 308–14, doi: 10.1016/j.radonc.2015.10.009
46 Takada, Mitsutaka, et al. ‘Inverse Association between Sodium Channel-Blocking Antiepileptic Drug
Use and Cancer: Data Mining of Spontaneous Reporting and Claims Databases’. International Journal
of Medical Sciences, vol. 13, no. 1 (2016), pp. 48–59, doi: 10.7150/ijms.13834
47 ‘An interview with Professor Mustafa Djamgoz’, External Speaker Series presentation, Metrion
BioSciences, Cambridge 2018
48 Quail, Daniela F., and Johanna A. Joyce. ‘Microenvironmental regulation of tumor progression and
metastasis’. Nature Medicine, vol. 19, no. 11 (2013), pp. 1423–37, doi: 10.1038/nm.3394
49 Zhu, Kan, et al. ‘Electric Fields at Breast Cancer and Cancer Cell Collective Galvanotaxis’. Scientific
Reports, vol. 10, no. 1 (2020), article 8712
50 Wapner, Jessica. ‘A New Theory on Cancer: What We Know About How It Starts Could All Be
Wrong’, Newsweek, 17 July 2017 <https://www.newsweek.com/2017/07/28/cancer-evolution-cells-
637632.html>; see also Davies, Paul. ‘A new theory of cancer’, The Monthly, November 2018
<https://www.themonthly.com.au/issue/2018/november/1540990800/paul-davies/new-theory-
cancer#mtr>
51 Silver, Brian, and Celeste Nelson. ‘The Bioelectric Code: Reprogramming Cancer and Aging From
the Interface of Mechanical and Chemical Microenvironments’. Frontiers in Cell and Developmental
Biology, vol. 6, no. 21 (2018)
52 Lobikin, Maria, Brook Chernet, Daniel Lobo, and Michael Levin. ‘Resting potential, oncogene-
induced tumorigenesis, and metastasis: the bioelectric basis of cancer in vivo’. Physical Biology, vol. 9,
no. 6 (2012), loc. 065002. doi: 10.1088/1478-3975/9/6/065002
53 Chernet, Brook, and Michael Levin. ‘Endogenous Voltage Potentials and the Microenvironment:
Bioelectric Signals that Reveal, Induce and Normalize Cancer’. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Oncology, Suppl. 1:S1-002 (2013), doi: 10.4172/2324-9110
54 Chernet & Levin, ‘Endogenous’
55 Gruber, Ben. ‘Battling cancer with light’, Reuters, 26 April 2016
<https://www.reuters.com/article/us-science-cancer-optogenetics-idUSKCN0XN1u9>
56 Chernet, Brook, and Michael Levin. ‘Transmembrane voltage potential is an essential cellular
parameter for the detection and control of tumor development in a Xenopus model’. Disease Models
& Mechanisms, vol. 6, no. 3 (2013), pp. 595–607, doi: 10.1242/dmm.010835
57 Silver & Nelson, ‘The Bioelectric Code’
58 Tuszynski, Jack, Tatiana Tilli, and Michael Levin. ‘Ion Channel and Neurotransmitter Modulators as
Electroceutical Approaches to the Control of Cancer’. Current Pharmaceutical Design, vol. 23, no. 32
(2017), pp. 4827–41
59 Schlegel, Jürgen, et al. ‘Plasma in cancer treatment’, Clinical Plasma Medicine, vol. 1, no. 2 (2013), pp.
2–7
1 Brown, Joshua. ‘Team Builds the First Living Robots’, The University of Vermont, 13 January 2020
<https://www.uvm.edu/news/story/team-builds-first-living-robots>
2 Lee, Y., et al. ‘Hydrogel soft robotics’. Materials Today Physics 15 (2020)
<https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mtphys.2020.100258>
3 Thubagere, Anupama, et al. ‘A Cargo-Sorting DNA Robot’. Science, vol. 357, article 6356 (2017),
eaan6558
4 Solon, Olivia. ‘Electroceuticals: swapping drugs for devices’, Wired, 28 May 2013
<https://www.wired.co.uk/article/electroceuticals>
5 Geddes, Linda. ‘Healing spark: Hack body electricity to replace drugs’, New Scientist, 19 February
2014 <https://www.newscientist.com/article/mg22129570-500-healing-spark-hack-body-electricity-
to-replace-drugs/>
6 Behar, Michael. ‘Can the nervous system be hacked?’, The New York Times, 23 May 2014
<https://www.nytimes.com/2014/05/25/magazine/can-the-nervous-system-be-hacked.html>
7 Mullard, Asher. ‘Electroceuticals jolt into the clinic, sparking autoimmune opportunities’. Nature
Reviews Drug Discovery 21 (2022), pp. 330–1
8 Hoffman, Henry, and Harold Norman Schnitzlein. ‘The Numbers of Nerve Fibers in the Vagus
Nerve of Man’. The Anatomical Record, vol. 139, no. 3 (1961), pp. 429–35
9 Davies, Dave. ‘Are Implanted Medical Devices Creating a “Danger Within Us”?’, NPR, 17 January
2018 <https://www.npr.org/2018/01/17/578562873/are-implanted-medical-devices-creating-a-
danger-within-us>
10 Golabchi, Asiyeh, et al. ‘Zwitterionic Polymer/Polydopamine Coating Reduce Acute Inflammatory
Tissue Responses to Neural Implants’. Biomaterials 225 (2019), 119519
<https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biomaterials.2019.119519>
11 Leber, Moritz, et al. ‘Advances in Penetrating Multichannel Microelectrodes Based on the Utah Array
Platform’. In: Xiaoxiang Zheng (ed.), Neural Interface: Frontiers and Applications. Singapore: Springer,
2019, pp. 1–40
12 Yin, Pengfei, et al. ‘Advanced Metallic and Polymeric Coatings for Neural Interfacing: Structures,
Properties and Tissue Responses’. Polymers, vol. 13, no. 16 (2021), article 2834
<https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8401399/pdf/polymers-13-02834.pdf>
13 Aregueta-Robles, U. A., et al. ‘Organic electrode coatings for next-generation neural interfaces’.
Frontiers in Neuroengineering, 27 May 2014 <https://doi.org/10.3389/fneng.2014.0001>
14 ‘The Nobel Prize in Chemistry 2000’, NobelPrize.org
<https://www.nobelprize.org/prizes/chemistry/2000/summary/>
15 Cuthbertson, Anthony. ‘Material Found by Scientists “Could Merge AI with Human Brain”’, The
Independent, 17 August 2020 <https://www.independent.co.uk/tech/artificial-intelligence-brain-
computer-cyborg-elon-musk-neuralink-a9673261.html>
16 Chen, Angela. ‘Why It’s so Hard to Develop the Right Material for Brain Implants’, The Verge, 30
May 2018 <https://www.theverge.com/2018/5/30/17408852/brain-implant-materials-
neuroscience-health-chris-bettinger>
Technically, there are also ways to inhibit action potentials, but that just means stimulating inhibitory
17 neurons – which are the kinds of neurons that make other neurons not fire. But it’s still the same
mechanism.
18 Some companies try to understand how the body has interpreted the action potential by implanting
even more electrodes to listen to the ensuing signals. But that carries additional surgical risk, and it’s
certainly not happening in humans.
19 Casella, Alena, et al. ‘Endogenous Electric Signaling as a Blueprint for Conductive Materials in
Tissue Engineering’. Bioelectricity, vol. 3, no. 1 (2021), pp. 27–41
20 Demers, Caroline, et al. ‘Natural Coral Exoskeleton as a Bone Graft Substitute: A Review’. Bio-
Medical Materials and Engineering, vol. 12, no. 1 (2002), pp. 15–35
21 Israel-based OkCoral and CoreBone grow coral on a special diet to make it especially suitable to
grafting.
22 Wan, Mei-chen, et al. ‘Biomaterials from the Sea: Future Building Blocks for Biomedical
Applications’. Bioactive Materials, vol. 6, no. 12 (2021), pp. 4255–85
23 DeCoursey, Thomas. ‘Voltage-Gated Proton Channels and Other Proton Transfer Pathways’.
Physiological Reviews, vol. 83, no. 2 (2003) pp. 475–579, doi: 10.1152/physrev.00028.2002
24 Lane, Nick. ‘Why Are Cells Powered by Proton Gradients?’. Nature Education, vol. 3, no. 9 (2010), p.
18
25 Kautz, Rylan, et al. ‘Cephalopod-Derived Biopolymers for Ionic and Protonic Transistors’. Advanced
Materials, vol. 30, no. 19 (2018), loc. 1704917
26 Ordinario, David, et al. ‘Bulk protonic conductivity in a cephalopod structural protein’. Nature
Chemistry, vol. 6, no. 7 (2014), pp. 596–602
27 Strakosas, Xenofon, et al. ‘Taking Electrons out of Bioelectronics: From Bioprotonic Transistors to
Ion Channels’. Advanced Science, vol. 4, no. 7 (2017), loc. 1600527
28 Kim, Young Jo, et al. ‘Self-Deployable Current Sources Fabricated from Edible Materials’. Journal of
Materials Chemistry B 31 (2013), p. 3781, doi: 10.1039/C3TB20183J
29 Ordinario, David, et al. ‘Protochromic Devices from a Cephalopod Structural Protein’. Advanced
Optical Materials, vol. 5, no. 20 (2017), loc. 1600751
30 Sheehan, Paul. ‘Bioelectronics for Tissue Regeneration’. Defense Advanced Projects Research
Agency <https://www.darpa.mil/program/bioelectronics-for-tissue-regeneration>. Accessed 31
May 2022
31 Kriegman, Sam, et al, ‘Kinematic Self-Replication in Reconfigurable Organisms’. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences, vol. 118, no. 49 (2021), loc. e2112672118
<https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2112672118>
32 Coghlan, Simon and Kobi Leins. ‘Will self-replicating “xenobots” cure diseases, yield new
bioweapons, or simply turn the whole world into grey goo?’, The Conversation, 9 December 2021
<https://theconversation.com/will-self-replicating-xenobots-cure-diseases-yield-new-bioweapons-
or-simply-turn-the-whole-world-into-grey-goo-173244>
33 Adamatzky, Andrew, et al. ‘Fungal Electronics’. Biosystems 212 (2021), loc. 104588, doi:
10.1016/j.biosystems.2021.104588
1 Nitsche, Michael A., et al. ‘Facilitation of Implicit Motor Learning by Weak Transcranial Direct
Current Stimulation of the Primary Motor Cortex in the Human’. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol.
15, no. 4 (2003), pp. 619–26, doi: https://doi.org/10.1162/089892903321662994
2 Trivedi, Bijal. ‘Electrify your mind – literally’, New Scientist, 11 April 2006
<https://www.newscientist.com/article/mg19025471-100-electrify-your-mind-literally/>
Marshall, L, M. Mölle, M. Hallschmid, and J. Born. ‘Transcranial direct current stimulation during
3
sleep improves declarative memory’. The Journal of Neuroscience vol. 24, no. 44 (2004), pp. 9985–92,
doi: 10.1523/Jneurosci.2725-04.2004
4 Walsh, Professor Vincent. ‘Cognitive Effects of TDC at Summit on Transcranial Direct Current
Stimulation (tDCS) at the UC-Davis Center for Mind & Brain’, UC Davis YouTube channel, 8
October 2013 <https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=9fz7r8VDV4o>. The relevant section of the
lecture begins around fourteen minutes in.
5 Wurzman, Rachel et al. ‘An open letter concerning do-it-yourself users of transcranial direct current
stimulation’. Annals of Neurology, vol 80, Issue 1. July 2016
6 Aschwanden, Christie. ‘Science isn’t broken: It’s just a hell of a lot harder than we give it credit for’,
Five Thirty-Eight, 19 August 2015 <https://fivethirtyeight.com/features/science-isnt-broken/>
7 Verma, N., et al. ‘Auricular Vagus Neuromodulation – A Systematic Review on Quality of Evidence
and Clinical Effects’. Frontiers in Neuroscience 15 (2021), article 664740
<https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2021.664740>
8 Young, Stella. ‘I’m not your inspiration, thank you very much.’ TED, June 2014,
www.ted.com/talks/stella_young_i_m_not_your_inspiration_thank_you_very_much/
9 Source is interview with the author at the International Neuroethics Society meeting, 2 November
2018. The issues are also explored in Drew, Liam. ‘The ethics of brain–computer interfaces’. Nature.
24 July 2019 <https://www.nature.com/articles/d41586-019-02214-2>
10 Strickland, Eliza. ‘Worldwide Campaign For Neurorights Notches Its First Win’, IEEE Spectrum, 18
December 2021 <https://spec-trum.ieee.org/neurotech-neurorights>
11 Coghlan, Andy. ‘Vaping really isn’t as harmful for your cells as smoking’, New Scientist, 4 January 2016
<https://www.newscientist.com/article/dn28723-vaping-really-isnt-as-harmful-for-your-cells-as-
smoking/>
12 ‘Committee on the Review of the Health Effects of Electronic Nicotine Delivery Systems and
Others’. In: Kathleen Stratton, Leslie Y. Kwan, and David L. Eaton (eds), Public Health Consequences of
E-Cigarettes, Washington, DC: 2018, 24952 <https://www.nap.edu/catalog/24952>
13 Moehn, Kayla,Yunus Ozekin, and Emily Bates. ‘Investigating the Effects of Vaping and Nicotine’s
Block of Kir2.1 on Humerus and Digital Development in Embryonic Mice’. FASEB Journal, vol. 36,
no. S1 (2022) <https://doi.org/10.1096/fasebj.2022.36.S1.R2578>
14 Benzonana, Laura, et al. ‘Isoflurane, a Commonly Used Volatile Anesthetic, Enhances Renal Cancer
Growth and Malignant Potential via the Hypoxia-Inducible Factor Cellular Signaling Pathway In
Vitro’. Anesthesiology, vol. 119, no. 3 (2013), pp. 593–605
15 Jiang, Jue, and Hong Jiang. ‘Effect of the Inhaled Anesthetics Isoflurane, Sevoflurane and
Desflurane on the Neuropathogenesis of Alzheimer’s Disease (Review)’. Molecular Medicine Reports,
vol. 12, no. 1 (2015), pp. 3–12
16 Robson, David. ‘This is what it’s like waking up during surgery’, Mosaic, 12 March 2019
<https://mosaicscience.com/story/anaesthesia-anesthesia-awake-awareness-surgery-operation-or-
paralysed/>
17 Edelman, Elazer, et al. ‘Case 30-2020: A 54-Year-Old Man with Sudden Cardiac Arrest’. New England
Journal of Medicine, vol. 383, no. 13 (2020), pp. 1263–75
18 Hesham, R. Omar, et al. ‘Licorice Abuse: Time to Send a Warning Message’. Therapeutic Advances in
Endocrinology and Metabolism, vol. 3, no. 4 (2012), pp. 125–38
19 Actually, I noticed two patterns: most of the scientists who got hit with the most scathing criticism
were women. The men sometimes didn’t recall any trouble at all.
20 Davies, Paul. The Demon in the Machine. London: Allen Lane, 2019,, p. 86
21 McNamara, H. M., et al. ‘Bioelectrical domain walls in homogeneous tissues’. Nature Physics 16
(2020), pp. 357–64 <https://doi.org/10.1038/s41567-019-0765-4>
22 Davies, The Demon in the Machine, pp. 82–3
23 Pietak, A., and Levin, M. ‘Exploring Instructive Physiological Signaling with the Bioelectric Tissue
Simulation Engine’. Frontiers in Bioengineering and Biotechnology, vol. 4, article 55 (2016), doi:
10.3389/fbioe.2016.00055
INDEX
DARPA (Defense Advanced Research Project Agency) ref1, ref2, ref3, ref4
and regeneration ref1
and tDCS ref1
Davies, Paul ref1, ref2
DBS (‘deep brain stimulation’) ref1, ref2, ref3, ref4, ref5
De Loof , Arnold ref1
death ref1, ref2; see also corpses
defibrilation ref1
Delgado, José ref1
dementia ref1, ref2, ref3
dendrites ref1
depression ref1, ref2, ref3, ref4, ref5
and DBS ref1
and tDCS ref1
Dermacorder ref1, ref2
Descartes, René ref1
diabetes ref1, ref2, ref3
die-back ref1
Dixon, Mike ref1
Djamgoz, Mustafa ref1, ref2, ref3, ref4
DNA ref1, ref2, ref3, ref4
Dobbs, David ref1
dogs ref1, ref2
Donoghue, John ref1, ref2, ref3
drugs see medicine
Du Bois-Reymond, Emil ref1, ref2, ref3, ref4
tDCS (transcranial direct current stimulation) ref1, ref2, ref3, ref4, ref5, ref6
teeth ref1
telegraphs ref1, ref2, ref3
telepathy ref1
tetrodotoxin ref1
torpillage ref1
Tosti, Elisabetta ref1, ref2
Tourette’s syndrome ref1
Tracey, Kevin ref1
tractors ref1
transistors ref1, ref2
trial volunteers ref1
Tseng, Ai-Sun ref1, ref2, ref3
yeast ref1
Young, Stella ref1