The Truth About Lie Detectors Aka Polygraph Tests
The Truth About Lie Detectors Aka Polygraph Tests
The Truth About Lie Detectors Aka Polygraph Tests
Findings
Lie detector tests have become a popular cultural icon — from crime dramas to comedies to
advertisements — the picture of a polygraph pen wildly gyrating on a moving chart is readily
recognized symbol. But, as psychologist Leonard Saxe, PhD, (1991) has argued, the idea that we
can detect a person's veracity by monitoring psychophysiological changes is more myth than
reality. Even the term "lie detector," used to refer to polygraph testing, is a misnomer. So-called
"lie detection" involves inferring deception through analysis of physiological responses to a
structured, but unstandardized, series of questions.
The instrument typically used to conduct polygraph tests consists of a physiological recorder that
assesses three indicators of autonomic arousal: heart rate/blood pressure, respiration, and skin
conductivity. Most examiners today use computerized recording systems. Rate and depth of
respiration are measured by pneumographs wrapped around a subject's chest. Cardiovascular
activity is assessed by a blood pressure cuff. Skin conductivity (called the galvanic skin or
electrodermal response) is measured through electrodes attached to a subject's fingertips.
The recording instrument and questioning techniques are only used during a part of the
polygraph examination. A typical examination includes a pretest phase during which the
technique is explained and each test question reviewed. The pretest interview is designed to
ensure that subjects understand the questions and to induce a subject's concern about being
deceptive. Polygraph examinations often include a procedure called a "stimulation test," which is
a demonstration of the instrument's accuracy in detecting deception.
Several questioning techniques are commonly used in polygraph tests. The most widely used
test format for subjects in criminal incident investigations is the Control Question Test (CQT). The
CQT compares responses to "relevant" questions (e.g., "Did you shoot your wife?"), with those of
"control" questions. The control questions are designed to control for the effect of the generally
threatening nature of relevant questions. Control questions concern misdeeds that are similar to
those being investigated, but refer to the subject's past and are usually broad in scope; for
example, "Have you ever betrayed anyone who trusted you?"
A person who is telling the truth is assumed to fear control questions more than relevant
questions. This is because control questions are designed to arouse a subject's concern about
their past truthfulness, while relevant questions ask about a crime they know they did not commit.
A pattern of greater physiological response to relevant questions than to control questions leads
https://www.apa.org/print-this 1/4
3/30/2019 The Truth About Lie Detectors (aka Polygraph Tests)
https://www.apa.org/print-this 2/4
3/30/2019 The Truth About Lie Detectors (aka Polygraph Tests)
Despite the lack of good research validating polygraph tests, efforts are on-going to develop and
assess new approaches. Some work involves use of additional autonomic physiologic indicators,
such as cardiac output and skin temperature. Such measures, however, are more specific to
deception than polygraph tests. Other researchers, such as Frank Andrew Kozel, MD, have
examined functional brain imaging as a measure of deception. Dr. Kozel's research team found
that for lying, compared with telling the truth, there is more activation in five brain regions (Kozel
et al., 2004). However, the results do not currently support the use of fMRI to detect deception in
real world individual cases.
The development of currently used "lie detection" technologies has been based on ideas about
physiological functioning but has, for the most part, been independent of systematic
psychological research. Early theorists believed that deception required effort and, thus, could be
assessed by monitoring physiological changes. But such propositions have not been proven and
basic research remains limited on the nature of deceptiveness. Efforts to develop actual tests
have always outpaced theory-based basic research. Without a better theoretical understanding
of the mechanisms by which deception functions, however, development of a lie detection
technology seems highly problematic.
For now, although the idea of a lie detector may be comforting, the most practical advice is to
remain skeptical about any conclusion wrung from a polygraph.
Lykken, D. (1998). A Tremor in the Blood: Uses and Abuses of the Lie Detector, 2d ed. New York:
Perseus.
National Academy of Sciences (2002). The Polygraph and Lie Detection. Washington, DC:
National Academy Press.
Saxe, L. (1991). Lying: Thoughts of an applied social psychologist. American Psychologist, 46(4):
409-15.
https://www.apa.org/print-this 3/4
3/30/2019 The Truth About Lie Detectors (aka Polygraph Tests)
Saxe, L. & Ben-Shakhar, G. (1999). Admissibility of polygraph tests: The application of scientific
standards post-Daubert. Psychology, Public Policy and the Law, 5(1): 203-23.
https://www.apa.org/print-this 4/4