The Truth About Lie Detectors Aka Polygraph Tests

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 4

3/30/2019 The Truth About Lie Detectors (aka Polygraph Tests)

The Truth About Lie Detectors


(aka Polygraph Tests)
Most psychologists agree that there is little evidence that polygraph tests can accurately detect
lies.

Findings
Lie detector tests have become a popular cultural icon — from crime dramas to comedies to
advertisements — the picture of a polygraph pen wildly gyrating on a moving chart is readily
recognized symbol. But, as psychologist Leonard Saxe, PhD, (1991) has argued, the idea that we
can detect a person's veracity by monitoring psychophysiological changes is more myth than
reality. Even the term "lie detector," used to refer to polygraph testing, is a misnomer. So-called
"lie detection" involves inferring deception through analysis of physiological responses to a
structured, but unstandardized, series of questions.
The instrument typically used to conduct polygraph tests consists of a physiological recorder that
assesses three indicators of autonomic arousal: heart rate/blood pressure, respiration, and skin
conductivity. Most examiners today use computerized recording systems. Rate and depth of
respiration are measured by pneumographs wrapped around a subject's chest. Cardiovascular
activity is assessed by a blood pressure cuff. Skin conductivity (called the galvanic skin or
electrodermal response) is measured through electrodes attached to a subject's fingertips.
The recording instrument and questioning techniques are only used during a part of the
polygraph examination. A typical examination includes a pretest phase during which the
technique is explained and each test question reviewed. The pretest interview is designed to
ensure that subjects understand the questions and to induce a subject's concern about being
deceptive. Polygraph examinations often include a procedure called a "stimulation test," which is
a demonstration of the instrument's accuracy in detecting deception.
Several questioning techniques are commonly used in polygraph tests. The most widely used
test format for subjects in criminal incident investigations is the Control Question Test (CQT). The
CQT compares responses to "relevant" questions (e.g., "Did you shoot your wife?"), with those of
"control" questions. The control questions are designed to control for the effect of the generally
threatening nature of relevant questions. Control questions concern misdeeds that are similar to
those being investigated, but refer to the subject's past and are usually broad in scope; for
example, "Have you ever betrayed anyone who trusted you?"
A person who is telling the truth is assumed to fear control questions more than relevant
questions. This is because control questions are designed to arouse a subject's concern about
their past truthfulness, while relevant questions ask about a crime they know they did not commit.
A pattern of greater physiological response to relevant questions than to control questions leads
https://www.apa.org/print-this 1/4
3/30/2019 The Truth About Lie Detectors (aka Polygraph Tests)

to a diagnosis of "deception." Greater response to control questions leads to a judgment of


nondeception. If no difference is found between relevant and control questions, the test result is
considered "inconclusive."
An alternative polygraph procedure is called the Guilty Knowledge Test (GKT). A GKT involves
developing a multiple-choice test with items concerning knowledge that only a guilty subject
could have. A test of a theft suspect might, for example, involve questions such as "Was $500,
$1,000, or $5,000 stolen?" If only a guilty suspect knows the correct answer, a larger
physiological reaction to a correct choice would indicate deception. With a sufficient number of
items, a psychometrically sound evaluation could be developed. GKTs are not widely employed,
but there is great interest in doing so. One limitation of the GKT is that it can be used only when
investigators have information that only a guilty subject would know. The interpretation of "no
deception" is also a potential limitation, since it may indicate lack of knowledge rather than
innocence.
The accuracy (i.e., validity) of polygraph testing has long been controversial. An underlying
problem is theoretical: There is no evidence that any pattern of physiological reactions is unique
to deception. An honest person may be nervous when answering truthfully and a dishonest
person may be non-anxious. Also, there are few good studies that validate the ability of
polygraph procedures to detect deception. As Dr. Saxe and Israeli psychologist Gershon Ben-
Shahar (1999) note, "it may, in fact, be impossible to conduct a proper validity study." In real-world
situations, it's very difficult to know what the truth is.
A particular problem is that polygraph research has not separated placebo-like effects (the
subject's belief in the efficacy of the procedure) from the actual relationship between deception
and their physiological responses. One reason that polygraph tests may appear to be accurate is
that subjects who believe that the test works and that they can be detected may confess or will
be very anxious when questioned. If this view is correct, the lie detector might be better called a
fear detector.
Some confusion about polygraph test accuracy arises because they are used for different
purposes, and for each context somewhat different theory and research is applicable. Thus, for
example, virtually no research assesses the type of test and procedure used to screen
individuals for jobs and security clearances. Most research has focused on specific incident
testing. The cumulative research evidence suggests that CQTs detect deception better than
chance, but with significant error rates, both of misclassifying innocent subjects (false positives)
and failing to detect guilty individuals (false negatives).
Research on the processes involved in CQT polygraph examinations suggests that several
examiner, examinee, and situational factors influence test validity, as may the technique used to
score polygraph charts. There is little research on the effects of subjects' differences in such
factors as education, intelligence, or level of autonomic arousal.

Evidence indicates that strategies used to "beat" polygraph examinations, so-called


countermeasures, may be effective. Countermeasures include simple physical movements,
psychological interventions (e.g., manipulating subjects' beliefs about the test), and the use of
pharmacological agents that alter arousal patterns.

https://www.apa.org/print-this 2/4
3/30/2019 The Truth About Lie Detectors (aka Polygraph Tests)

Despite the lack of good research validating polygraph tests, efforts are on-going to develop and
assess new approaches. Some work involves use of additional autonomic physiologic indicators,
such as cardiac output and skin temperature. Such measures, however, are more specific to
deception than polygraph tests. Other researchers, such as Frank Andrew Kozel, MD, have
examined functional brain imaging as a measure of deception. Dr. Kozel's research team found
that for lying, compared with telling the truth, there is more activation in five brain regions (Kozel
et al., 2004). However, the results do not currently support the use of fMRI to detect deception in
real world individual cases.

Significance & Practical Application


Polygraph testing has generated considerable scientific and public controversy. Most
psychologists and other scientists agree that there is little basis for the validity of polygraph tests.
Courts, including the United States Supreme Court (cf. U.S. v. Scheffer, 1998 in which Dr.'s Saxe's
research on polygraph fallibility was cited), have repeatedly rejected the use of polygraph
evidence because of its inherent unreliability. Nevertheless, polygraph testing continues to be
used in non-judicial settings, often to screen personnel, but sometimes to try to assess the
veracity of suspects and witnesses, and to monitor criminal offenders on probation. Polygraph
tests are also sometimes used by individuals seeking to convince others of their innocence and,
in a narrow range of circumstances, by private agencies and corporations.

The development of currently used "lie detection" technologies has been based on ideas about
physiological functioning but has, for the most part, been independent of systematic
psychological research. Early theorists believed that deception required effort and, thus, could be
assessed by monitoring physiological changes. But such propositions have not been proven and
basic research remains limited on the nature of deceptiveness. Efforts to develop actual tests
have always outpaced theory-based basic research. Without a better theoretical understanding
of the mechanisms by which deception functions, however, development of a lie detection
technology seems highly problematic.

For now, although the idea of a lie detector may be comforting, the most practical advice is to
remain skeptical about any conclusion wrung from a polygraph.

Cited Research & Additional Sources


Kozel, F.A., Padgett, T.M. & George, M.S. (2004). A Replication Study of the Neural Correlates of
Deception. Behavioral Neuroscience, 118(4): 852-56.

Lykken, D. (1998). A Tremor in the Blood: Uses and Abuses of the Lie Detector, 2d ed. New York:
Perseus.

National Academy of Sciences (2002). The Polygraph and Lie Detection. Washington, DC:
National Academy Press.

Saxe, L. (1991). Lying: Thoughts of an applied social psychologist. American Psychologist, 46(4):
409-15.

https://www.apa.org/print-this 3/4
3/30/2019 The Truth About Lie Detectors (aka Polygraph Tests)

Saxe, L. & Ben-Shakhar, G. (1999). Admissibility of polygraph tests: The application of scientific
standards post-Daubert. Psychology, Public Policy and the Law, 5(1): 203-23.

American Psychological Association, August 5, 2004

Find this article at:


https://www.apa.org/research/action/polygraph

https://www.apa.org/print-this 4/4

You might also like