College Students Uses and Motives For e
College Students Uses and Motives For e
College Students Uses and Motives For e
by
Fall 2003
by
Approved: __________________________________________________________
Elizabeth M. Perse, Ph.D.
Professor in charge of thesis on behalf of the Advisory Committee
Approved: __________________________________________________________
Elizabeth M. Perse, Ph.D.
Chair of the Department of Communication
Approved: __________________________________________________________
Mark W. Huddleston, Ph.D.
Dean of the College of Arts and Science
Approved: __________________________________________________________
Conrado M. Gempesaw II, Ph.D.
Vice Provost for Academic and International Programs
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
My fellow graduate students, Erin Ritter, Jascha Fields, James Rayburn, Lisa
McFadden, and Myriah Goldenberg, who made the journey of graduate school enjoyable
My committee members, Scott E. Caplan, Ph.D., and Xiaomei Cai, Ph.D., for
My brother John, without whom this thesis would not have been realized, I
am forever indebted to you. My mom and dad, for your unending love and support, I
could not have achieved this much without you. My sister Jessica, for always being there
to listen to me and for being my sounding board, I am blessed to have you as my sister.
My friends, Laura, Hillary, Lori, and Julie, for their support and encouragement the past
two years, I do not know where I would be without you. My family, Aunt Pat, Poppie,
Mimmie, and Mom Mom, for providing me with a home away from home, I am forever
grateful. My surrogate family, Mr. and Mrs. Gennaria and KC, for opening their hearts
and their home to me, I am grateful for the kindness you have extended. My boyfriend
Mike, my pillar of strength, for your love and patience, for having confidence in me when
I do not always have confidence in myself, I am a better person for having you in my life.
iii
TABLE OF CONTENTS
Chapter
1 INTRODUCTION ..............................................................................................1
Technology Justification.....................................................................................3
Literature Review..............................................................................................14
iv
2 METHOD .........................................................................................................41
Procedure ..........................................................................................................41
The Nature of the Sample .................................................................................42
Uses...................................................................................................................43
Motives .............................................................................................................47
Measures ...........................................................................................................52
Shyness ...................................................................................................52
Loneliness ...............................................................................................54
Unwillingness to Communicate..............................................................57
Interpersonal Communication Satisfaction.............................................60
Social Networks ......................................................................................62
Involvement in Student Activities ..........................................................62
Long Distance Relationships ..................................................................63
Statistical Analysis............................................................................................63
3 RESULTS .........................................................................................................65
Uses...................................................................................................................65
Motives .............................................................................................................84
Psychological Antecedents .............................................................................105
Social Antecedents..........................................................................................112
Demographic Antecedents ..............................................................................113
4 DISCUSSION .................................................................................................126
Summary of Results........................................................................................126
Uses.......................................................................................................127
Motives .................................................................................................129
Psychological Antecedents ...................................................................131
Social Antecedents................................................................................136
Demographic Antecedents ....................................................................138
v
APPENDIX: SURVEY INSTRUMENT...................................................................147
REFERENCES ...........................................................................................................163
vi
LIST OF TABLES
11 Online Uses..............................................................................................76
vii
21 Summary of Means of Motives..............................................................104
viii
ABSTRACT
The purpose of this study was to explore college students’ uses and motives
of e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms, as well as to examine the effect of
psychological, social, and demographic antecedents on college students’ CMC use. This
study was grounded in the uses and gratifications perspective, which posits that people
use certain communication channels to fulfill needs and motives. The hypotheses
predicted that e-mail would be used more for task related and instrumental
communication whereas instant messaging and online chat rooms would be used more for
college students to measure e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms uses and
and online chat rooms share five general uses: task-related, social-related, offline, online,
and long distance. Results also indicated that e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat
rooms share some motives, but this study found that there were unique motives reported
for each of the three forms of CMC: convenience for e-mail, companionship and
anonymity for instant messaging, and chat room benefits for online chat rooms. Finally,
this study found that psychological, social, and demographic antecedents have an effect
on college students’ use of e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms. People who
are lonely, who are less satisfied with interpersonal communication, and who find
ix
communication to be less rewarding spend more time in online chat rooms. Results
showed that the more college students are involved in student activities the more time
they spend e-mailing. This study also found that female college students use e-mail more
than male students and that younger college students, those 17-20 years of age use instant
messaging more than older college students, those 21 years of age and older. The results
of this study support uses and gratifications view that people are motivated by different
reasons to use certain channels of communication and that social and psychological
antecedents affect these choices. The finding that e-mail, instant messaging, and online
chat rooms have unique attributes indicate that these various forms of CMC and other
what people are using the Internet for and why they are using it.
x
Chapter 1
INTRODUCTION
Home computers, with the help of the Internet, are offering people services that
are competing with the landline telephone as a main source of communication. Services
available through the home computer, such as electronic mail, instant messaging, and
users can connect with friends, family, coworkers, and even meet new people via the
text messages that are relayed from senders’ computers to receivers’” (Walther, 1992, p.
52). Modes of communication that are termed CMC now include various forms of e-mail
and computer conferencing including instant messaging, online chat rooms, listservs, and
bulletin boards just to name a few. CMC technologies enable users to communicate in
with e-mail. They also allow group online conversations, as with chat rooms, or for
One population, in particular, has become a wide user of the Internet and its
communication capabilities. College students have connected to the Internet more than
the general population, according to a survey by The Pew Internet & American Life
1
Project (Jones, 2002). This survey found that 86% of college students are online, which
is considerably higher than the 59% of the general population that is connecting to the
Internet. Most college students have grown up with computers in their homes and their
schools, and the Internet has almost always been a part of their world. The University of
Michigan’s Information Technology Central Services reports that around 90% of its
incoming first year students already own a PC and are bringing it with them to campus
(Brown, 2002). Use of the Internet as a means of communication is not a new concept to
today’s college students. With high-speed connections available in many dormitories and
computer labs on campus, as well as some off-campus housing offering the service,
The more students use the Internet and its communication technologies, the more
it becomes a part of their daily activities. What impact will the daily use of CMC have
on college students once they leave the campus and enter the workforce? Will many
students become Internet dropouts after college as some researchers have suggested
(Kingsley & Anderson, 1998)? Or will they continue to make daily use of the Internet
and the technologies it offers? These are questions that researchers would like to be able
to answer, but which they cannot answer just yet. First, more research needs to be done
looking at why college students are using CMC, what are their purposes and what are
they seeking from these forms of communication. Second, more research needs to be
done looking at who, in particular, is drawn to each type of CMC. The goal of this study
is to look at these two concepts: who, of college students, is using e-mail, instant
messaging, and online chat rooms, and why are they using these forms of CMC.
2
Technology Justification
Many forms of CMC exist in the virtual world of the Internet. With so many
CMC outlets, it can be difficult for researchers to decide which forms to study. It would
be difficult to try to look at every single form in just one study. This study will look at
three forms of CMC: e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms. These three
forms of CMC are not the only ways of communicating online; newsgroups, bulletin
boards, and listservs are just a few more of the many ways college students and the
general population have to communicate online. But, there are several reasons that lend
themselves to why studying these three specific forms of CMC usage in college students
Widespread Use
These three specific forms of CMC usage have become a part of many college
use of different technologies can have implications for the future depending on how and
why the technologies are used by certain groups of people. College students’ usage of
CMC technologies needs to be explored to lead researchers to ask questions about the
impact of these technologies on students’ lives and face-to-face interaction with other
people. In choosing how to do this research, examining the widely used technologies will
be more beneficial in that more information can be learned about a larger part of the
population.
messages in the form of electronic letters to other users in asynchronous time. The Pew
3
Internet & American Life Project indicated that e-mail is the most frequently used
Internet communication tool among college students (Jones, 2002). Among the
respondents, 62% of students surveyed reported using e-mail as their primary Internet
medium; the study also found that 72% of college students check their e-mail at least
once a day, which points to e-mail usage becoming part of their daily routines (Jones,
2002). Some researchers have pointed to e-mail as the main reason people are getting
online (Baym, 2002). E-mail is among the top five activities on the Internet, along with
users are able to communicate with other users in real time. Users send a message to
another user, which appears on the receiver’s computer screen. The receiver then
Many different instant messaging programs are available, including two of the most
popular: America Online’s Instant Messenger (AIM) and ICQ (“I seek you”). The
popularity of instant messaging has taken hold in the college student population; they are
twice as likely as the average Internet user to use it (Jones, 2002). The Pew Internet &
American Life Project found that 29% of students surveyed reported that instant
messaging is their primary Internet tool and that on any day 26% of college students use
instant messaging (Jones, 2002). Leung (2001) found that for many Chinese college
students, use of ICQ was a daily activity, with 32% of respondents indicating that they
chat on ICQ daily. In another study conducted by the Pew Internet & American Life
Project, researchers found that instant messaging was primarily a young person’s activity
4
(Rainie, 2000). They found that 59% of those people 18-24 years old, who were
Online chat rooms appear to be an activity in which many young adult Internet
users take part as well. Online chat rooms are a form of mediated communication
technology in which users, like instant messaging, are able to communicate with others in
real time but also add the possibility of conversation with many people rather than just a
single person. Users enter a chat room, which may be for people with a specific interest,
such as knitting, or for people of a certain age, such as those 50 years or older. Once in
the chat room users are able to carry on a conversation. Fifty-three percent of Internet
users ages 18-24 have participated in online chat rooms at some point and 8% of this
population engage in the activity daily (Rainie, 2000). Rumbough (2001) found that 37%
of the college students he surveyed have joined a chat room to interact with other people
at some point with 5.8% of those doing so daily. While the numbers of college students
using online chat rooms as a daily activity is not as high as those using e-mail and instant
messaging daily, large percentages of college students have participated in chat rooms
and the gratifications they seek from this particular form of CMC is worth studying.
Another important reason for focusing on e-mail, instant messaging, and online
chat rooms has to do with the technologies themselves. E-mail, instant messaging, and
online chat rooms are conceptually similar technologies in that they allow for
allows people the ability to communicate with others and convey information without
5
actually having face-to-face interactions, but they are able to convey the same sort of
information that they may have done in a face-to-face setting. Other forms of CMC, such
as bulletin boards and newsgroups, are technologies that are geared more towards person-
to-group interaction; whereas, e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms allow
Studying technologies that are more person-to-person in nature brings the study of
CMC into the realm of interpersonal communication, which has just recently begun to
explore the effect of CMC on relationship development and maintenance. Most of the
earlier work done with CMC has been in an organizational setting, focusing on the effect
of CMC on relationships within organizations (Stafford, Kline, & Dimmick, 1999) rather
than on how it affects the everyday, personal relationships of users and what reasons they
Exploring college students’ uses of e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat
rooms allows the study of the asynchronous and synchronous communication dichotomy.
These three forms of CMC differ in regards to this particular characteristic. E-mail is a
form of CMC that is asynchronous in nature; users are able to send an e-mail without the
recipient being available at the time the e-mail is sent. E-mail’s asynchronous nature has
been labeled as a critical feature because it expands a person’s potential for interpersonal
Kline, & Stafford, 2000), and may be an advantage that it holds over other types of CMC.
6
In contrast to e-mail’s asynchronous feature, instant messaging and online chat rooms are
usually synchronous in nature; users are able to “chat” with another user in real time.
One aspect of instant messaging and online chat rooms’ synchronous feature makes it
unique from the telephone’s synchronous nature. While the CMC interaction occurs in
real time, a user can be typing an e-mail, searching the web, and downloading music
while “chatting” with two or three people and never miss a beat of each and every
conversation since unlike the telephone a user reads the other person’s responses rather
benefits that make them more desirable for certain people and for certain occasions.
Future research in the area of CMC will help to discover these people and occasions.
Much of the research that has been conducted so far on people’s use of CMC
technologies has looked at e-mail (Dimmick et al., 2000; Golden, Beauclair, & Sussman,
1992; Hill & Monk, 2000; Romm & Pliskin, 1999; Stafford et al., 1999) or CMC/Internet
usage as a whole (Dainton & Aylor, 2002; Flaherty, Pearce, & Rubin, 1998; McKenna,
Green, & Gleason, 2002; Papacharissi & Rubin, 2000). There is not as much information
available about the specific use of instant messaging (Hard af Segerstad & Ljungstrand,
2002; Lenhart, Rainie, & Lewis, 2001; Leung, 2001) and online chat rooms (Markey &
Wells, 2002; Peris, Gimeno, Pinazo, Ortet, Carrero, Sanchiz, & Ibanez, 2002; Whitty,
2002). More and more studies are being done on these two forms of CMC, but even
more research is still needed. Now, there are many different Internet activities that a
person can engage in, such as interpersonal communication, online games, online
7
shopping, information searching, streaming audio and video as well as many more.
Because of this diversity it is now more useful to study the specific activities of the
Internet separately. There are differences between the activities and there are differences
between the people who engage in these activities. For example, the people who get
online to play games are not necessarily the same people who get online to shop. To
better understand the impact of the Internet on people’s lives, it is necessary to have an
understanding of all the different activities that people engage in online and the best way
activities that people engage in while on the Internet: e-mailing, instant messaging, and
participating in online chat rooms. Previous research on e-mail usage will be helpful in
discovering why and how college students use e-mail. More information regarding why
and how students use instant messaging and online chat rooms will be discovered from
this study and be able to be used by future researchers. The limited amount of research
that has looked at separate forms of CMC indicates that there are differences in relation
to motives for using various forms of CMC. Seeking conversation and initiating
relationships with others are two motives reported for online chat room use (Peris et al.,
2002). These motives differ from the motives found for e-mail, which include sustaining
relationships and convenience (Stafford et al., 1999), and for instant messaging, which
include keeping up with fashion trends (Leung, 2001). The difference in motives
indicates that these three forms of CMC are unique and deserve to be studied separately.
8
Uses and Gratifications Perspective
various forms of technology. It allows researchers to ask the question “why?” without
adding a value judgment to the answer (Katz, Blumler, & Gurevitch, 1974). This
perspective gives researchers the opportunity to conduct pure exploratory research. Uses
and gratifications suspends value judgments about specific content, so researchers would
not assume that soap opera viewing is dysfunctional, for example, instead researchers ask
soap opera viewers why they watch, and then identify the functions that these television
communication is well suited to the uses and gratifications perspective. Uses and
gratifications has been suggested as a useful perspective to study new technology (Rubin
& Bantz, 1987). Interpersonal communication via the Internet is still rather new that it
could benefit from a more exploratory perspective. The Internet is a technology that is
seen as both functional and dysfunctional; it has been criticized by some as having a
negative impact on society and hailed by others for its positive outcomes (Bargh, 2002).
Because the Internet and the services that it provides are still evolving, these value
judgments are ones which should be set aside until more information can be found out
Uses and gratifications is built on five assumptions (Katz et al., 1974): (a) the
audience is viewed as active, (b) the choice to use a particular media to fulfill a certain
gratification lies within the user, (c) media compete with other media to satisfy users,
9
(d) the audience is capable of self-report, and (e) value judgments should be suspended
Several of these assumptions show how uses and gratifications can illuminate
understanding of CMC. The CMC audience is certainly active, for example, choosing to
use e-mail or instant messaging. The choice to use a particular CMC to fulfill a certain
gratification lies within the user. And CMC also competes with the telephone and face-
A key point to the uses and gratifications perspective is that social and
et al., 1974; Rosengren, 1974). These social and psychological factors should not be
neglected in research. Rosengren (1974) explained that people have needs, which are
influenced by individual and societal characteristics; these needs produce motives, which
Rosengren’s paradigm has led to some criticism that uses and gratifications’ terms
lack precision (Swanson, 1979). So, it is important to define terms. The term uses is
preferences or amount of time spent with the selected channel. A use is a specific
activity that an individual performs using a communication channel. People use various
channels to fulfill communication motives and needs. Motives and needs are difficult to
separate since the latter is manifested in the former (Rubin, Perse, & Barbato, 1988). For
clarity, a need is referred to as some sort of basic drive or problem and it produces a
motive to try to solve the problem. For example, if a person has a need for interaction
10
with others, this need will produce the motive to seek out some sort of communication
channel that will allow him or her to interact with other people. A motive is a reason that
prompts a certain use. Another term that can be used to describe a motive is gratification
sought. According to uses and gratifications, the gratifications sought can be indirectly
1974). In terms of CMC usage these social and psychological characteristics may consist
of factors of college students’ lives, including social networks, social skills, and
perspective, which sought to examine the role played by the media in people’s lives.
The five assumptions of uses and gratifications are consistent with views about
interpersonal communication (Rubin & Rubin, 1985). The first assumption, which states
that mass media use is goal-directed, is also true of interpersonal communication. People
seek out others with whom to interact and socialize. Second, the assumption that users
seek out media to gratify certain needs, coincides with many interpersonal approaches to
needs such as Schutz’s (1958) approach which states that people have three basic needs:
inclusion, affection, and control. The third assumption states that various forms of media
11
compete with each other to satisfy the user’s needs. This is the case with interpersonal
communication as well; people choose from among their friends and family to initiate
and conduct interactions with and are able to structure the interactions in a certain way.
The uses and gratifications perspective assumes that the audience is able to self-report
their motives for using certain media; self-report is a popular method used in
interpersonal communication research also. The last assumption, which states that
judgments should be withheld from research until audience motives can be explored
the question of how communication takes place and how relationships develop, but
researchers have not always been asking why communication takes place and why
relationships develop (Rubin et al., 1988; Rubin & Rubin, 1985). The uses and
gratifications perspective allows researchers to ask both how and why. Certain aspects of
the uses and gratifications perspective that directly relate to the question of why are
and need fulfillment. Uses and gratifications helps to understand how certain
people’s motives, information about media use in general has been discovered; thus, an
1988). Based on the understanding of uses and gratifications research, Rubin, Perse, and
12
Barbato (1985) were able to not only develop an instrument to measure interpersonal
communication motives, but they were able to generate information about how the uses
Another aspect of uses and gratifications that lends itself to interpersonal research
channel a person has for fulfilling a particular need. For example, in mass
communication, a person may have a number of different friends from whom to choose
to fulfill a need for belongingness and each of these friends represents a different
alternative to mass communication channels and vice versa (Rubin & Rubin, 1985). In
more recent research, the Internet is being studied to see if it is a functional alternative to
A third aspect of uses and gratifications theory that has also been applied to recent
Internet research (Papacharissi & Rubin, 2000) and is applicable to the study of
and instrumental communication that are seen in interpersonal research are similar to
those found in mass communication research (Rubin & Rubin, 1985). People engage in
13
communication with friends many times just because it is a habit and a way of passing
time, which marks ritualized communication. At other times, people interact with others
to find out information such as how much a pair of shoes costs at a department store; this
interpersonal communication. The assumptions and various aspects of the approach lend
themselves to extend the perspective’s use beyond its initial applications. Researchers
have started to expand uses and gratifications first to explore the realm of interpersonal
communication (Rubin et al., 1988) and even further to explore the Internet (Papacharissi
& Rubin, 2000). Now researchers are expanding even more and studying computer-
mediated communication, one specific area of the Internet, from a uses and gratifications
perspective.
Literature Review
communication, has started to grow, but it is still in its beginning stages. One reason for
this is that it is an area of study that sometimes blurs the line that exists in communication
interpersonal communication. Some researchers argue that CMC may actually present a
new communication system, called hyperpersonal, which may need its own theories to
explain the usage of these new communication technologies (Caplan, 2001; Walther,
1996).
14
What is interesting and possibly somewhat difficult about CMC research is that it
is an area of research that can be and has been examined from a number of different
Richness Theory (Daft & Lengel, 1984), Social Presence Model (Short, Williams, &
Christie, 1976), and Social Penetration Theory (Altman & Taylor, 1973), can add
something to the body of research about the subject. Regardless of whether a study was
provide researchers with the information they need to further the area of computer-
mediated communication. While this thesis is mostly driven by the uses and
gratifications perspective, it will be helpful to look at other types of research that have
Prior research has studied the Internet and CMC technologies as single
Internet as a whole and did not differentiate various forms of CMC or other non-CMC
activities of the Internet. This might have made some sense in the 1980s and early 1990s
when e-mail was virtually the only widespread CMC use of the Internet. There were,
however, other communication activities then, such as bulletin boards, listservs, and
MUDs and MOOs. Researchers looked at topics such as CMC compared to face-to-face
on the Internet (McKenna et al., 2002; Parks & Floyd, 1996; Rumbough, 2001), CMC
15
Haythornthwaite, 1996), deceptive behaviors in CMC (Rumbough, 2001), and CMC use
Past research has shown that meaningful relationships are able to form on the
Internet (McKenna et al., 2002). Parks and Floyd (1996) found that two-thirds of
respondents reported forming a personal relationship with someone they had met on the
contradicts some of the earlier research findings that suggested that relationships formed
online were impersonal because of a lack of nonverbal cues (Walther, 1996). The
absence of cues such as physical appearance has been shown to positively affect first
impressions formed online. McKenna and her colleagues (2002) found that people who
met online liked one another better than they would have initially had they met face-to-
face. Research found that many people used the Internet technologies as a means of
initiating and establishing face-to-face relationships (Rumbough, 2001). Those who meet
online many times contact each other using other forms of communication such as the
telephone and face-to-face encounters (Parks & Floyd, 1996). One negative aspect of
online relationship formation that has been researched is the use of deception in online
behavior. Many times people pretend to be someone else or lie about aspects of their life
types of online behavior, but it has also looked at various settings in which CMC and the
Internet are used. The Internet and its CMC capabilities have been shown to be a way for
16
(Scheerhorn et al., 1995). Bresnahan and Murray-Johnson (2002) found that women
dealing with menopause and midlife transition experienced social support via CMC. A
more traditional area of Internet and CMC research has been in the realm of
organizational use of the technology. Much of the research looking at organizations’ uses
of CMC and the Internet has focused on aspects of group interactions such as decision-
complete tasks, and lower satisfaction among members (Baltes, Dickson, Sherman,
Researchers have also examined CMC and the Internet from a uses and
gratifications perspective. Much has been found about people’s use of CMC and Internet
respondents had used the Internet to meet someone new. McKenna and her colleagues
(2002) also found that people are using the Internet to form new relationships with people
People are using the Internet as a way of maintaining relationships with friends and
family whom they know offline (McKenna et al., 2002). In particular, mediated
communication seems to play a role in the maintenance of one type of relationship: long-
distance relationships (Dainton & Aylor, 2002). Although the telephone has been found
17
to be a popular means of long distance communication, computer-mediated
communication has become a way for many people to maintain relationships across a
distance. One explanation that Dainton and Aylor (2002) propose for people’s choice to
of the medium.
Papacharissi and Rubin (2000) also found that interpersonal utility and
convenience were two motives for people’s computer use along with information
seeking, entertainment, and pass time. Flanagin and Metzger (2001) found ten motive
clusters for people’s Internet use including information, learning, playing, leisure,
insight. These researchers found that mediated interpersonal technologies, including both
CMC and the telephone, were used mainly for social bonding, relationship maintenance,
antecedents that affect people’s motives (Rosengren, 1974). Prior research has tended to
associate those who are less socially skilled and lonelier as more likely to be users of the
McKenna and her colleagues (2002) found that the Internet is a helpful way for people
who are shy, lack social skills, or have social anxiety to form relationships. Specifically,
they found that those who are socially anxious and lonely feel that they can better express
themselves on the Internet than with the people they know offline. Caplan (2002) also
18
found that people who have higher levels of depression, shyness, and loneliness and
Other research has found a difference in Internet usage between those adolescents
who are more socially isolated and who have problems forming intimate friendships and
those who do not. Adolescents who have difficulty in forming intimate friendships were
more likely to be Internet users (Mesch, 2001). Other researchers have even found that
the Internet has a greater importance for people who are less satisfied with their social
interactions and that they use the Internet as a functional alternative to face-to-face
communication (Papacharissi & Rubin, 2000). Papacharissi and Rubin (2000) found that
people who found interpersonal communication to be less rewarding and were anxious
about communicating in face-to-face situations used the Internet for interpersonal utility.
studied in relation to the Internet and CMC usage. Matei and Ball-Rokeach (2001) found
that belonging to offline community groups increases the likelihood of a person forming
relationships online. McKenna and her colleagues (2002) also found that those who were
“friend-rich” offline became even richer when they got online, but also that those who
were “friend-poor” increased their social circle when they got online. One particular type
including CMC, is long-distance relationships. Many college students have used the
Internet and its CMC capabilities to stay in touch with friends from high school, who
attend other universities, which indicate that today’s college students have broader social
19
networks than ever before (Jones, 2002). Involvement in large networks may be one
characteristics such as age and gender. Whitty (2002) argues that demographic
characteristics need to be examined when dealing with online activity. Weiser (2000)
found that males use the Internet for entertainment and leisure, whereas women use the
majors. Anderson (2001) found that students in the hard science majors (chemistry,
computer science, engineering, math, and physics) might be more likely to become
Internet dependent.
Much of the research regarding people’s use of e-mail has looked at its presence
in organizational settings. Golden and her colleagues (1992) concluded that due to e-
mail’s lack of nonverbal cues, members of organizations are more likely to use it to send
task-related messages. Other research regarding organization use of e-mail suggests that
the use of e-mail by key leaders within an organization, who may apply direct or indirect
pressure, assist in the diffusion process of e-mail usage within the organization (Golden
Another area of research regarding e-mail has been the comparison of the CMC
technology with other forms of communication. The use of e-mail has been compared to
other forms of mediated communication such as voice mail. Marold and Larsen (1999)
20
found that e-mail was preferred to voice mail by a sample of staff, faculty, and
traditional mode of communication: printed text, such as letters and printed documents.
Hill and Monk (2000) found that e-mail was no less persuasive than a printed document
received through the mail. They also found that the mode of communication (e-mail or
print communication) did not affect the way messages and the senders of these messages
gratifications perspective as well. What many researchers have found is that people are
recent study, Dimmick and his colleagues (2000) compared gratifications of e-mail and
telephone usage. They found that e-mail’s ability to allow for asynchronous
communication was an advantage it held over the telephone. E-mail allowed people to
communicate with friends and family who lived far away, or in different time zones, and
with those who they did not have time to connect with in person. These researchers
found that a gratification that e-mail fulfilled is the ability to communicate and maintain
relationships with people even when face-to-face and telephone communication is not
presents are part of the reason that people are motivated to use it as a means of sustaining
relationships. Some of these gratification opportunities include that it is faster than postal
mail, but cheaper than the telephone (Stafford et al., 1999). E-mail is also a convenient
21
way to voice opinions, share information and ideas with friends and family who lived in
different time zones and different places and those with different schedules (Stafford et
al., 1999). Information seeking and entertainment have been found to be motives for the
Stafford and her colleagues (1999) found that demographic characteristics such as
age, income level, and education signify home e-mail users from nonusers. Home e-mail
users are younger and have a higher level of income and education than nonusers.
Gender also seems to be a factor in use of e-mail. Females tend to communicate via e-
mail more than males (Weiser, 2000). E-mail also tends to be a technology more likely
Instant Messaging
available on general CMC/Internet usage and e-mail usage. The research that has been
widely used by a younger population. Teenagers, in particular, use it to ask each other
out, to break up with each other, and to make plans with friends (Lenhart, Rainie, &
Lewis, 2001). Hard af Segerstad and Ljungstrand (2002) found that college students use
coordinate social activities. This finding suggests that instant messaging is used for both
task and social activities and has instrumental and ritual uses. Leung (2001) found that
students’ motives for ICQ differed depending on whether they were heavy or light users.
22
Heavy use of ICQ was motivated by affection and sociability; light ICQ use on the other
hand was motivated by keeping up with fashion trends. Leung (2001) found that males
used ICQ to fill time between classes; females on the other hand used ICQ to show or
seek affection and to socialize with friends. One aspect of instant messaging that is an
advantage it holds over the telephone is its ability to enable users to multi-task. Instant
al., 2001).
Research regarding online chat rooms is another area that has not been researched
as heavily as general CMC or e-mail usage. It has been explored however, more than
instant messaging. One particular area of interest has focused on users’ perceptions of
those with whom they are communicating. Markey and Wells (2002) found that in group
interactions in chat rooms people saw little difference among the personalities of the
various people with whom they were interacting. One reason given by the researchers for
this finding is the chaotic nature of group chat rooms. People’s communication behavior
has also been an area of interest regarding chat rooms, particularly that of deceptive
behavior. Whitty (2002) found that people who spend more time in chat rooms are more
likely to be open about themselves. He also found some gender differences in regard to
deceptiveness. Men are more likely to lie than women, and specifically they are more
Another area of interest with chat rooms is linguistics, examining aspects of the
statements used in chat rooms. Research has found that aspects of people’s statements
23
have an effect on the reciprocal communication in a chat room. The length of statements
(Rollman, Krug, & Parente, 2000). The type of statements used in chat rooms also has an
derogatory to a specific person in a chat room or to the entire group in a chat room, elicit
a higher amount of reciprocal communication than other types of statements (Rollman &
Parente, 2001). When Stone and Pennebaker (2002) examined the content of
conversations in chat rooms following the death of Princess Diana, they found that
Some limited research has looked at people’s motives for using online chat
rooms. Motivations for online chat rooms include seeking conversation and initiating
relationships with others (Peris et al., 2002). Whitty (2002) found that 63% of
respondents found some emotional support from the use of chat rooms. Stone and
Pennebaker (2002) examined the use of chat rooms for coping following the death of
Princess Diana and found that some people use online chat rooms as a way of coping
with trauma.
Researchers have examined characteristics about the people who use online chat
rooms. Peris et al. (2002) found, contrary to the belief of many, shyness and emotional
instability were not characteristics of chat users as a whole. Peris et al. (2002) found that
people who engage in and fulfill social needs through online chat are just as able to fulfill
these needs through face-to-face interactions as well. In fact, Peris et al. (2002)
24
concluded that online relationships are healthy and complement face-to-face
relationships. A finding that may contradict that of Peris et al. (2002) is that of Markey
and Wells (2002). They found that individuals who were introverts became moderate
extraverts in chat rooms, which may indicate that the anonymous nature provided by chat
rooms allow individuals to be less shy and more outgoing. Rumbough (2001) also found
a difference between men and women’s CMC use in that women are more likely than
men to disclose personal information about themselves in chat rooms. Age is another
demographic characteristic that has been explored. People aged 21-55 found more
emotional support from chat rooms than did those aged 17-20 (Whitty, 2002).
A goal of this proposed study is to build on the literature of general CMC and e-
mail uses and gratifications, but also to expand the literature on uses and gratifications of
instant messaging and online chat rooms. I would like to answer the following questions:
What are the uses college students report for e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat
rooms? What motives drive college students’ use of e-mail, instant messaging, and
online chat rooms? Are there differences between e-mail, instant messaging, and online
chat rooms regarding uses and motives? Are there social, psychological, and
demographic antecedents that affect CMC use? Three constructs are contained in the
previous questions, which will be explored in this study: uses of CMC, motives for use
of CMC, and social, psychological, and demographic antecedents that affect motives.
Uses of CMC refer to the reasons that college students give for using a particular
form of CMC and with whom they are communicating via CMC. For example, a use
25
would be if a college student reports using e-mail to contact classmates. Knowing the
reasons that college students report using CMC technologies can add valuable
information to this area of research. Research has found that CMC users communicate
with friends and family, and in particular, those who live far away (Dainton & Aylor,
2002; McKenna et al., 2002). Other research has found that people are using CMC and
the Internet as a means of meeting new people (Rumbough, 2001). This study attempts
to explore the specific uses of e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms by
college students and discover any differences in the uses between these three forms of
CMC. Researchers have also been concerned with discovering people’s motives for
using CMC.
this case a particular form of CMC. Several motives have been found by researchers for
entertainment, and to pass time are motives for Internet usage found by Papacharissi and
Rubin (2000). Other researchers have found that motives for interpersonal mediated
problem solving (Flanagin & Metzger, 2001). Researchers have looked at motives of
specific forms of CMC. Peris et al. (2002) found that seeking conversation and initiating
relationships were two motives for chat room use. More research needs to be done
examining the specific motives for e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat room use,
26
The uses and motives that have been identified for CMC thus far by researchers
seem to fall along two continuums. They appear to be either task or social related or
instrumental or ritualized. These two factors have been found in past uses and
gratifications research. Rubin (1984) found that people’s motives for television viewing
respectively, of media use. Past research in television viewing has also indicated that
motives for such activity include seeking information, which coincides with the idea of a
task-related motive, and using television to seek interpersonal connection with others,
which coincides with the idea of a social-related motive (Rubin & Bantz, 1987). In more
recent research regarding CMC use, Papacharissi and Rubin (2000) concluded that there
are instrumental and ritualized elements to people’s motives for Internet usage. People
who use the Internet to find information may be more instrumental in their usage than
those who use it to pass time or for entertainment, these motives may be more ritualized
in nature. Examining how people’s motives and uses fall along these continuums may
provide information indicating that a particular form of CMC may be more instrumental
in use or more ritual in use; this study will attempt to look at this aspect of CMC use.
This study will also examine antecedents for CMC use. Social, psychological,
and demographic antecedents refer to variables about people or their social situation that
social skills have been studied by researchers looking at CMC. A great deal of research
has found that people who are shy, lonely, anxious about communicating, and less
satisfied with their social relationships are more likely to use the Internet or CMC
27
(Caplan, 2002; Markey & Wells, 2002; McKenna et al., 2002; Papacharissi & Rubin,
2000). Peris et al. (2002) found contradicting evidence for these past claims; they found
that shyness and emotional instability were not characteristics of Internet chat users. The
Another social characteristic of college students that might relate to their CMC
usage is their level of involvement in student activities. This has not been studied
few of the many possible activities. Given that CMC use has taken hold in the college
student population with almost half using it to contact classmates and 42% using it
primarily to communicate socially (Jones, 2002), it holds that students will use CMC to
many times more task-related than social-related, which may indicate a different type of
CMC use.
CMC use has also been shown to be affected by demographic antecedents. One
such antecedent that has been previously studied is gender; males and females have been
found to use the Internet and CMC differently from each other. Females tend to use the
Internet for interpersonal communication and communicate via e-mail more often than
males; whereas males use the Internet more for leisure (Weiser, 2000). Age has also
been explored by researchers indicating that many of the CMC technologies are used
28
more often by a younger audience (Weiser, 2000). Age differences in chat room use has
also been looked at and results suggest that people aged 21-55 find more support from
online chat rooms than their 17-20 counterparts (Whitty, 2002). While most college
students are within a relatively small age difference from one another, the previous
finding suggests that there may be a difference between first and second year college
students and those who are in their third or fourth year. So, age and year in school may
indicate differences in CMC usage. Anderson’s (2001) finding that Internet use differs
across academic major also points to a demographic antecedent unique to the college
The first goal of this study is to explore the uses, or reasons, college students
report for using e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms. Other studies have
failed to separate CMC into specific forms to examine the differences between the
various forms of CMC. The following research questions are posed to attempt to reach
this goal:
RQ1c: What are college students’ uses for online chat rooms?
RQ1d: Do the uses of e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms differ
Research findings indicate that people use e-mail as a way of communicating with
others who live far away such as friends and family (Dimmick, Kline, & Stafford, 2000;
29
Stafford, Kline, & Dimmick, 1999). Research on instant messaging has shown that
people use it to communicate with people they also interact with in face-to-face settings
on a regular basis such as friends and classmates for reasons such as collaborating on
assignments and planning social activities (Hard af Segerstad & Ljungstrand, 2002;
Lenhart et al., 2001). Research on online chat rooms has found that a use of this type of
CMC is to meet new people (Peris et al., 2002). Also research in the area of
organizations suggests that e-mail is used to send task-related messages (Golden et al.,
1992). Instant messaging, however, is used for both task and social activities (Hard af
Segerstad & Ljungstrand, 2002). Online chat rooms have also been found to be used for
social activities (Pennebaker, 2002; Peris et al, 2002; Whitty, 2002). These findings
H1a: College students will use e-mail more than instant messaging to
communicate with members of their social networks who do not live in the same
geographic area.
H1b: College students will use e-mail more than online chat rooms to
communicate with members of their social networks who do not live in the same
geographic area.
H2a: College students will use e-mail more than online chat rooms to
H2b: College students will use instant messaging more than online chat rooms to
30
H2c: College students will use online chat rooms more than e-mail to
H2d: College students will use online chat rooms more than instant messaging to
H3a: College students will use e-mail more than online chat rooms for task-
related communication.
H3b: College students will use instant messaging more than online chat rooms
H4a: College students will use online chat rooms more than e-mail for social-
related communication.
H4b: College students will use instant messaging more than e-mail for social-
related communication.
The second goal of this study is to examine the motives, or gratifications sought,
of college students for e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms. Previous
research has identified motives for CMC in general and for certain types of CMC, but
they have not attempted to compare the motives for different forms of CMC to identify
differences between the forms, possibly along the continuums of task-social and
RQ2b: What are college students’ motives for instant messaging use?
RQ2c: What are college students’ motives for online chat room use?
31
RQ2d: Do the motives of e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms differ
Research has found that the speed and inexpensiveness of e-mail has been a
motive for people’s use of this form of CMC (Stafford et al., 1999). Maintaining
relationships has also been reported as a motive of both e-mail and instant messaging use
(Dimmick et al., 2000; Leung, 2001; Stafford et al., 1999). These motives, however,
have not been reported in the literature regarding online chat rooms. Research has shown
that a motive for people’s use of online chat rooms is emotional support (Pennebaker,
H5: College students will report convenience as a motive for using e-mail more
H6a: College students will report maintaining relationships as a motive for using
H6b: College students will report maintaining relationships as a motive for using
H7: College students will report emotional support as a motive for using online
Past research has found support for the idea of people having both instrumental
and ritualized uses for the Internet (Papacharissi & Rubin, 2000). Based on this finding,
it seems likely that people would have instrumental and ritualized uses for technologies
available through the Internet such as e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms.
32
The next hypotheses concern instrumental and ritual uses of CMC. Because I expect
college students to report instrumental uses for e-mail more than for online chat rooms:
H8a: College students will report information seeking as a motive for e-mail
H8b: College students will report interpersonal utility as a motive for e-mail
Because I expect college students to report ritualized uses for instant messaging more
H8c: College students will report convenience as a motive for instant messaging
H8d: College students will report passing time as a motive for instant messaging
H8e: College students will report entertainment as a motive for instant messaging
Because I expect college students to report ritualized uses for online chat rooms more
H8f: College students will report passing time as a motive for online chat rooms
H8g: College students will report entertainment as a motive for online chat rooms
The third goal of this study is to identify social, psychological, and demographic
antecedents of e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat room use. Previous research
33
has identified antecedents, but some of the findings have been contradictory, and
previous findings have not often identified the difference in motives and uses of CMC in
terms of these antecedents. Four specific areas of antecedents will be examined in this
study.
The first of these antecedents is level of social adeptness of CMC users. This
concept will be explored by looking at four psychological conditions that affect social
skills that have been commonly found in past research on Internet and CMC use:
others” (Leary, 1991, p. 182). Loneliness can be defined as the emotional response to the
discrepancy between desired and actual social contact (Shaver & Brennan, 1991).
which is the likeliness of a person engaging in communication with others, and Reward,
which is how rewarding people find communication with others (Rubin, 1994).
217). Because of the contradicting findings in this area of research, the following
RQ3a: Is shyness related to college students’ uses and motives for e-mail, instant
34
RQ3b: Is loneliness related to college students’ uses and motives for e-mail,
uses and motives for e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms?
communication, and anxiousness about oral communication are more likely to use the
Internet and CMC (Caplan, 2002; Markey & Wells, 2002; McKenna et al., 2002;
Papacharissi & Rubin, 2000). These findings prompt the following hypotheses:
H9a: Shyness will be positively related to amount of time spent in online chat
rooms.
messaging.
H10a: Loneliness will be positively related to amount of time spent in online chat
rooms.
messaging.
35
H11a: Communication avoidance will be positively related to amount of time
spent e-mailing.
The second antecedent that will be explored in this study is the offline social
networks of college students. As not much prior research regarding social networks and
CMC use has been examined, another goal of this study is to explore whether college
students who are involved in a large number of offline social relationships/networks have
36
different motives and uses for e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms, than
college students who are not involved in a large number of offline social
relationships/networks.
While research has not previously looked at how the size of a person’s social
network affects Internet or CMC usage, people who are a part of a large social network
follows with the findings that people use e-mail and instant messaging as a way to
maintain relationships. Thus CMC usage may be higher for those types of CMC that
enable people with large social networks to be able to easily maintain these large
The research that has looked at the types of relationships CMC users are involved
in has found that people involved in long distance relationships use mediated
communication as a way of maintaining the relationship (Jones, 2002; Matei & Ball-
Rokeach, 2001). Another interest of this study is whether the type of offline social
relationship affects college students’ motives and uses of e-mail, instant messaging, and
37
H14a: Involvement in long distance relationships will be positively related to
The third antecedent that will be examined in this study is college students’ level
and student government organizations. Although research has not been conducted to
examine this specific antecedent, involvement in student activities may prompt more
task-related or instrumental use of forms of CMC. So another goal of this study was to
explore if college students who are involved in student activities have different motives
and uses for e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms than students who are not
The final antecedent that will be explored in this study is the demographic
are gender, age, and academic major. Females have been found to use e-mail more than
males (Weiser, 2000). Males and females report different motives for instant messaging
use as well. Males report using instant messaging to pass time; women report using
instant messaging to maintain relationships (Leung, 2001). Age of CMC users has also
shown a difference in instant messaging use. Whitty (2002) found that people aged 17-
20 reported finding less emotional support than people aged 21-55. Another finding
38
regarding demographic characteristics of CMC users is that college students in the hard
science majors are more likely to become dependent on the Internet (Anderson, 2001).
So, the final area of study focused on differences in how gender, age, and academic major
are revealed in motives and uses of e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms.
H16: Female college students will spend more time e-mailing than male college
students.
H17: Males will report passing time as a motive for instant messaging use more
than females.
H19: College students who are 17-20 years of age will use instant messaging
H20: College students who are 17-20 years of age will report emotional support
as a motive for using online chat rooms less than college students 21 years of age or
older.
H21a: College students in the hard science majors (chemistry, computer science,
engineering, math, and physics) will use e-mail more than students in majors other than
hard sciences.
H21b: College students in the hard science majors (chemistry, computer science,
engineering, math, and physics) will use instant messaging more than students in majors
39
H21c: College students in the hard science majors (chemistry, computer science,
engineering, math, and physics) will use online chat rooms more than students in majors
These research questions and hypotheses will be tested in the present study.
40
Chapter 2
METHOD
Procedure
This study was designed to examine college students’ uses and motives as well as
communication: e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms. In Spring 2003, a
complete them at home and brought back to class. Respondents were informed that their
messaging, and online chat rooms uses and motives, and the psychological and social
following demographic characteristics were also obtained: gender, age, year in school,
41
The Nature of the Sample
undergraduate classes at the University of Delaware. The sample was 37.7% (n = 168)
male and 62.3% (n = 278) female. The respondents ranged from 18 to 33 years of age,
with the mean age being 19.80 years (SD = 1.52). For statistical analysis, the respondents
were divided into two age categories: 1) those 17 to 20 years of age and 2) those 21
years of age or older. The sample was 70.2% (n = 313) 17 to 20 years of age and 29.7%
The respondents were instructed to indicate their year in school by circling it from
a list provided. First-years accounted for 30.5% of the sample (n = 136), sophomores
33.6% (n = 150), juniors 16.6% (n = 74), and seniors 18.8% (n = 84). Two respondents
(0.4%) indicated “Other” as their class standing. The respondents were instructed to
indicate their major by writing it into a blank. The overall sample represented 61 majors
accounted for 15.2% of the sample (n = 68). The second largest group was
Communication majors, which accounted for 13.9% (n = 62). The third largest group
was Economics majors, which accounted for 6.5% (n = 29). The fourth largest group was
Exercise Science majors, which accounted for 6.3% (n = 28). In order to test hypothesis
21, the majors were recoded to represent hard science majors (3.6%, n = 16) and nonhard
42
Computer Access and Experience
computer and to the Internet where they lived right now. Virtually all students had access
to a computer (98.0%, n = 437) and to the Internet (97.3%, n = 434). The respondents
who had access to the Internet were also instructed to indicate the type of Internet
connection they had by circling it from a list provided. About half of all respondents
(50.9%, n = 227) indicated using an Ethernet connection. Cable High Speed Access was
the second highest indicated (20.0%, n = 89) followed by Dial-up Modem (15.5%,
n = 69). Respondents were also asked to estimate their own computer expertise on a
Uses
Uses of e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms were measured by
presenting subjects with a list of 5-point Likert-type statements for each of the three
forms of CMC being studied. The lists were generated from the findings of a number of
studies (Dimmick et al., 2000; Hard af Segerstad & Ljungstrand, 2002; Lenhart et al.,
2001; Stafford et al., 1999). Prior to responding to each of the three lists of uses, subjects
were asked a conditional question to assess whether they had used the particular form of
CMC under question. If so, they then responded to the list of statements, if not, they
moved onto the next section. The same statements were used for each of the three forms
43
The list of uses presented statements that contained task and social-related uses.
Task-related uses included statements such as “I use e-mail to ask professors about
material covered in class.” Social-related uses included statements such as “I use e-mail
to keep in touch with friends.” See Table 1 for the list of statements and descriptive
statistics.
Respondents were also asked to report how many minutes they spend online on a
typical day using e-mail (M = 39.90, SD = 139.85), using instant messaging (M = 204.34,
SD = 1006.90), and using online chat rooms (M = 68.22, SD = 225.28). The length of
time, in years, that respondents had used e-mail (M = 7.64, SD = 13.33), instant
messaging (M = 6.18, SD = 10.72), and online chat rooms (M = 10.80, SD = 23.40) was
also measured.
44
Table 1
1. Keep in touch with friends. 3.94 0.96 4.60 0.70 2.98 1.33
3. Keep in touch with friends or relatives who 3.86 1.01 3.87 1.14 2.53 1.33
live far away.
4. Keep in touch with people I’ve met online. 1.71 1.05 1.93 1.22 2.09 1.26
5. Let professors know why I missed/will miss 3.58 1.05 1.64 1.13 1.62 1.04
class.
6. Ask professors questions about material 3.53 1.05 1.65 1.08 1.66 1.10
covered in class.
7. Coordinate group assignments with 3.36 1.05 3.00 1.17 2.12 1.27
classmates.
8. Keep in touch with boyfriend/ girlfriend. 2.75 1.40 3.23 1.48 1.99 1.21
10. Ask classmates questions about material 2.98 1.12 3.46 1.01 2.16 1.31
covered in class.
11. Find others who have the same interests. 1.47 0.83 1.76 1.05 2.27 1.24
12. Keep in touch with people I only know 1.82 1.22 2.88 1.56 2.33 1.35
online.
45
Table 1 continued
________________________________________________________________________
13. Keep in touch with family or relatives. 3.80 1.06 3.77 1.15 2.31 1.32
14. Make friends of the opposite sex. 1.45 0.85 1.86 1.13 2.03 1.22
15. Keep in touch with boyfriend/girlfriend who 2.46 1.44 2.96 1.54 1.72 1.11
lives far away.
16. Talk to business and professional contacts. 2.74 1.29 2.05 1.20 1.57 0.99
17. Send and receive files. 3.58 1.01 3.34 1.16 2.20 1.34
________________________________________________________________________
46
Motives
Motives for using e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms were
measured using 5-point Likert-type statements for each of the three forms of CMC being
studied. The lists of motives have been generated from the findings of a number of
different studies (Lenhart et al., 2001; Leung, 2001; Papacharissi & Rubin, 2000; Stone
& Pennebaker, 2002; Peris et al., 2002; Stafford et al., 1999; Whitty, 2002). Prior to
responding to each of the three lists of uses, subjects were asked a conditional question of
whether they have used the particular form of CMC under question. If so, they then
responded to the list of statements, if not, they moved onto the next section. The same
statements were used for each of the three forms of CMC, but in a different order so as
There were four clerical errors in the preparation of the questionnaire. The
statement “Because I enjoy answering questions” was inadvertently not included in the
instant messaging and online chat rooms lists of motives. The statement “Because I
wonder what other people said” was inadvertently not included in the e-mail list of
motives. The wording of two statements, “Because people don’t have to be there to
receive e-mail” and “Because it is easier to e-mail than to talk to some people,” was not
47
Table 2
I use (technology): M SD M SD M SD
________________________________________________________________________
8. To feel involved with what’s going on with 3.05 1.23 3.20 1.20 2.20 1.16
other people.
9. Because I need someone to talk to or be with. 2.06 1.07 2.65 1.17 1.97 1.06
10. Because I just need to talk about my 2.20 1.15 2.85 1.22 1.89 1.12
problems sometimes.
11. To feel less inhibited when I communicate. 2.38 1.13 2.67 1.21 2.28 1.22
48
Table 2 continued
________________________________________________________________________
I use (technology): M SD M SD M SD
________________________________________________________________________
16. To give me something to occupy my time. 2.55 1.16 3.76 1.11 2.90 1.24
17. Just because it is available. 3.14 1.15 3.84 1.03 2.87 1.70
18. When I have nothing better to do. 2.60 1.15 3.80 1.11 2.87 1.22
19. Because it’s thrilling. 1.84 1.01 2.33 1.16 1.98 1.04
20. When there is no one else to talk or be with. 2.13 1.10 3.06 1.30 2.35 1.24
21. Because it passes the time away, 2.48 1.19 3.74 1.15 2.76 1.26
particularly when I am bored.
22. To show others encouragement. 2.46 1.20 2.98 1.23 2.04 1.10
23. To feel connected to other people. 2.98 1.22 3.36 1.20 2.30 1.14
24. Because it makes me feel less lonely. 1.99 1.07 2.49 1.27 2.04 1.09
25. Because it’s a habit, just something I do. 2.88 1.23 3.69 1.15 2.57 1.28
26. Because it is more comfortable than talking 2.49 1.16 2.77 1.21 2.45 1.20
to people face to face.
27. So I won’t have to feel alone. 1.80 0.94 2.10 1.17 1.88 1.00
28. Because I can express myself freely. 2.56 1.19 2.78 1.22 2.32 1.16
49
Table 2 continued
________________________________________________________________________
I use (technology): M SD M SD M SD
________________________________________________________________________
30. So I can get away from what I’m doing. 2.46 1.80 3.30 1.18 2.54 1.23
31. To tell others what to do. 2.18 1.04 2.41 1.18 1.84 1.07
32. To get someone to do something for me. 2.49 1.11 2.63 1.21 1.79 0.94
33. Because I can pretend to be anyone I want 1.57 0.91 1.77 1.11 2.03 1.12
to be when interacting with other people online.
36. To get more points of view. 2.26 1.15 2.94 1.24 2.13 1.13
38. To get information for free. 3.20 1.31 2.90 1.36 2.36 1.35
39. To look for information. 3.03 1.26 2.52 1.29 2.14 1.19
40. To see what is out there. 2.66 1.29 2.44 1.21 2.36 1.14
41. I just like to use it. 3.32 1.19 4.16 0.98 2.96 1.31
43. To let others know I care about their 3.08 1.25 3.42 1.23 2.05 1.02
feelings.
44. Because I am concerned about others. 3.03 1.27 3.38 1.19 2.02 1.10
50
Table 2 continued
________________________________________________________________________
I use (technology): M SD M SD M SD
________________________________________________________________________
45. Because it is a pleasant rest. 2.44 1.16 3.50 1.08 2.37 1.17
46. Because it makes me feel less tense. 2.18 1.07 3.01 1.18 2.14 1.13
47. To get away from pressures and 2.28 1.15 3.27 1.18 2.35 1.20
responsibilities.
48. To put something off that I should be doing. 2.89 1.27 3.98 1.09 2.40 1.27
49. To forget about my problems. 2.05 1.04 2.58 1.17 2.02 1.06
50. So I can have control over when and if I 2.90 1.26 3.22 1.27 2.33 1.17
respond.
51. So I can have electronic copies of my 2.65 1.26 2.09 1.17 2.23 1.18
correspondence.
52. Because I wonder what other people said.** - - 3.02 1.16 2.14 1.16
Note. 1 = Strongly Disagree, 2 = Disagree Some, 3 = Agree Some and Disagree Some,
4 = Agree Some, 5 = Strongly Agree.
*motive statements with very low salience that were not included in further analyses.
**
motive statements that were affected by a clerical error.
51
Measures
Shyness
Shyness was measured using the nine-item Shyness Scale (Cheek & Buss, 1981).
Respondents were asked to answer on a 5-point Likert-type scale how much they agreed
summarizes the means and standard deviations of the statements. The scale has been
used reliably in prior research (α = .79, Cheek & Buss, 1981). To create an overall
shyness score for each respondent, the responses to the items in the scale were summed
and averaged. The mean score was 2.40 (SD = 0.76) and the median was 2.44. The
Cronbach alpha reliability coefficient for the nine-item scale was .87.
52
Table 3
M SD
________________________________________________________________________
3. I feel tense when I’m with people I don’t know well. 2.65 1.04
________________________________________________________________________
Note. 1 = Strongly Disagree, 2 = Disagree Some, 3 = Agree Some and Disagree Some,
4 = Agree Some, 5 = Strongly Agree.
53
Loneliness
Loneliness was measured using the 20-item revised UCLA Loneliness Scale
(Russell, Peplau, & Ferguson, 1980). Respondents were asked to answer on a 5-point
Likert-type scale how much they agreed (from 1 = “Strongly Disagree” to 5 = “Strongly
Table 4 summarizes the means and standard deviations of the statements. The scale has
been used reliably in prior research (α = .94, Shaver & Brennan, 1991). To create an
overall loneliness score for each respondent, the responses to the items in the scale were
summed and averaged. The mean score was 1.97 (SD = 0.69) and the median was 1.78.
The Cronbach alpha reliability coefficient for the 20-item scale was .94.
54
Table 4
M SD
________________________________________________________________________
6. I have a lot in common with the people around me.* 2.06 1.04
16. There are people who really understand me.* 1.96 1.00
18. People are around me but not with me. 2.05 1.02
55
Table 4 continued
________________________________________________________________________
M SD
________________________________________________________________________
________________________________________________________________________
Note. 1 = Strongly Disagree, 2 = Disagree Some, 3 = Agree Some and Disagree Some,
4 = Agree Some, 5 = Strongly Agree.
*recoded items.
56
Unwillingness to Communicate
Communicate Scale. This measure assessed two dimensions using a 20-item scale: 10
items assess Approach-Avoidance, which is how likely it is that a person will engage in
interaction with others, and 10 items assess Reward, which is how rewarding people find
a 5-point Likert-type scale. Table 5 summarizes the means and standard deviations of the
statements. The scale has been used reliably in prior research: coefficient alphas for the
Approach-Avoidance dimension of the scale have ranged from .85 to .98 and for the
Reward dimension of the scale have ranged from .70 to .95 (Rubin, 1994). To create an
overall Approach-Avoidance score for each respondent, the responses to the items that
coincide with the Approach-Avoidance dimension in the scale were summed and
averaged. To create an overall Reward score for each respondent, the responses to the
items that coincide with the Reward dimension in the scale were summed and averaged.
The mean score for the Approach-Avoidance dimension was 2.56 (SD = 0.72) and the
median was 2.60. The Cronbach alpha reliability coefficient for the Approach-
Avoidance dimension was .87. The mean score for the Reward dimension was 3.94
(SD = 0.58) and the median was 4.00. The Cronbach alpha reliability coefficient for the
57
Table 5
M SD
________________________________________________________________________
Approach-Avoidance Dimension:
Reward Dimension:
1. My friends and family don’t listen to my ideas and suggestions.* 4.10 0.91
3. I don’t ask for advice from family or friends when I have to make 3.88 1.04
decisions.*
5. My family doesn’t enjoy discussing my interests and activities with 4.24 0.95
me.*
58
Table 5 continued
________________________________________________________________________
M SD
________________________________________________________________________
7. Other people are friendly only because they want something out of 3.84 0.96
me.*
10. I don’t think my friends are honest in their communication with me.* 4.00 0.99
________________________________________________________________________
Note. 1 = Strongly Disagree, 2 = Disagree Some, 3 = Agree Some and Disagree Some,
4 = Agree Some, 5 = Strongly Agree.
*recoded items.
59
Interpersonal Communication Satisfaction
conversation in general (Rubin, Perse, & Barbato, 1988). Respondents indicated their
5 = “Strongly Agree”). Table 6 summarizes the means and standard deviations of the
statements. The Cronbach alpha reliability coefficient for the scale has ranged from .72
score for each respondent, the responses to the items in the scale were summed and
averaged. The mean score was 3.80 (SD = 0.64) and the median was 3.86. The
60
Table 6
M SD
________________________________________________________________________
2. Other people express a lot of interest in what I have to say. 3.55 0.93
7. During conversations with others, I am able to present myself as I want 3.69 0.93
others to view me.
12. We usually talk about something I am NOT interested in.* 4.08 0.96
13. I would like to continue having conversations like the ones I have 3.70 0.99
now.
14. I feel like I can talk about anything with other people. 3.38 1.10
15. Other people show me they understand what I say. 3.66 0.94
________________________________________________________________________
Note. 1 = Strongly Disagree, 2 = Disagree Some, 3 = Agree Some and Disagree Some,
4 = Agree Some, 5 = Strongly Agree.
*recoded items.
61
Social Networks
Offline and online social networks were measured by providing respondents with
with and asking them to indicate, by writing in, how many people in each of the given
groups they communicate with both online and offline on a typical day. The respondents
were provided four separate lists, one for each of the technologies being studied: e-mail,
instant messaging, and online chat rooms, and one for face-to-face communication used
to indicate offline social networks. Some misunderstanding occurred with this measure.
Only 57.8% (n = 258) filled in numbers, the rest of the sample instead placed an “X” to
indicate that they do indeed communicate with people using this technology. For those
respondents who filled out the numerical measure the mean score was 31.48 (SD = 30.42)
and the median was 23.50. So, to test hypothesis 13, the responses were recoded by
summing the number of categories (i.e. groups of people) that respondents indicated they
communicated with face-to-face in order to get a score for involvement in offline social
networks. The mean score was 5.16 (SD = 1.75) and the median was 4.00.
by marking with an “X,” which activities they take part in both on campus and off
campus from a given list of activities. Respondents were also able to write in any
activities they are involved in that were not on the given list. In order to get a measure of
the number of activities in which respondents were involved, a variable was created that
62
summed the number of activities in which respondents indicated they were involved. The
average number of activities ranged from zero to eight. The mean score was 1.70
was needed. This measure was derived by recoding the use statement: “Keep in touch
with boyfriend/girlfriend who lives far away” to indicate whether or not a respondent was
respondents who were not involved in a long distance relationship and 44.6% (n = 199)
of respondents who were involved in a long distance relationship. This measure proved
Statistical Analysis
After scale construction and reliability analyses, several statistical analyses were
conducted to answer the research questions and test the hypotheses in this study. To
identify the uses and motives for each of the technologies, principal factor analyses with
oblique rotation were performed. Paired-sample t-tests were conducted to compare the
technologies in regard to college students’ uses and motives for them (Research
to assess the relationships between the psychological conditions and amount of time spent
using the technologies, as well as between college students’ involvement in offline social
networks and in social activities and amount of time spent using the technologies
(Research Question 3 and Hypotheses 9, 10, 11, 12, 13, and 15). A Paired sample t-test
63
was used to assess the relationship between college students’ involvement in long
distance relationships and amount of time spent using the technologies (Hypothesis 14).
Independent sample t-tests were also used to assess relationships between gender and
time spent using technologies, gender and motives, age and time spent using
technologies, age and motives, major and time spent using technologies (Hypotheses 16,
64
Chapter 3
RESULTS
The purpose of this chapter is to present the findings of this study. The statistical
analyses used to test the research questions and hypotheses presented in chapter one and
the results of these analyses will be discussed in greater length in the following section.
The research questions and hypotheses fall into five main areas of interest: uses, motives,
Uses
One goal of this study was to understand college students’ uses of e-mail, instant
messaging, and online chat rooms. The first set of research questions asked what these
uses are and whether they differ from one another. Specifically, research question 1a
asked about college students’ uses for e-mail. An exploratory principal factor analysis
with oblique rotation identified three major and two minor factors, which accounted for
56.1% of the variance. The first (eigenvalue = 4.56) was made up of four items marking
use of e-mail for task-related school activities. Factor two (eigenvalue = 2.33) was
comprised of five items reflecting developing and maintaining relationships online. The
third factor (eigenvalue = 1.27) consisted of three items that focused on using e-mail for
social contact with friends and relatives. The fourth minor factor (eigenvalue = .90)
reflected using e-mail to contact a boyfriend or girlfriend. The final minor factor
65
(eigenvalue = .48) was made up of three items dealing with task-related activities. Table
66
Table 7
1. Ask professors questions about material .92 .00 .00 .00 .00
covered in class.
2. Let professors know why I missed/will miss .74 .00 .00 .00 .00
class.
4. Ask classmates questions about material .41 .13 .00 .15 .28
covered in class.
5. Make friends of the opposite sex. .00 .81 -.11 .00 .00
7. Find others who have the same interests. .00 .78 .00 .00 .18
8. Keep in touch with people I’ve met online. .00 .67 .00 .00 -.11
9. Keep in touch with people I only know .00 .62 .00 .00 .00
online.
10. Keep in touch with friends or relatives who .00 .00 .94 .00 .00
live far away.
11. Keep in touch with family or relatives. .00 .00 .76 .00 .00
12. Keep in touch with friends. .00 .00 .61 .17 .00
13. Keep in touch with boyfriend/ girlfriend. .00 .00 .00 .90 .00
14. Keep in touch with boyfriend/girlfriend who .00 .00 .00 .80 .00
lives far away.
67
Table 7 continued
________________________________________________________________________
15. Talk to business and professional contacts. .00 .00 .00 .00 .68
17. Send and receive files. .12 .00 .14 .00 .30
________________________________________________________________________
Note. N = 441
68
Research question 1b asked about college students’ uses for instant messaging.
An exploratory principal factor analysis with oblique rotation identified three major and
two minor factors, which accounted for 59.9% of the variance. The first
(eigenvalue = 4.89) was made up of five items marking use of instant messaging for
developing and maintaining relationships online. Factor two (eigenvalue = 2.32) was
comprised of three items that focused on using instant messaging for social contact with
friends and relatives. The third factor (eigenvalue = 1.24) consisted of four items that
focused on using instant messaging for task-related activities. The fourth minor factor
The final minor factor (eigenvalue = .77) was made up of three items dealing with task-
69
Table 8
2. Keep in touch with people I’ve met online. .84 .00 .00 .00 .00
3. Find others who have the same interests. .77 .00 .15 .00 .00
4. Make friends of the opposite sex. .77 .00 .00 .00 .00
5. Keep in touch with people I only know .54 .00 .00 .00 .11
online.
6. Keep in touch with friends or relatives who .00 .84 .00 .00 .00
live far away.
7. Keep in touch with family or relatives. .00 .81 .14 .00 .00
9. Let professors know why I missed/will miss .00 .00 .93 .00 .00
class.
10. Ask professors questions about material .00 .00 .84 .00 .00
covered in class.
12. Talk to business and professional contacts. .19 .14 .42 .00 .00
13. Keep in touch with boyfriend/ girlfriend. .00 .00 .00 .97 .00
14. Keep in touch with boyfriend/girlfriend who .00 .00 .00 .76 .00
lives far away.
70
Table 8 continued
________________________________________________________________________
15. Coordinate group assignments with .00 .00 .00 .00 .88
classmates.
16. Ask classmates questions about material .00 .00 .00 .00 .72
covered in class.
17. Send and receive files. .19 .18 .00 .12 .28
________________________________________________________________________
Note. N = 394
71
Research question 1c focused on college students’ uses for online chat rooms.
Because only 93 respondents reported using online chat rooms, it was not possible to
between task and social activities and the maintenance of offline and online relationships,
performed using e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms, I created new scales,
guided by the results of the factor analyses. Two scales represented using e-mail, instant
messaging, and online chat rooms for either task or social activities. The task-related use
factor was created from the items that loaded on Factors 1 and 5 in the e-mail factor
analysis and on Factors 3 and 5 in the instant messaging factor analysis. The social-
related use factor was created from the items that loaded on Factors 2, 3, and 4 in the
e-mail factor analysis and on Factors 1, 2, and 4 in the instant messaging factor analysis.
Another three scales identified using e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms for
maintaining online, offline, and long distance relationships. To create these scales,
respondents’ scores on items were averaged. The online use factor was created from the
items that loaded on Factor 2 in the e-mail factor analysis and on Factor 1 in the instant
messaging factor analysis. The offline use factor was created from the items that loaded
on Factors 1, 3, 4 and one item from Factor 5 in the e-mail factor analysis and on Factors
2 and 4 and three items from Factor 3 and two items from Factor 5 in the instant
messaging factor analysis. The long distance use factor was created from two items that
asked about keeping in touch with people who live far away. One item loaded on Factor
4 in the e-mail factor analysis and on Factor 4 in the instant messaging factor analysis and
72
the second item loaded on Factor 3 in the e-mail factor analysis and on Factor 2 in the
instant messaging factor analysis. See Tables 9, 10, 11, 12, and 13 for descriptive
statistics.
73
Table 9
Task-Related Uses
________________________________________________________________________
________________________________________________________________________
74
Table 10
Social-Related Uses
________________________________________________________________________
________________________________________________________________________
75
Table 11
Online Uses
________________________________________________________________________
________________________________________________________________________
76
Table 12
Offline Uses
________________________________________________________________________
________________________________________________________________________
77
Table 13
________________________________________________________________________
78
The first set of hypotheses focused on the differences in the uses of e-mail, instant
messaging, and online chat rooms. The means of all uses are summarized in Table 14.
Hypothesis 1a predicted that college students will use e-mail more than instant
messaging to communicate with members of their social networks who do not live in the
same geographic area. This hypothesis was not supported, in fact, college students used
e-mail (M = 3.20) significantly less than instant messaging (M = 3.42) for communicating
Hypothesis 1b predicted that college students will use e-mail more than online
chat rooms to communicate with members of their social networks who do not live in the
same geographic area. This hypothesis was supported; college students used e-mail
(M = 3.20) significantly more than online chat rooms (M = 2.14) for communicating long
Hypothesis 2a predicted that college students will use e-mail more than online
chat rooms to communicate with people they know offline. This hypothesis was
supported; college students used e-mail (M = 3.32) significantly more than instant
messaging (M = 3.42) for communicating with people they know offline: t(392) = 8.86,
p < .001.
Hypothesis 2b predicted that college students will use instant messaging more
than online chat rooms to communicate with people they know offline. This hypothesis
was supported; college students used instant messaging (M = 3.10) significantly more
than online chat rooms (M = 2.04) for communicating with people they know offline:
79
Hypothesis 2c predicted that college students will use online chat rooms more
than e-mail to communicate with people they do not know offline. This hypothesis was
not supported; college students did not use online chat rooms (M = 2.19) significantly
more than e-mail (M = 2.08) for communicating with people they do not know offline:
Hypothesis 2d predicted that college students will use online chat rooms more
than instant messaging to communicate with people they do not know offline. This
hypothesis was not supported, in fact, college students used instant messaging (M = 2.63)
significantly more than online chat rooms (M = 2.19) for communicating with people
Hypothesis 3a predicted that college students will use e-mail more than online
chat rooms for task-related communication. This hypothesis was supported; college
students used e-mail (M = 3.24) significantly more than online chat rooms (M = 1.92) for
Hypothesis 3b predicted that college students will use instant messaging more
than online chat rooms for task-related communication. This hypothesis was supported;
college students used instant messaging (M = 2.74) significantly more than online chat
Hypothesis 4a predicted that college students will use online chat rooms more
than e-mail for social-related communication. This hypothesis was not supported, in fact,
college students used e-mail (M = 2.75) significantly more than online chat rooms
80
Hypothesis 4b predicted that college students will use instant messaging more
than e-mail for social-related communication. This hypothesis was supported; college
students used instant messaging (M = 2.78) significantly more than e-mail (M = 2.51) for
Research question 1d asked about the way the uses of e-mail, instant messaging,
and online chat rooms differ from one another. Beyond the differences identified in the
tests of Hypotheses 1, 2, 3 and 4, there were several other significant differences. First,
college students used e-mail (M = 3.19) significantly more than instant messaging
(M = 2.47) for task-related communication: t(392) = 18.19, p < .001. Second, college
students used instant messaging (M = 2.03) significantly more than e-mail (M = 1.61) for
communicating with people they do not know offline: t(392) = 11.59, p < .001. Third,
college students used e-mail (M = 3.32) significantly more than online chat rooms
(M = 2.07) for communicating with people they know offline: t(90) = 11.68, p < .001.
Fourth, college students used instant messaging (M = 3.01) significantly more than online
chat rooms (M = 2.24) for social-related communication: t(85) = 7.80, p < .001. Finally,
college students used instant messaging (M = 3.29) significantly more than online chat
rooms (M = 2.13) for communicating long distance: t(85) = 8.41, p < .001.
Other significant results that can be determined from these means are what
college students use the technology for the most and for the least. First, e-mail is used
most often for communicating with people known offline and least for communicating
with people known only online. Second, instant messaging is used most often for
communicating long distance and least for communicating with people only known
81
online. Third, online chat rooms are used most often for social-related communication
82
Table 14
________________________________________________________________________
83
Motives
A second goal of this study was to understand college students’ motives for using
e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms. The second set of research questions
asked what these motives are and whether they differ from one another. Specifically,
research question 2a focused on college students’ motives for e-mail use. An exploratory
principal factor analysis with oblique rotation identified six factors, accounting for 56.9%
of the variance. The criteria for factor retention were at least two loadings of at least .45.
The first (eigenvalue = 17.71) was made up of five items marking Interpersonal Utility as
a motive for e-mail use. Factor two (eigenvalue = 2.70) was comprised of four items
for using e-mail. A fourth item was eliminated to increase internal consistency. The
fourth factor (eigenvalue = 1.53) consisted of six items which reflected Passing Time as a
motive for e-mail use. The fifth factor (eigenvalue = 1.37) was made up of three items
reflecting Information Seeking as a motive for e-mail use. The sixth factor
(eigenvalue = 1.14) consisted of five items marking Convenience as a motive for e-mail
use. Table 15 summarizes the results of the factor analysis. To create measures of e-mail
84
Table 15
2. To let others know I care about .71 .15 .00 .00 .00 .00
their feelings.
4. To feel connected to other people. .53 .00 .11 .20 .00 .00
6. To feel involved with what’s going .45 .00 .19 .00 .00 .00
on with other people.
7. To get away from pressures and .11 .62 .12 .20 .13 .00
responsibilities.
8. Because it makes me feel less .12 .58 .19 .14 .00 .00
tense.
10. To forget about my problems. .13 .49 .00 .26 .00 .19
14. Because it relaxes me. .00 .19 .68 .00 .00 .00
85
Table 15 continued
________________________________________________________________________
15. When I have nothing better to do. .00 .00 .00 .82 .00 .00
16. Because it passes the time away, .00 .00 .00 .77 .00 .00
particularly when I am bored.
17. To give me something to occupy .00 .00 .15 .67 .00 .00
my time.
18. Just because it is available. .00 .00 .00 .54 .00 .14
19. To put something off that I should .16 .27 .00 .49 .00 .11
be doing.
20. When there is no one else to talk .00 .00 .00 .48 .00 .00
or be with.
21. To look for information. .00 .00 .00 .00 .99 .00
22. To see what is out there. .00 .00 .00 .00 .77 .00
23. To get information for free. .00 -.10 .00 .00 .68 .00
24. Because it is easier to e-mail than .00 .00 .00 .11 .00 .64
talk to some people.
25. Because it is more comfortable .00 .16 .00 .00 .00 .61
than talking to people face to face.
26. Because people don’t have to be .16 -.20 .13 .00 .11 .55
there to receive e-mail.
27. To feel less inhibited when I .00 .00 .00 .00 .00 .54
communicate.
86
Table 15 continued
________________________________________________________________________
28. Because I can express myself .00 .29 .00 .00 .00 .52
freely.
30. Because I just need to talk about .35 .00 .00 .19 .00 .13
my problems sometimes.
31. Because I need someone to talk to .28 .00 .00 .21 .00 .00
or be with.
32. To get more points of view. .25 .14 .00 .00 .15 .00
33. To tell others what to do. .18 -.10 .00 .10 .00 .00
34. To get someone to do something .21 -.13 .00 .00 .00 .10
for me.
35. Because it’s thrilling. .00 .26 .20 .14 .00 .00
36. Because it makes me feel less .18 .23 .00 .17 .00 .00
lonely.
37. Because I can pretend to be -.24 .16 .12 .00 .00 .00
anyone I want to be when interacting
with other people online.
38. Because it’s a habit, just .00 .00 .00 .42 .00 .28
something I do.
39. So I can get away from what I’m .12 .27 .00 .42 .00 .00
doing.
40. I just like to use it. .13 .11 .27 .29 .14 .19
41. So I can have control over when .20 .17 .00 .00 .00 .43
and if I respond.
87
Table 15 continued
________________________________________________________________________
42. So I can have electronic copies of .00 .15 .00 .00 .14 .33
my correspondence.
45. So I won’t have to feel alone. .00 .15 .00 .17 .00 .18
________________________________________________________________________
Note. N = 441
88
Table 16
________________________________________________________________________
89
Research question 2b asked about college students’ motives for instant messaging
use. An exploratory principal factor analysis with oblique rotation identified six factors,
accounting for 58.4% of the variance. The criteria for factor retention were at least two
loadings of at least .45. The first factor (eigenvalue = 18.06) was made up of six items
(eigenvalue = 3.35) was comprised of three items reflecting Anonymity as a motive for
instant messaging use. The third factor (eigenvalue = 2.02) consisted of three items that
focused on Information Seeking as a motive for using instant messaging. The fourth
factor (eigenvalue = 1.61) consisted of five items, which reflected Interpersonal Utility as
a motive for instant messaging use. The fifth factor (eigenvalue = 1.31) was made up of
five items reflecting Escape as a motive for instant messaging use. The sixth factor
instant messaging use. Table 17 summarizes the results of the factor analysis. To create
measures of instant messaging motivation, responses to items loading on each factor were
averaged. Descriptive statistics for each factor are summarized in Table 18.
90
Table 17
1. Because it makes me feel less .75 .00 .00 .00 .00 .18
lonely.
2. Because I just need to talk about .73 .00 .00 .18 .00 .00
my problems sometimes.
3. Because I need someone to talk to .67 .00 .00 .00 .00 .00
or be with.
4. So I won’t have to feel alone. .60 .31 .00 .00 .00 .00
5. When there is no one else to talk or .54 .00 .15 .00 .00 .00
be with.
6. To feel less inhibited when I .51 .21 .00 .13 .00 .00
communicate.
7. Because I can pretend to be anyone .00 .79 .00 .00 .00 .00
I want to be when interacting with
other people online.
9. So I can have electronic copies of .00 .51 .24 .00 .00 .12
my correspondence.
10. To look for information. .00 .00 .88 .00 .00 .00
11. To get information for free. .00 .00 .85 .00 .00 .00
12. To see what is out there. .00 .14 .79 .00 .00 .00
91
Table 17 continued
________________________________________________________________________
13. To let others know I care about .00 .00 .00 .84 .00 .00
their feelings.
14. Because I am concerned about .00 .00 .00 .78 .11 .00
others.
15. To show others encouragement. .00 .00 .00 .71 .00 .10
18. Because it makes me feel less .00 .14 .00 .00 .76 .10
tense.
19. Because it is a pleasant rest. .00 .00 .00 .00 .69 .00
20. To get away from pressures and .12 .00 .16 .00 .65 .00
responsibilities.
21. To forget about my problems. .18 .12 .15 .11 .52 .00
22. Because it relaxes me. .00 .00 .00 .00 .45 .44
26. To feel involved with what’s .28 .00 -.11 .32 .00 .10
going on with other people.
92
Table 17 continued
________________________________________________________________________
28. Because people don’t have to be .15 .11 .22 .10 -.19 .26
there to receive e-mail.
29. Because it is easier to e-mail than .00 .19 .00 .22 .00 .00
talk to some people.
30. To give me something to occupy .00 .00 .00 .00 .17 .19
my time.
31. Just because it is available. .00 .00 .13 .00 .00 .22
32. When I have nothing better to do. .18 -.13 .00 -.16 .00 .17
33. Because it’s thrilling. .19 .16 .18 .00 .31 .24
34. Because it passes the time away, .16 .00 .00 .00 .16 .16
particularly when I am bored.
35. To feel connected to other people. .27 .00 .00 .40 .17 .19
36. Because it’s a habit, just .00 .00 .00 .00 .00 .29
something I do.
37. Because it is more comfortable .22 .43 .00 .00 .00 .00
than talking to people face to face.
38. Because I can express myself .30 .32 .00 .24 .00 .00
freely.
39. So I can get away from what I’m .18 .00 .00 .10 .30 .00
doing.
40. To tell others what to do. .00 .29 .00 .30 .00 .00
93
Table 17 continued
________________________________________________________________________
41. To get someone to do something .00 .28 .00 .37 .00 .00
for me.
42. To get more points of view. .12 .00 .19 .44 .15 .00
44. I just like to use it. .00 -.10 .00 .00 .15 .26
45. To put something off that I should .00 -.10 .00 .00 .40 .00
be doing.
46. So I can have control over when .19 .17 .28 .17 .00 .00
and if I respond.
________________________________________________________________________
Note. N = 394
94
Table 18
________________________________________________________________________
95
Research question 2c asked about college students’ motives for online chat rooms
use. An exploratory principal factor analysis with oblique rotation identified five factors,
accounting for 65.4% of the variance. Because of the fact that there were only 93
respondents for online chat rooms, the factor analysis included only the items that loaded
on the e-mail motive factor analysis and the instant messaging motive factor analysis.
The first factor (eigenvalue = 16.00) was made up of 10 items marking Chat Room
Benefits as a motive for online chat room use. Factor two (eigenvalue = 2.80) was
comprised of six items reflecting Passing Time as a motive for online chat room use. The
third factor (eigenvalue = 1.97) consisted of four items that focused on Information
Seeking as a motive for using online chat rooms. The fourth factor (eigenvalue = 1.26)
consisted of five items, which reflected Interpersonal Utility as a motive for online chat
room use. The fifth factor (eigenvalue = 1.02) was made up of four items reflecting
Entertainment as a motive for online chat room use. Table 19 summarizes the results of
the factor analysis. To create measures of online chat room motivation, responses to
items loading on each factor were averaged. Descriptive statistics for each factor are
summarized in Table 20. One item that cross-loaded on both the third and fourth factors
was used in creating the mean for Interpersonal Utility from the third factor to the fourth
96
Table 19
1. Because it is more comfortable than talking .85 .11 .00 .00 .00
to people face to face.
4. To feel less inhibited when I communicate. .73 .00 .00 .00 .00
6. Because I can express myself freely. .61 .18 .00 .14 .13
7. Because I need someone to talk to or be with. .56 .23 .00 .16 -.10
8. Because I can pretend to be anyone I want to .53 .00 .00 .00 .17
be when interacting with other people online.
9. Because I just need to talk about my .52 .00 .00 .24 .00
problems sometimes.
10. So I can have control over when and if I .47 .31 -.17 .13 .18
respond.
11. When I have nothing better to do. .11 .76 .10 -.11 .22
12. Because it passes the time away, .00 .75 .00 .10 .00
particularly when I am bored.
13. To give me something to occupy my time. .00 .73 .16 .00 .31
14. To put something off that I should be doing. .00 .57 .00 .00 .00
97
Table 19 continued
________________________________________________________________________
16. When there is no one else to talk or be with. .28 .53 -.13 .30 -.11
19. So I can have electronic copies of my .00 .17 .61 .10 .00
correspondence.
20. To see what is out there. .29 .00 .55 -.15 .11
21. To get information for free. .33 -.11 .53 .00 .28
22. Because I am concerned about others. .00 .00 .00 .85 .13
23. To let others know I care about their .15 .00 .21 .66 .14
feelings.
24. Because it makes me feel less lonely. .22 .11 .00 .56 .00
30. To feel involved with what’s going on with .00 .00 .24 .33 .36
other people.
98
Table 19 continued
________________________________________________________________________
33. Because it makes me feel less tense. .34 .00 .00 .28 .22
34. To get away from pressures and .26 .12 .25 .00 .23
responsibilities.
________________________________________________________________________
Note. N = 92
99
Table 20
________________________________________________________________________
100
The next set of hypotheses focused on the differences in the motives for e-mail,
instant messaging, and online chat rooms. The means of all motives are summarized in
Table 21.
motive for using e-mail more than for using online chat rooms. This hypothesis could not
be tested because no motive of convenience was found for online chat rooms.
relationships as a motive for using e-mail more than for using online chat rooms. This
hypothesis could not be tested because no motive for maintaining relationships was
found.
relationships as a motive for using instant messaging more than for using online chat
rooms. Once again, this hypothesis could not be tested because no motive for
motive for using online chat rooms than for using e-mail. This hypothesis could not be
a motive for e-mail more than for online chat rooms. This hypothesis was supported;
significantly more than for online chat rooms (M = 2.28): t(89) = 5.70, p < .001.
101
Hypothesis 8b predicted that college students would report interpersonal utility as
a motive for e-mail more than for online chat rooms. This hypothesis was supported;
significantly more than for online chat rooms (M = 2.04): t(90) = 7.32, p < .001.
motive for instant messaging more than for e-mail. This hypothesis could not be tested
motive for instant messaging more than for e-mail. This hypothesis could not be tested
motive for instant messaging more than for e-mail. This hypothesis was supported;
significantly more than for e-mail (M = 3.17): t(390) = 10.62, p < .001.
motive for online chat rooms more than for e-mail. This hypothesis was not supported;
college students did not report passing time as a motive for online chat rooms (M = 2.71)
motive for online chat rooms more than for e-mail. This hypothesis was not supported; in
significantly more than for online chat rooms (M = 2.85): t(90) = 3.43, p < .001.
102
Research question 2d asked if there are differences between college students’
motives for using e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms. Beyond the
differences identified in the tests of the hypotheses, there were several other non-
instant messaging (M = 3.83) significantly more than for online chat rooms (M = 2.83):
t(85) = 7.23, p < .001. Second, college students reported information seeking as a motive
for instant messaging (M = 2.88) significantly more than for online chat rooms
(M = 2.29): t(85) = 4.58, p < .001. Third, college students reported information seeking
as a motive for e-mail (M = 2.97) significantly more than for instant messaging
(M = 2.62): t(391) = 5.66, p < .001. Fourth, college students reported interpersonal
utility as a motive for instant messaging (M = 3.22) significantly more than for e-mail
(M = 3.01): t(392) = 5.21, p < .001. Finally, college students reported escape as a motive
for instant messaging (M = 3.10) significantly more than for e-mail (M = 2.28):
t(392) = 18.78, p < .001. Other significant results that can be determined from these
means are what motives college students report the most and the least for each
technology. Entertainment is reported most as a motive for all three forms of CMC, e-
mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms. For e-mail escape is reported least, for
instant messaging, anonymity is reported least, and for online chat rooms, interpersonal
103
Table 21
Convenience 2.86 - -
Companionship - 2.64 -
Anonymity - 1.88 -
________________________________________________________________________
104
Psychological Antecedents
satisfaction affect college students’ uses and motives for using e-mail, instant messaging,
spent in online chat rooms. This hypothesis was not supported; there was no significant
relationship between shyness and time spent in online chat rooms: r = .08, p = .22.
spent instant messaging. This hypothesis was not supported; there was no significant
relationship between shyness and time spent instant messaging: r = -.05, p = .15.
spent e-mailing. This hypothesis was not supported; there was no significant relationship
time spent in online chat rooms. This hypothesis was supported; loneliness was
significantly and positively related to time spent in online chat rooms: r = .27, p < .01.
time spent instant messaging. This hypothesis was not supported; there was no
significant relationship between loneliness and time spent instant messaging: r = -.04,
p = .24.
105
Hypothesis 10c predicted that loneliness will be negatively related to amount of
time spent e-mailing. This hypothesis was not supported; there was no significant
to amount of time spent in online chat rooms. This hypothesis was not supported; there
negatively related to amount of time spent in online chat rooms. This hypothesis was
to amount of time spent instant messaging. This hypothesis was not supported; there was
negatively related to amount of time spent instant messaging. This hypothesis was not
to amount of time spent e-mailing. This hypothesis was not supported; there was no
106
significant relationship between communication avoidance and time spent e-mailing:
r = -.03, p = .29.
negatively related to amount of time spent e-mailing. This hypothesis was not supported;
there was no significant relationship between finding communication rewarding and time
negatively related to amount of time spent in online chat rooms. This hypothesis was
positively related to amount of time spent instant messaging. This hypothesis was not
positively related to amount of time spent e-mailing. This hypothesis was not supported;
related to college students’ uses and motives for e-mail, instant messaging, and online
chat rooms. Several significant relationships between these variables were identified.
First, there was a significant positive relationship between loneliness and using e-mail for
107
social-related communication: r = .15, p < .01. Second, there was a significant positive
relationship between shyness and using e-mail to communicate with people only known
online: r = .24, p < .01. Third, there was a significant negative relationship between
messaging: r = -.25, p < .01. Fourth, there was a significant positive relationship
messaging: r = .13, p < .01. Fifth, there was a significant negative relationship between
with people known offline: r = -.28, p < .01. Finally, there was a significant positive
relationship between loneliness and reporting interpersonal utility as a motive for online
chat rooms: r = .33, p < .01. See Tables 22, 23, and 24 for a summarized list of all
correlations.
108
Table 22
Involve- Offline
IP satis- ment in social
Loneliness Shyness Reward Avoidance faction activities network
________________________________________________________________________
Uses
Motives
________________________________________________________________________
109
Table 23
Involve- Offline
IP satis- ment in social
Loneliness Shyness Reward Avoidance faction activities network
________________________________________________________________________
Uses
Motives
________________________________________________________________________
110
Table 24
Involve- Offline
IP satis- ment in social
Loneliness Shyness Reward Avoidance faction activities network
________________________________________________________________________
Uses
Motives
________________________________________________________________________
111
Social Antecedents
A fourth goal of this study was to understand how social antecedents affect
college students’ uses and motives for using e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat
rooms.
will be negatively related to amount of time spent in online chat rooms. This hypothesis
was not supported; there was no significant relationship between involvement in offline
social relationships/networks and time spent in online chat rooms: r = .04, p = .35.
messaging. This hypothesis was not supported; there was no significant relationship
will be positively related to amount of time spent e-mailing. This hypothesis was not
positively related to amount of time spent instant messaging. This hypothesis was not
supported; people who are involved in long distance relationships (M = 119.77) did not
spend significantly more time instant messaging than those not involved in a long
112
Hypothesis 14b predicted that involvement in long distance relationships will be
positively related to amount of time spent e-mailing. This hypothesis was not supported,
people who are involved in long distance relationships (M = 21.76) did not spend
significantly more time e-mailing than people not involved in a long distance relationship
related to amount of time spent e-mailing. This hypothesis was supported; involvement
in student activities was significantly and positively related to time spent e-mailing:
Demographic Antecedents
A fifth goal of this study was to understand how demographic antecedents affect
college students’ uses and motives for using e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat
rooms.
Hypothesis 16 predicted that female college students will spend more time e-
mailing than male college students. This hypothesis was supported; female college
students (M = 22.81) spent significantly more time e-mailing than male college students
Hypothesis 17 predicted that male college students will report passing time as a
motive for instant messaging use more than female college students. This hypothesis
could not be tested since no motive of passing time was found for instant messaging.
relationships as a motive for instant messaging use more than male college students.
113
Since no motive of maintaining relationships was found, the motive of interpersonal
utility was used instead to test this hypothesis. This hypothesis was supported; female
messaging significantly more than male college students (M = 3.03): t(392) = 2.90,
p < .004.
Hypothesis 19 predicted that college students who are 17-20 years of age will use
instant messaging more than college students 21 years of age and older. This hypothesis
was supported; college students 17-20 years of age (M = 113.45) used instant messaging
more than college students 21 years of age or older (M = 74.17): t(388) = 2.01, p < .05.
Hypothesis 20 predicted that college students 21 years of age or older will report
emotional support as a motive for using online chat rooms more than college students 17-
20 years of age. This hypothesis could not be tested since no motive of emotional
Hypothesis 21a predicted that college students in the hard science majors will use
e-mail more than college students in majors other than hard sciences. This hypothesis
was not supported; college students in the hard science majors (M = 20.00) did not use e-
mail significantly more than college students in majors other than hard sciences
Hypothesis 21b predicted that college students in the hard science majors will use
instant messaging more than college students in majors other than hard sciences. This
hypothesis was not supported; college students in the hard science majors (M = 61.33)
114
did not use instant messaging significantly more than college students in majors other
Hypothesis 21c predicted that college students in the hard science majors will use
online chat rooms more than college students in majors other than hard sciences. This
hypothesis was not supported; college students in the hard science majors (M = 14.00)
did not use online chat rooms significantly more than college students in majors other
Beyond the differences identified in the tests of Hypotheses 16-21, there were
several other significant differences in regards to demographic conditions. The means for
gender, age, and major are summarized in Tables 25, 26, and 27, respectively. First,
college students 21 years of age and older (M = 3.31) used e-mail significantly more than
t(439) = 2.13, p < .03. Second, college students 17 to 20 years of age (M = 3.08) used
instant messaging significantly more than college students 21 years of age and older
(M = 2.85) to communicate with people they know offline: t(392) = 2.89, p < .004.
Third, college students in majors other than hard science (M = 3.01) reported
interpersonal utility as a motive for using e-mail significantly more than college students
in the hard science majors (M = 2.46): t(432) = 2.13, p < .03. Fourth, college students in
majors other than hard science (M = 3.45) used instant messaging significantly more than
college students in the hard science majors (M = 2.83) for communicating long distance:
t(386) = 2.18, p < .03. Fifth, male college students (M = 2.15) reported anonymity as a
motive for using instant messaging significantly more than female college students
115
(M = 1.74): t(244.48) = 3.98, p < .001. Finally, male college students (M = 2.21) used
online chat rooms significantly more than female college students (M = 1.69) for task-
116
Table 25
M male M female t df P
________________________________________________________________________
E-mail Uses
E-mail Motives
117
Table 25 continued
________________________________________________________________________
M male M female t df P
________________________________________________________________________
118
Table 25 continued
________________________________________________________________________
M male M female t df P
________________________________________________________________________
Time spent in online chat rooms 21.68 10.73 1.51 63.13 .14
(minutes)
________________________________________________________________________
119
Table 26
M younger M older
students* students** t df p
________________________________________________________________________
E-mail Uses
E-mail Motives
120
Table 26 continued
________________________________________________________________________
M younger M older
students* students** t df p
________________________________________________________________________
121
Table 26 continued
________________________________________________________________________
M younger M older
students* students** t df p
________________________________________________________________________
Note. *Younger students were 17-20 years old. **Older students were 21 years or older.
122
Table 27
M nonhard M hard
science science t df p
________________________________________________________________________
E-mail Uses
E-mail Motives
123
Table 27 continued
________________________________________________________________________
M nonhard M hard
science science t df p
________________________________________________________________________
124
Table 27 continued
________________________________________________________________________
M nonhard M hard
science science t df p
________________________________________________________________________
125
Chapter 4
DISCUSSION
The purpose of this study was to examine why college students are using
chat rooms, and who, of these college students, is using each of the three forms of CMC.
Grounded in the uses and gratifications perspective, this study examined five main areas
antecedents. This chapter, first, presents the hypothesized and nonhypothesized findings
in each of these areas. It discusses the theoretical implications of the results of this study.
Then it examines future directions of research based on the findings. Finally, this chapter
Summary of Results
nonhypothesized, which suggest directions for future CMC research. Before discussing
the various directions for future research, it is necessary to look at the findings and their
implications. This section discusses the general findings of each of the five areas of this
study. Many of the findings in this study support previous findings by researchers and
also expand the research in the areas of CMC and uses and gratifications.
126
Uses
found five general uses for e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms: task-related
online, communicating with people known offline, and long distance communication.
The uses that were found in this study for e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat
rooms coincided with many of those found in previous research (Dainton & Aylor, 2002;
Hard af Segerstad & Ljungstrand, 2002; Jones, 2002; McKenna et al., 2002; Lenhart, et
While earlier research had explored the uses of CMC, few studies had considered
several different forms of CMC. Many of the hypothesized findings in this study
expanded on previous research in the area of CMC by comparing the uses of the different
types of mediated communication. For example, when comparing e-mail and instant
messaging, this study found that college students use instant messaging more than e-mail
for communicating long distance and for social-related communication, but they use e-
mail more than instant messaging for communicating with people they know offline.
This finding is an indication that in future research, types of CMC should be studied
separately, as this study attempted to do. This study found that even though e-mail,
instant messaging, and online chat rooms share the same general uses, college students do
127
For task-related communication, such as asking a professor about material
covered in class, e-mail is used by college students significantly more than instant
messaging and online chat rooms. This finding might be explained by the more formal
and also asynchronous nature of e-mail compared to instant messaging and online chat
rooms, which are more informal and allow for synchronous communication to occur. E-
mail is similar to sending letters or memos, so it conceptually makes sense that when
using a form of CMC for task-related communication, students would choose e-mail. E-
mail allows people the chance to think about what they want to say, so, college students
might feel more comfortable using e-mail to contact professors. Westmyer, DiCioccio,
and Rubin (1998) also concluded that task-related needs might be more effectively
direction for future research would be to look at how relationship types, such as
messaging is used by college students significantly more than e-mail and online chat
rooms. This finding may suggest that there are aspects of instant messaging that make it
more likely that college students will use it to communicate socially with others. It may
e-mail and it may be the ability to have multiple, spontaneous one-on-one conversations
For communicating with people known offline, e-mail is used by college students
significantly more than instant messaging and online chat rooms. Given that college
128
students are using e-mail for task-related communication, such as communicating with
professors, classmates, coworkers, and business contacts, it might make sense that they
are using it more than the other two forms of CMC to communicate with people known
offline. As Dimmick, Kline, and Stafford (2000) concluded, the asynchronous nature of
e-mail allows people the opportunity to keep in touch with and communicate with others
significantly more than e-mail and online chat rooms. One reason for this finding may be
simply that it allows for synchronous communication to occur, but at a much cheaper
price than a long distance telephone call. Future research might want to look at whether
Motives
that prompts use of a particular communication channel to fulfill a need. Much of the
research regarding motives for using the Internet and for using CMC have been based
upon traditional uses and gratifications research on television viewing (e.g. Conway &
Rubin, 1991; Palmgreen, Wenner, & Rayburn, 1981; Rubin, 1984; Rubin & Perse, 1987)
however, as Rubin (2003) points out, there might be other motives for the Internet that
have not been considered yet. This study found some motives that the three forms of
CMC had in common and some motives that were unique to either one or two of the
forms of CMC.
129
Similar to prior research on computer-mediated communication (Papacharissi &
Rubin, 2000; Stafford et al., 1999), college students reported such motives as
entertainment, information seeking, and interpersonal utility for using e-mail, instant
messaging, and online chat rooms. This study’s attempt to identify motives specific for
each of the three forms of CMC examined was meant to build on past research, which
The results of this study identified motives unique to each form of CMC. Four
motives reported in this study were unique to only one form of CMC: convenience for e-
mail, companionship and anonymity for instant messaging, and chat rooms benefits for
online chat rooms. Two motives were reported for only two of the three forms of CMC
studied: passing time for e-mail and online chat rooms and escape for e-mail and instant
messaging. The unique motives found for each channel of CMC supports the uses and
gratifications approach that says that people use different channels because they are
motivated by different reasons (Katz et al., 1974; Rosengren, 1974). The Internet is no
longer a single channel of communication; it now has many different channels involved
in it. The unique attributes of these channels give rise to different motives and uses
indicating that each form of CMC should be studied individually in future research.
However, given that e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms are all
found on the Internet, it makes sense that they would share some of the same motives.
Three motives were common to all three forms of CMC studied: information seeking,
interpersonal utility, and entertainment. Although all three forms of CMC studied
reported these three motives, the amount by which college students were motivated by
130
them differed depending upon the technology. College students were motivated to use e-
mail more than instant messaging and online chat rooms to seek out information. Also,
college students were motivated to use instant messaging more than e-mail and online
chat rooms for entertainment and for communicating interpersonally. Although users of
all three forms of CMC reported both instrumental and ritualized motives, given that
information seeking was reported as a motive for e-mail more often than for instant
messaging and online chat rooms there is some support for the idea that e-mail might be
more instrumental in use than instant messaging and online chat rooms, which both might
be more ritualized forms of CMC. This study has found that the orientations towards
instrumental and ritualized use exist within CMC; however, it needs to be explored
expand these findings (Katz et. al., 1974; Perse, 1990; Rubin & Perse, 1987). One such
audience activity and CMC use, similar to the way Rubin and Perse (1987) used them to
Psychological Antecedents
This study found that certain psychological antecedents are related to computer-
mediated communication use, which supports prior research in the area (Caplan, 2002;
Markey & Wells, 2002; McKenna et al., 2002; Papacharissi & Rubin, 2000). This
finding also lends support to uses and gratifications, which claims that psychological
antecedents have an effect on people’s use of communication channels (Katz et al., 1974;
Rosengren, 1974).
131
The findings in this study support prior research that indicates that those people
who are lonely tend to be more likely to use CMC (Caplan, 2002; McKenna et al., 2002).
Specifically, this study found that there is a positive relationship between loneliness and
amount of time college students spend using online chat rooms. This may be because
people who are lonely can better express themselves on the Internet rather than in face-to-
face interactions (McKenna et al., 2002). College students who are lonely may find
comfort in forming relationships with people they meet in chat rooms. Other research
that may point to a different explanation for the relationship between loneliness and time
spent using online chat rooms is that of Caplan (2002) who found that people with a
higher preference for online communication also had higher levels of loneliness, shyness,
and depression. These findings indicate that further research needs to explore the
attributes of online chat rooms that attract people who experience feelings such as
loneliness to them.
Other findings relating the effect of psychological antecedents on chat room use
lend some support to Papacharissi and Rubin’s (2000) finding that CMC is a functional
rewarding and who are less satisfied with interpersonal communication spend more time
in online chat rooms. So, for these people online chat rooms might be a functional
(1998) contradictory finding that indeed the Internet and face-to-face communication are
not functional alternatives more research needs to be done. As Flaherty, Pearce, and
Rubin’s (1998) data was collected in the Spring of 1996, it would be safe to assume that
132
more people are using CMC and that it is more readily available to a larger group of
people, so their findings may be outdated. Future research might want to concentrate on
This study did not find that psychological antecedents were related to amount of
time spent e-mailing or instant messaging. This might indicate that previous
loneliness, and emotional instability were characteristics of Internet chat users (Markey &
Wells, 2002; Peris et al., 2002) may be because the technologies were not studied
separately. These antecedents might only be related to online chat room use as was found
in this study. Certain attributes of chat rooms such as the lack of nonverbal cues and its
anonymous nature, which allow meeting new people to be easier, might be attractive to
people who are lonely or shy, but might not necessarily attract an e-mail or instant
messaging user who communicates via CMC with people that they already know offline.
This study hypothesized only about psychological antecedents and the amount of
time college students spend using the various forms of CMC. However, this study also
antecedents and the specific uses and motives found for e-mail and instant messaging.
The uses and gratifications approach says that psychological antecedents can affect the
needs that produce motives and this study found this to be true.
When looking at e-mail uses and motives, loneliness and shyness both related
133
online, and with the motive of escape. In contrast, the psychological antecedent of
finding communicating rewarding related negatively to these uses and motives for e-mail.
These findings indicate that people who are shy and lonely are more likely to have
developed relationships online and thus use e-mail to communicate socially with these
people. They also might use e-mail as a way to escape their face-to-face communication.
The relationships between e-mail uses and motives and the psychological antecedents
mentioned indicate that while psychological antecedents might have little effect on total
amount of time spent e-mailing, they could have some effect in terms of what college
students use e-mail for and what types of motives drive them to use e-mail. Future
research needs to look not only look at the relationship that exists between psychological
antecedents and the amount of time people spend using a particular form of CMC, but
also look at how psychological antecedents relate to motives and uses as past uses and
gratifications research asserts (Conway & Rubin, 1991; Perse & Rubin, 1990). Certain
psychological antecedents might produce different needs and motives for different
people. Examining the motives reported by people who are lonely or shy might produce
results that enable researchers to more fully understand the positive and negative effects
to all of the psychological antecedents looked at in this study. Both anonymity and
shyness, loneliness, and who avoid communication use instant messaging to feel
134
companionship with others, while at the same time enjoying the anonymity of the
Internet. So, those people who are not as comfortable in face-to-face interactions find
comfort in the anonymous companionship they receive through CMC. The motives of
people who enjoy face-to-face communication do not necessarily use instant messaging
because of its anonymity nor because of the companionship they experience. They might
Again these relationships indicate that while psychological antecedents may have
little effect on total amount of time instant messaging, they might have an effect on what
motivates people to use instant messaging. These relationships also indicate that there
are aspects of instant messaging that make it unique from other forms of CMC given that
these two motives that are exclusive to instant messaging have such significant
relationships with all of the psychological antecedents. Once again, the results of this
study show that future research needs to separate the various forms of CMC to identify
Three of the psychological antecedents studied related to all of the uses identified
in this study for online chat rooms. Loneliness related positively with all five uses. So,
people who are lonely use online chat rooms for a number of different reasons: to
communicate with people known online and offline. On the other hand the psychological
135
antecedents, finding communication rewarding and interpersonal communication
satisfaction related negatively with all five uses. People who are comfortable in face-to-
face interactions tend not to use online chat rooms for much of their communication
behavior. This may indicate that there is something about online chat rooms that draws
in people who are lonely, but not people who find communication rewarding and
the forms of CMC separately. Online chat rooms seem to be used by different types of
people. Understanding the motives and psychological antecedents of the people who do
use online chat rooms will provide researchers with knowledge and insight into how this
Social Antecedents
While no support was found for involvement in offline social networks and
relationships being related to amount of time spent using the three forms of CMC as
originally hypothesized, there were some other nonhypothesized findings relating this
social antecedent to specific uses of CMC. People involved in offline social networks
use e-mail for task-related communication and for communicating with people they know
offline. Also, they use instant messaging for social-related communication and
communicating with people they know offline. So, while this study did not confirm past
research that found that people involved in offline social networks also tend to form
relationships online (Matei & Ball-Rokeach, 2001; McKenna et al., 2002), it does
indicate that social networks are an antecedent for CMC use and deserve to be explored
136
This study also found that college students’ involvement in student activities is an
antecedent for CMC use. Prior research in CMC technologies did not look at how
involvement in student activities would affect CMC use. Given that this study focused
on college students’ uses and motives for CMC, it seems natural to examine how this
social antecedent might affect their CMC use. This study found that the more students
are involved in student activities the more time they spend e-mailing. Many student
organizations, just like many course professors, choose to communicate with members
through electronic means such as e-mail. Therefore, the more activities a student is
involved in, the more likely they will use e-mail to communicate with other members of
their groups. This supports a key aspect of uses and gratifications, which states that
social antecedents affect people’s choice of communication channel (Katz et al., 1974;
Rosengren, 1974).
This study also allows for conclusions to be made regarding relationships between
involvement in student activities and the uses and motives found for each form of CMC.
In particular the more students are involved in student activities the more they use e-mail
people known offline. This conceptually makes sense because if students are involved in
activities, e-mail may be one mode of communication that groups use to keep members
aware of information. Also, if students are involved in student activities, they might have
a large number of people to communicate with and e-mail may provide one mode of
communication for doing this. Another finding in this study is that the more students are
involved in student activities the more they use instant messaging for social-related
137
communication and communicating with people known offline. However, unlike with e-
mail, involvement in student activities was not related to using instant messaging for
with people that they know in the groups they are a part of, they may not use it to conduct
any group business. Since involvement in student activities is not an antecedent that has
been explored previously, future research should look to expand upon the findings in this
study. Also, future research should examine whether other social conditions of college
Demographic Antecedents
concludes that indeed this is true for CMC usage. Gender and age appear to have an
Similar to past research (Weiser, 2000) this study found that female college
students spend more time using e-mail than male college students. Another finding that
is specific to instant messaging but that supports general CMC findings (Weiser, 2000) is
that female college students report interpersonal utility as a motive for instant messaging
more than male college students. This finding also supports Rubin, Perse, and Barbato’s
(1988) finding that women use communication more often to express affection and to
seek inclusion. Males used online chat rooms for all five uses found in this study
significantly more than females. Males also reported anonymity as a motive for using
instant messaging significantly more than females. Coincidentally, they also used instant
138
messaging significantly more than females to communicate with people known online.
This might indicate that males feel comfortable using instant messaging because of its
anonymity as a way to meet people online and establish relationships online. This relates
that male friendships exhibit much more protectiveness than female friendships, which
exhibit more expressiveness. Through the anonymity of online communication men are
able to be both expressive and protective in their online relationships, thus relieving the
tension of this dialectic. Future research should employ interpersonal theories to explore
A finding of this study that expands previous research by Weiser (2000) is the
finding that younger college students, those 17-20 years of age use instant messaging
more than older college students, those over 21 years of age. Weiser (2000) found that e-
mail tends to be a technology used more often by a younger population. This study also
finds that instant messaging is a technology used by a younger population and in general
supports the idea that the Internet as a whole is used more often by younger people. One
important and interesting area of future research is to look at whether instant messaging is
displacing other forms of interpersonal communication, such as the telephone and face-
to-face, for its younger users. Are teenagers and college students who use instant
findings in this area include that students 21 years of age and older used e-mail for task-
related communication more than those 17-20 years of age. One reason for this finding is
that students 21 years of age and older might just have more task-related communication
139
to do. By that age, most students are nearing the end of their college years and are
therefore deeply involved in their major courses as well as possibly looking for jobs and
internships. Another finding related to instant messaging and age differences is that
students 17-20 years of age use instant messaging for communicating with people known
offline and report interpersonal utility as a motive for instant messaging more than
students 21 years of age and older. This finding makes sense given that instant
messaging tends to be a young person’s activity (Rainie, 2000). So, while the younger
users may not have a need to use instant messaging for task-related communication as
much as older users, they do still enjoy using it for social reasons. Future research should
explore the findings related to instant messaging use and age further by asking the
Not many significant differences were found between those students in the hard
science majors and those in nonhard science majors in regards to their CMC usage as
originally thought. However, this study did find that students in nonhard science majors
used e-mail significantly more than those in hard science majors for social-related
communication and for communicating with people known offline. This slightly
contradicts Anderson’s (2001) finding that students in hard science majors are more
likely to become dependent on the Internet. Given that these students are not reporting
using e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms for social communication more
than students in majors other than the hard sciences and are in fact reporting it less, there
does not seem to be any support from this study for Anderson’s (2001) finding.
However, future research should explore the relationship between academic major and
140
CMC usage more in depth. Researchers should differentiate between more than just hard
science and nonhard science majors. At the time of data collection, the University of
Delaware is the second most wired university in the United States, which indicates that a
student certainly does not need to be a hard science major to have access to the Internet
this study was exploratory in nature. Its purpose was to examine the way in which
college students use CMC technology and to find aspects of their lives that affect their
use of such communication tools. The findings of this study are rich and plentiful and
should aid researchers in future study of the Internet. While many directions for future
research were mentioned previously, this next section focuses on general areas for
researchers to focus.
One area for future research would be to expand on the findings regarding uses of
e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms. While it was found from this study that
college students are using certain forms of CMC more than others for certain uses, one
question that researchers might ask is why. What is it about these technologies that
prompt this type of use? For example, what is it about e-mail that causes students to use
it more often than instant messaging and online chat rooms to communicate with people
known offline?
Another area for future research to look at is the area of motives related to
computer-mediated communication. This study found that there are some motives that
141
e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms have in common, but there are other
motives that are unique to each of these three forms of CMC. Similar to the questions
posed in the previous paragraph about uses, researchers need to ask why in regards to
motives. Why are college students reporting convenience as a motive for e-mail but not
for instant messaging and online chat rooms? One specific motive that requires more
examination is the motive of chat room benefits. This motive consisted of many of the
benefits that have been reported about online chat rooms in past research (Markey &
Wells, 2002). When factored, these benefits did not disperse out into many different
motives, but rather correlated together as one defining motive. This finding indicates that
online chat rooms may be more different from other forms of CMC than researchers have
antecedents, future research into the relationship between psychological antecedents and
CMC technology use is needed. Many nonhypothesized findings in this study deserve
further examination. While this study did not find that overall use of e-mail and instant
messaging are related to the psychological antecedents, it did find that certain uses and
motives were related. Further exploration into these relationships between uses, motives,
and psychological antecedents is necessary. Also, further study into the use of online
chat rooms and psychological antecedents is needed since it was found that they are
related.
It is clear from this study that social antecedents have some effect on the CMC
usage of college students. Since involvement in student activities is one social antecedent
142
that has not been studied before, it is necessary for future research to be done to confirm
the findings of this study. But also, research should look at the social networks that
students are a part of including the activities they are involved in and examine how these
networks and relationships affect their use of CMC technology. Research needs to look
more specifically at the types of networks and relationships that students have in their
lives. It may be useful for future research to look at how students use CMC to
communicate with professors and business contacts and how that use differs from their
This study has shown that certain demographic antecedents affect college
students’ use of CMC technology. However, much more research in this area is needed.
More specific research needs to look at gender and age and the specific uses and motives
for CMC. Although little was found in regard to academic major, there were some slight
differences between the two groups. More differentiation between majors may provide
further insight into how this antecedent affects students’ CMC use.
One type of CMC that was looked at in this study, but that deserves further
mention in terms of future research is online chat rooms. This form of CMC was not able
to be tested fully in this study given the small amount of respondents who indicated using
online chat rooms. However, some interesting findings emerged and deserve further
exploration. For example, the motive of chat room benefits was quite unique to online
chat rooms and requires further examination. Also, given the still contradictory findings
in past research regarding psychological antecedents and their effect on chat room use,
more research needs to look at who is using online chat rooms and what uses do they
143
have for them. One other interesting area of research in regards to online chat rooms is to
look at maybe why college students are not using this form of CMC as much as other
forms. Do online chat rooms have a negative connotation attached to them? Are they
seen as a dysfunctional way to use the Internet? These are questions that should be
Limitations
Although this study has contributed greatly to the literature on the Internet and
Like other uses and gratifications research, this study used a self-report method,
which in itself has some drawbacks. Self-report methods such as the survey used in this
study rely on respondents to answer honestly. The chance researchers take with this type
of method is that respondents might not always answer questions truthfully or be able to
accurately estimate measures. The time measures used in this study asked respondents to
estimate how many minutes per week they spend using a particular form of CMC and
how many years they had been using that form of CMC. These measures provided
numbers that indicate that the respondents were not able to accurately estimate time or
years and thus are a limitation to this study. One reason that the respondents were not
able to accurately measure their time using CMC might be because it is a mundane
behavior, similar to Ferguson’s (1994) finding that respondents were unable to accurately
report how often they changed the channel while watching television, also a mundane
behavior.
144
One major limitation of this study is that there were only 93 respondents who
reported using online chat rooms. While this finding is somewhat informative in the fact
that it signals that college students are not large users of chat rooms, it also did not allow
for rich testing to be done. In future studies, it might be helpful to find a large number of
online chat room users in order to adequately compare them to users of e-mail and instant
messaging. However, it was unforeseen that so few respondents in this study would be
Another limitation of this study is the manner in which some measures were
determined. The measures for long distance and social networks were not adequate for
assessing what they were intended to evaluate. The long distance measure was computed
from only one item on the uses list. A more comprehensive measure would most likely
have helped to assess involvement in a long distance relationship more easily. The social
networks measure was not filled out correctly by many of the respondents. This may
have been because the wording was confusing or that it was at the end of the survey. The
incompleteness of many of the surveys did not allow for a proper measuring of social
networks. A better measure for this factor would be helpful in future research to
A fourth limitation of this study is the lack of random sampling. The sampling
used in this study was convenience sampling in order to obtain a large number of
respondents given the exploratory nature of the sample. In future research a random
sample may help to find even more information about college students’ use of CMC and
145
Finally, this study was intended to examine college students’ use of computer-
mediated communication. This is an important population to study because they are such
wide users of the technology and they are the future users of CMC. So, studying how
college students use the technologies now will help predict how they will use it later in
their lives. However, having such a narrow focus does not allow the results of this study
to be generalized past college students. Further exploration into how the general
population uses CMC would provide even greater depth into this area of research.
Conclusion
about college students’ uses and motives for CMC. The exploratory nature of the study
demographic antecedents. More research still needs to be done in the area of the Internet
a great starting point for researchers. There are numerous questions and topics that could
be and should be explored to further understand how college students and the general
population use CMC technologies in their lives. The Internet is fast becoming a major
part of most people’s daily lives. Therefore, it is important for researchers and scholars
146
Appendix
SURVEY INSTRUMENT
147
University of Delaware
Department of Communication
Internet Survey
If you responded “Yes” please continue on to the next question, if you responded “No” please skip to the next section
(page 3).
About how many minutes do you spend e-mailing on a typical day? ______ minutes
About how long have you been using email? ______ years
DIRECTIONS: For each statement below, please CIRCLE the number that represents your response.
Very
I use e-mail to: Frequently Often Sometimes Rarely Never
148
Very
I use e-mail to: Frequently Often Sometimes Rarely Never
DIRECTIONS: For each statement below, please CIRCLE the number that best expresses your own reasons
for using e-mail.
Agree
Some and
Strongly Agree Disagree Disagree Strongly
I use e-mail: Agree Some Some Some Disagree
1. Because it is entertaining 5 4 3 2 1
2. Because it is enjoyable 5 4 3 2 1
3. Because it is fun 5 4 3 2 1
4. Because it relaxes me 5 4 3 2 1
6. To look stylish 5 4 3 2 1
7. To look fashionable 5 4 3 2 1
149
Agree
Some and
Strongly Agree Disagree Disagree Strongly
I use e-mail: Agree Some Some Some Disagree
29. To be anonymous 5 4 3 2 1
150
Agree
Some and
Strongly Agree Disagree Disagree Strongly
I use e-mail: Agree Some Some Some Disagree
If you responded “Yes” please continue on to the next question, if you responded “No” please skip to the next section
(page 6).
About how many minutes do you spend instant messaging on a typical day? ______ minutes
About how long have you been using instant messaging? ______ years
DIRECTIONS: For each statement below, please CIRCLE the number that represents your response.
Very
I use instant messaging to: Frequently Often Sometimes Rarely Never
151
Very
I use instant messaging to: Frequently Often Sometimes Rarely Never
15. Keep in touch with friends or relatives who live far away 5 4 3 2 1
DIRECTIONS: For each statement below, please CIRCLE the number that best expresses your own reasons
for using instant messaging.
Agree
Some and
Strongly Agree Disagree Disagree Strongly
I use instant messaging: Agree Some Some Some Disagree
8. Because it is easy 5 4 3 2 1
152
Agree
Some and
Strongly Agree Disagree Disagree Strongly
I use instant messaging: Agree Some Some Some Disagree
31. To be anonymous 5 4 3 2 1
153
Agree
Some and
Strongly Agree Disagree Disagree Strongly
I use instant messaging: Agree Some Some Some Disagree
If you responded “Yes” please continue on to the next question, if you responded “No” please skip to the next section
(page 9).
About how many minutes do you spend in online chat rooms on a typical day? ______ minutes
About how long have you been using online chat rooms? ______ years
154
DIRECTIONS: For each statement below, please CIRCLE the number that represents your response.
Very
I use online chat rooms to: Frequently Often Sometimes Rarely Never
10. Keep in touch with friends or relatives who live far away 5 4 3 2 1
DIRECTIONS: For each statement below, please CIRCLE the number that best expresses your own reasons
for using online chat rooms.
Agree
Some and
Strongly Agree Disagree Disagree Strongly
I use online chat rooms: Agree Some Some Some Disagree
155
Agree
Some and
Strongly Agree Disagree Disagree Strongly
I use online chat rooms: Agree Some Some Some Disagree
18. To be anonymous 5 4 3 2 1
156
Agree
Some and
Strongly Agree Disagree Disagree Strongly
I use online chat rooms: Agree Some Some Some Disagree
157
DIRECTIONS: The next section of this questionnaire asks you questions about yourself and about your beliefs. For
each item below, please indicate the extent to which you agree or disagree with each statement.
Agree
Strongly Agree Some and Disagree Strongly
Agree Some Disagree Some Disagree
Some
158
Agree
Strongly Agree Some and Disagree Strongly
Agree Some Disagree Some Disagree
Some
43. I feel tense when I’m with people I don’t know well. 5 4 3 2 1
159
Agree
Strongly Agree Some and Disagree Strongly
Agree Some Disagree Some Disagree
Some
DIRECTIONS: The next section of this questionnaire asks you questions about your involvement in student activities.
For each item below, please indicate whether you are involved in the listed activity by placing an “X” next to the activity.
Please check all that apply.
160
DIRECTIONS: The next section of this questionnaire asks you questions about the people you communicate with
face-to-face and also online. For each category below, please indicate about how many people in each category you
communicate with on a typical day by writing in the number next to the category.
DIRECTIONS: This final section asks you to provide some basic information about yourself.
1. How old are you? (As of your last birthday) ______ years
4. Do you have a computer where you live right now? (CIRCLE your response) Yes or No
5. Do you have Internet access where you live right now? (CIRCLE your response) Yes or No
6. If you have Internet access where you live right now, how do you connect to the Internet? (CIRCLE your response
below)
Dial-up Modem Ethernet High Speed DSL Cable High Speed Other
Access
1 2 3 4 5
7. How fast would you estimate the speed of your connection to the Internet where you live right
161
8. About how often do you use the Internet for activities, other than e-mail, instant messaging, and online chat rooms?
1. Web surfing 5 4 3 2 1 0
2. Downloading music 5 4 3 2 1 0
3. Downloading video 5 4 3 2 1 0
4. Downloading pictures 5 4 3 2 1 0
5. Research 5 4 3 2 1 0
10. What year in school are you? (CIRCLE your response below)
Novice Expert
11. How would you rate your own expertise with computers in general? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
END OF SURVEY
THANK YOU VERY MUCH FOR YOUR HELP !! YOUR ANSWERS ARE ANONYMOUS &
CONFIDENTIAL
162
REFERENCES
Baltes, B. B., Dickson, M. W., Sherman, M. P., Bauer, C. C., & LaGanke, J. S. (2002).
(Eds.), Handbook of new media: Social shaping and consequences of ICTs (pp.
Bresnahan, M. J., & Murray-Johnson, L. (2002). The healing web. Health Care for
Brown, E. (2002, June 24). 33 days 8 campuses 127 kids and infinity of gizmos:
163
Caplan, S. E. (2001). Challenging the mass-interpersonal communication dichotomy:
[http://www.cios.org/www/ejc/v11n101.htm.]
Caplan, S. E. (2002). Preference for online conversation, problematic Internet use, and
Cheek, J. M., & Buss, A. H. (1981). Shyness and sociability. Journal of Personality
Cole, J. I. (2001). The UCLA Internet Report: Surveying the digital future. [Online].
http://ccp.ucla.edu/pages/internet-report.asp.
Behavior, 6, 191-233.
Dainton, M., & Aylor, B. (2002). Patterns of communication channel use in the
19, 118-129.
Dimmick, J., Kline, S. L., & Stafford, L. (2000). The gratification niches of personal e-
164
Ferguson, D. A. (1994). Measurement of mundane TV behaviors: Remote control
device flipping frequency. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 38, 35-
47.
Flaherty, L. M., Pearce, K. J., & Rubin, R. B. (1998). Internet and face-to-face
268.
Flanagin, A. J., & Metzger, M. J. (2001). Internet use in the contemporary media
Golden, P. A., Beauclair, R., & Sussman, L. (1992). Factors affecting electronic mail
Hard af Segerstad, Y., & Ljungstrand, P. (2002). Instant messaging with WebWho.
Hill, K., & Monk, A. F. (2000). Electronic mail versus printed text: The effects on
Jones, S. (2002, September 15). The Internet goes to college: How students are living in
the future with today’s technology. [Online]. Washington, D. C.: Pew Internet
http://www.pewinternet.org/reports/index.asp.
165
Katz, E., Blumler, J. G., & Gurevitch, M. (1974). Utilization of mass communication by
Kingsley, P., & Anderson, T. (1998). Facing life without the Internet. Internet
Lenhart, A., Rainie, L., & Lewis, O. (2001). Teenage life online: The rise of the instant-
http://www.pewinternet.org/reports/index.asp.
Leung, L. (2001). College student motives for chatting on ICQ. New Media & Society,
3, 483-500.
Markey, P. M., & Wells, S. M. (2002). Interpersonal perception in Internet chat rooms.
Marold, K. A., & Larsen, G. (1999). Is the range war over? An investigation into
preferences for e-mail and v-mail. Social Science Computer Review, 17, 466-471.
Matei, S., & Ball-Rokeach, S. J. (2001). Real and virtual social ties: Connections in the
45, 55-564.
166
McKenna, K. Y. A., Green, A. S., & Gleason, M. E. J. (2002). Relationship formation
on the Internet: What’s the big attraction? Journal of Social Issues, 58, 9-31.
Mesch, G. S. (2001). Social relationships and Internet use among adolescents in Israel.
Palmgreen, P., Wenner, L. A., & Rayburn, J. D. (1981). Gratification discrepancies and
Peris, R., Gimeno, M. A., Pinazo, D., Ortet, G., Carrero, V., Sanchiz, M., & Ibanez, I.
(2002). Online chat rooms: Virtual spaces of interaction for socially oriented
Perse, E. M., & Rubin, A. M. (1990). Chronic loneliness and television use. Journal of
Rainie, L. (2000, May 10). Tracking life online: How women use the Internet to
http://www.pewinternet.org/reports/index.asp.
167
Rawlins, W. K. (1991). Friendship matters: Communication, dialectics, and the life
Rollman, J. B., Krug, K., & Parente, F. (2000). The chat room phenomenon: Reciprocal
Rollman, J. B., Parente, F. (2001). Relation of statement length and type and type of
Behavior, 4, 617-622.
Romm, C., & Pliskin, N. (1999). The role of charismatic leadership in diffusion and
290.
36-53.
168
Rubin, R. B. (1994). Unwillingness-to-communicate scale. In R. B. Rubin, P.
Rubin, R. B. (2003, April). Interpersonal uses of the Internet: Past research and future
18, 223-229.
Russell, D., Peplau, L. A., & Cutrona, C. E. (1980). The revised UCLA loneliness scale:
169
Shaver, P. R., & Brennan, K. A. (1991). Measures of depression and loneliness. In J. P.
social psychological attitudes (pp. 195-290). San Diego: Academic Press, Inc.
Short, J., Williams, E., & Christie, B. (1976). The social psychology of
Stafford, L., Kline, S. L., & Dimmick, J. (1999). Home e-mail: Relational maintenance
659-669.
Stone, L. D., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2002). Trauma in real time: Talking and avoiding
online conversations about the death of Princess Diana. Basic and Applied Social
Swanson, D. L. (1979). Political communication research and the uses and gratifications
Behavior, 3, 167-178.
170
Wellman, B., Salaff, J., Dimitrova, D., Garton, J., Gulia, M., & Haythornthwaite, C.
Whitty, M. T. (2002). Liar, liar! An examination of how open, supportive and honest
171