s11218 020 09551 5
s11218 020 09551 5
s11218 020 09551 5
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-020-09551-5
Sławomir Trusz1
Received: 4 October 2018 / Accepted: 12 March 2020 / Published online: 18 April 2020
© The Author(s) 2020
Abstract
The article attempts to answer the question why females prefer humanities/social
studies, whereas males opt for technology/science. For this purpose, the study
majors selected by 445 females and 431 males were linked by logit functions with:
(1) parents’ and (2) teachers’ expectancies, (3) students’ self-expectancies, (4) their
self-concepts of abilities and (5) time spent on learning mathematics and (6) lit-
eracy, (7) test results in mathematics and (8) literacy, (9) gender of mathematics,
and (10) literacy teacher in the 12th grade. Interaction effects of the mathematics
and literacy teacher’s gender with the abovementioned predictors were also quanti-
fied. Females’ selections were mostly influenced by teachers’ expectancies, while for
males, by their self-concepts. The teacher’s gender modified tested relations in five
(females) and nine (males) cases. The results were discussed on the grounds of the
theory of intra- and interpersonal expectancies as learning regulators.
1 Introduction
Are girls actually more polite and hard-working than boys, and boys more aggres-
sive and pugnacious than girls? Feminists say not, arguing that gender stereotypes
are sources of significant differences in behavior, and later, in educational attain-
ment of students. Gender stereotypes, i.e., unjustified representations of various
groups of people, by definition omitting any differences between them (Kite and
Whitley 2016; Stangor 2009), may incline parents and teachers to treat their daugh-
ters (female students) and sons (male students) differently.
* Sławomir Trusz
trusz@up.krakow.pl
1
Institute of Educational Sciences, Pedagogical University of Kraków, 4 Ingardena St.,
30‑060 Kraków, Poland
13
Vol.:(0123456789)
616 S. Trusz
Teachers at school may encourage male students to learn science more, e.g., by
praising them for their correctly conducted physical experiments, whereas female stu-
dents might be motivated to analyze the beauty of Gabriele Mistral’s poetry, according
to socially shared schema of technically talented boys and humanistically predisposed
girls (Eccles et al. 1990; Jussim et al. 1996; Watson et al. 2017). Similarly, at home,
parents who keep their daughters away from screwdrivers or their sons away from dolls
may show them how to bake cakes and replace a bicycle hub, respectively (Jacobs et al.
2004; Simpkins et al. 2018).
The noticeable gap in educational experience and performance of schematically
male versus female subjects among students cannot be explained solely by their abili-
ties (individual differences), but to some extent also by the cultural transmission of gen-
der stereotypes (e.g., Li 1999; Nguyen and Ryan 2008; Robnett 2016; Spencer et al.
1999; Tiedemann 2000; Watson et al. 2017).
In terms of this gap, the importance of expectations of significant others on the typi-
cal behavior of girls and boys, and correlated children’s self-expectations (e.g., Pajares
and Miller 1994; Rudman and Phelan 2010; Trusz 2018), their self-concept of ability in
humanities/social studies (H/SS) versus technology/science (T/S) (e.g., Sullivan 2009;
Szumski and Karwowski 2019; Trusz 2018), and time spent on gender-stereotyped and
gender-neutral activities must be emphasized (e.g., Debacker and Nelson 2000; Hyde
2014; Plante et al. 2019).
As a result, initially inaccurate gender stereotypes may be the source of real differ-
ences in the functioning of females and males, sized according to a specific domain of
human activity. The reason for this is provided by the results of meta-analyses in which
the average sizes of the gender effect for mathematical performance were Cohen’s
d = − .11 (Else-Quest et al. 2010), .16 (Lindberg et al. 2010), .22 (Reilly 2012), .29 and
.32 (Hyde et al. 1990), .43 (Hyde 1990); for cognitive abilities, e.g., mental rotation,
were .51 (Voyer 2011), .56 (Voyer et al. 1995), 1.03 (Voyer 2011); for literacy perfor-
mance were − .11 (Hyde and Linn 1988), − .37 (Voyer and Voyer 2014), − .44 (Reilly
2012); for academic (Huang 2013) and global self-efficacy (Kling et al. 1999), were .08
and .21, respectively; for leadership were − .13, .16, and .27 (Eagly et al. 2003); and for
prosocial behavior was .37 (Eagly 2009). Negative values of Cohen’s d represent higher
female results than male ones in the tested domain.
An important mechanism of this transformation seems to be the stereotype threat
(e.g., Nguyen and Ryan 2008; Schmader 2002; Shapiro and Williams 2012) and self-
fulfilling prophecy (e.g., Fiedler et al. 2002; Gentrup and Rjosk 2018; Watson et al.
2017). The indicated phenomena, together with the regulatory function of students’
self-expectations and their self-concept of ability in T/S and H/SS (e.g., Fan 2011;
Rudman and Phelan 2010; Szumski and Karwowski 2019) are discussed in more detail
in the following sections of this paper.
13
Why do females choose to study humanities or social sciences,… 617
behavior, teachers and parents expect females to be more submissive, warm, and
protective than males. In contrast, a prototype male should be dynamic, success-
ful, and tough (Eccles et al. 1990; Kite and Whitley 2016). Hall and Briton (1993)
identified representative gender-stereotypical adjectives. The authors claim
that “in general masculine and feminine traits fall into two categories: agentic
or instrumental for the masculine and expressive or communal for feminine” (p.
278).
Different expectancies also apply to educational achievement of female and
male students (Jussim et al. 1996; Simpkins et al. 2006). If females are more emo-
tional and warmer, they should present higher scores in H/SS classes, the mas-
culine, analytical, and lively males’ minds allegedly predispose them to achieve
higher results in T/S classes.
Through the analysis of the achievements in mathematics of mid-grade female
students at primary school, Tiedemann (2000) showed that they are significantly
differentiated by their mothers’ and fathers’ gender stereotype-tinged expectan-
cies, as well as by the transformation of the children’s self-concept of mathemati-
cal ability caused by parents’ bias perception. Parents who believed in the ste-
reotype, as opposed to those immune to its impact, assigned lower mathematical
abilities (but not the effort put into learning) to their daughters than their sons.
Commenting on the obtained results, the author underlines: “although the effects
are not large, they are consistent, and they appear to influence children’s self-per-
ceptions in a manner consistent with the self-fulfilling prophecy hypothesis” (p.
149). Analogous results were revealed for gender stereotype-tinged expectancies.
In the next study, involving a similar age structure sample, McKown and Wein-
stein (2002) found that girls were more than 3.5 times as likely to confirm teach-
ers’ low expectancies on their mathematics ability compared to high expectan-
cies. In contrast, boys were only about 0.5 times as likely to confirm teachers’ low
compared to high expectancies. Furthermore, the power of the identified trend
intensified as the age of students increased. A similar impact of the gender stereo-
type did not occur in the case of students’ literary achievements.
Expectancies of others based on gender stereotypes may affect students’ edu-
cational achievement and subsequently their selection of T/S versus H/SS courses
and majors. Hattie (2009), based on meta-analysis of 674 studies, calculated that
the average effect size of expectancies for students’ educational achievement
was d = .43. This means that about 35% of students show higher or lower aca-
demic performance than would be predicted based on other factors than teachers’
expectancies.
It seems that consistent communication of teachers’ expectancies related to gen-
der stereotypes reflected in preferential treatment of females and males in arts and
mathematics classes, respectively, may lead the former to select H/SS majors and
the latter T/S ones (Harris and Rosenthal 1985; Rubie-Davies 2015).
However, it is hardly known to what extent the transmission of gender stereotype-
tinged expectancies of significant others occurs specifically in diades of the same
versus different sexes (i.e., from teacher/parent man/woman to girl/boy vs. from
teacher/parent man/woman to boy/girl). The available evidence is not conclusive
(Brophy and Good 1974; Bernard et al. 1981; Rubie-Davies 2015).
13
618 S. Trusz
Gender stereotypes are the source of interpersonal expectancies and, on the other
hand, of students’ expectancies about themselves. By asking questions such as
“What am I good at?” and “What subjects should I learn?” children seek answers
through the analysis of parents’ and teachers’ expectancy-tinged communications
(Babad 2009; Cvencek et al. 2011; Darley and Fazio 1980). Therefore, boys who
are socialized to fulfill roles typically attributed to males know that activities
including emotional involvement are appropriate for girls and that they should
stop themselves from crying even though they have a broken knee hurts a great
deal. On the other hand, girls are aware that climbing trees is not the kind of
activity that “ladies” do, whereas feeling sorry for people in need is.
For instance, Pajares and Miller (1994) found a strong link between students’ judg-
ments about their capability to solve math problems and their mathematics achievement.
Therefore, self-efficacy mediated the effect of gender and prior experience on math-
ematics self-concept, and then on mathematics problem-solving performance. Male and
female students differed in mathematics performance, self-efficacy, and self-concept,
and these differences were mediated by differences in the students’ self-efficacy percep-
tions. It should be emphasized that lower performance and more negative self-concept
of the female students were largely determined by lower judgments of their capabilities.
Similar results, in more recent studies, were obtained by Plante et al. (2019), Rudman
and Phelan (2010), Szumski and Karwowski (2019), and Trusz (2018).
To sum up, interpersonal expectancy effects can be regulated by students’
self-expectancies and self-concepts of abilities. The indicated factors connect the
expectancies of significant others with students’ achievement, and at the same
time, directly influence their educational careers (Trusz 2018; Rubie-Davies 2006;
Tosto et al. 2016; Wigfield and Eccles 2000). Consequently, inter and intraper-
sonal stereotype-tinged expectancies may influence the results of the matriculation
tests and subsequent selection of study majors made by females and males.
13
Why do females choose to study humanities or social sciences,… 619
13
620 S. Trusz
2 Objectives
In the current study, an attempt was made to: (1) establish factors that make female
students give preference to H/SS versus T/S majors, (2) establish factors that make
male students give preference to T/S versus H/SS majors, and (3) test whether rela-
tions between the established predictors and the study majors chosen by females and
males are modified by the gender of mathematics and literacy class teachers.
The study assumed that educational paths of females and males can be influenced
by two groups of factors: (1) variables related to stereotypical beliefs on which study
majors are appropriate for females and males, and (2) objective variables—with
opposite vector—affecting academic performance. The first group included: parents’
and the teacher’s expectancies regarding children/students’ achievement in mathe-
matics and literacy, students’ self-expectancies, and their self-concepts of abilities
and predicted successes in exams in these subjects.
The second group included: the results of mathematics and literacy sections of
the matriculation test (equivalent to the Scholastic Assessment Test or American
College Testing in the USA) taken five months earlier in secondary school (scale
from 0 to 100 points, with a 30-point grade threshold) and average daily time (in
minutes) spent on learning mathematics and literacy prior to taking the matricula-
tion test in these subjects. Test results and time of learning particular subjects reflect
the effort invested in learning and correlate with predispositions of people to study
them (Good and Brophy 2008; Wentzel and Wigfield 2009). Moreover, the math-
ematics and literacy tests results are critical for admission to university. The study
also assumed that the relations between factors from groups 1/2 and the outcome
variable (selection of T/S vs. H/SS majors) could be modified by the gender of
mathematics and literacy teacher in the final grade of secondary school.
To summarize, in the current study, two models were tested in which the out-
come variable was the study major selected by females (model 1) and males (model
2), and predictors were: (1) parents’ expectancies concerning their child achieve-
ment in mathematics and (2) literacy, (3) the teacher’s expectancies regarding their
students’ achievement in mathematics and (4) literacy, (5) students’ self-expectan-
cies concerning matriculation test results in mathematics and (6) literacy, (7) their
self-concept of mathematical and (8) literacy abilities, (9) the average declared time
spent on learning mathematics and (10) literacy per day, (11) declared results in the
13
Why do females choose to study humanities or social sciences,… 621
mathematics and (12) literacy sections of the matriculation test, and (13) gender of
the mathematics and (14) literacy teacher in the final grade of secondary school.
3 Method
3.1 Participants
3.2 Materials
3.3 Research procedure
The study was conducted individually. On the campuses of the universities where
the subjects studied, e.g., in libraries, canteens, university corridors, during breaks
between classes, in dormitories etc., the assistants of the principal investigator asked
the subjects to complete the questionnaires. Having obtained the participant’s writ-
ten consent, the interviewer provided the respondent with appropriate forms and
waited for him or her to complete them. If the participant requested additional infor-
mation on the aims of the study, the interviewer provided a standard answer pointing
out that “it concerns circumstances related to the subject’s selection of study major,
e.g., their parents and the teacher’s beliefs on their mathematical and literacy abili-
ties, the time spent on learning various subjects, their own interests, etc.”.
13
622
13
Part of tool Number of Content of illustrative items Cronbach’s
items alpha value
Parents’ expectancies
Mathematics 7 My parents told me that I am good at mathematics .911
My parents are convinced that I cannot deal well with science
Literacy 7 My parents have always told me that I am skilled in humanities .899
My parents think I am weak at arts
Teacher’s expectancies
Mathematics 7 Teachers were convinced that I would select technology or science majors .928
At secondary school, teachers stressed that I am good at mathematics
Literacy 7 Teachers were convinced that I would successfully pass the literacy section of the matriculation test .911
Teachers believed in my humanistic abilities
Students’ self-expectancies
Mathematics 7 I was convinced that I would pass the mathematics section of the matriculation test better than most of my .857
friends
In the final year of secondary school, I was more concerned about the mathematics than the literacy section
of the matriculation test
Literacy 7 I believed that I would be able to complete most of the literacy section of the matriculation test .800
I assumed that I would not be able to pass the literacy section of the matriculation test
Students’ self-concept
Mathematics 6 I am very good at mathematics .852
Solving physics problems has always been my weak point
Literacy 6 I could successfully publish my own texts in various magazines or on the Internet .840
The grammar of my mother tongue makes me confused
Total 54 .779
S. Trusz
Why do females choose to study humanities or social sciences,… 623
4 Results
Table 2 Descriptive statistics of predictors included in models within the group of females and males
Predictors Descriptive statistics
Min Max Mean SEM SD Kurtosis Skewness
Females
Parents’ expectancies—mathematics 1 4 2.815 .040 .834 − .214 − 1.056
Parents’ expectancies—literacy 1 4 2.922 .037 .775 − .255 − .904
Teacher’s expectancies—mathematics 1 4 2.536 .041 .865 .052 − 1.040
Teacher’s expectancies—literacy 1 4 2.668 .039 .813 .004 − .993
Students’ self-expectancies—mathemat- 1 4 2.693 .037 .769 − .148 − .856
ics
Students’ self-expectancies—literacy 1 4 2.953 .030 .638 − .472 − .221
Students’ self-concept—mathematics 1 4 2.536 .038 .795 .029 − .954
Students’ self-concept—literacy 1 4 2.554 .029 .609 − .101 − .843
Time spent on learning—mathematics 0 250 65.236 2.211 46.387 1.664 3.246
Time spent on learning—literacy 0 360 45.972 1.957 41.049 2.589 10.971
Results of the matriculation test—math- 30 100 69.769 .990 20.758 − .251 − 1.086
ematics
Results of the matriculation test—lit- 30 100 73.034 .670 14.063 − .471 .047
eracy
Males
Parents’ expectancies—mathematics 1 4 2.782 .040 .837 − .242 − 1.005
Parents’ expectancies—literacy 1 4 2.736 .037 .769 − .003 − .872
Teacher’s expectancies—mathematics 1 4 2.534 .043 .889 .074 − 1.096
Teacher’s expectancies—literacy 1 4 2.524 .039 .808 .147 − .916
Students’ self-expectancies—mathemat- 1 4 2.733 .036 .750 − .091 − .897
ics
Students’ self-expectancies—literacy 1 4 2.908 .029 .605 − .149 − .517
Students’ self-concept—mathematics 1 4 2.641 .039 .812 − .024 − 1.097
Students’ self-concept—literacy 1 4 2.497 .030 .630 − .017 − 1.017
Time spent on learning—mathematics 0 270 52.782 1.994 41.630 1.591 3.977
Time spent on learning—literacy 0 240 36.501 1.725 36.011 1.993 5.362
Results of the matriculation test—math- 30 100 69.311 1.002 20.920 − .201 − 1.218
ematics
Results of the matriculation test—lit- 30 100 70.731 .730 15.248 − .224 − .563
eracy
All statistics are calculated on the basis of data collected from 876 subjects; SE of the kurtosis and skew-
ness for all predictors = .233 and .117, respectively
13
624
13
(1) Parents’ expectancies—mathemat- – − .605** .861** − .648** .759** − .441** .805** − .646** .205** − .242** .737** − .235**
ics − .516** .856** − .629** .788** − .420** .799** − .591** .203** − .255** .740** − .302**
(2) Parents’ expectancies—literacy – − .621** .865** − .533** .716** − .646** .816** − .157** .311** − .525** .466**
− .587** .841** − .523** .708** − .615** .794** − .063 .370** − .483** .532**
(3) Teacher’s expectancies—math- – − .636** .764** − .480** .814** − .659** .212** − .214** .722** − .229**
ematics − .652** .756** − .455** .850** − .653** .277** − .261** .771** − .319**
(4) Teacher’s expectancies—literacy – − .546** .683** − .678** .863** − .159** .311** − .556** .454**
− .594** .663** − .706** .811** .108* .384** − .598** .516**
(5) Students’ self-expectancies—math- – − .439** .718** − .556** .142** − .186** .710** − .193**
ematics − .393** .706** − .566** .131** − .272** .768** − .236**
(6) Students’ self-expectancies—lit- – − .465** .678** − .177** .147** − .368** .479**
eracy − .486** .683** − .067 .291** − .367** .595**
(7) Students’ self-concept—mathemat- – − .661** .162** − .243** .687** − .256**
ics − .696** .204** − .298** .723** − .344**
(8) Students’ self-concept—literacy – − .195** .270** − .536** .410**
− .132** .335** − .568** .498**
(9) Time spent on learning—math- – .285** .189** − .151**
ematics .265** .210** .001
(10) Time spent on learning—literacy – − .193** .143**
− .269** .283**
(11) Results of the matriculation – − .010
test—mathematics − .184**
(12) Results of the matriculation –
test—literacy
All statistics are calculated on the basis of data collected from 876 subjects; in the cells, the first value refers to females and the second one refers to males; *p < .05;
**p < .01; two-tailed test
S. Trusz
Why do females choose to study humanities or social sciences,… 625
females and 41 males were excluded from the analysis. Ultimately, the models were
tested based on materials obtained from 402 females and 395 males.
The relationships between the predictors and the outcome variable were linear
(RESET test value for females was 1.288, df1 = 28, df2 = 365, p > .05 and for males
was 1.410, df1 = 28, df2 = 352, p > .05). Tested models met the assumption that the
predictors were not collinear (VIF statistics for females below 6.135 and tolerance
above .163; and for males below 6.198 and above .161, respectively). All statistics
presented in the paper were conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics 22 and R version
3.1.0 software.
The assumed models were tested using logistic regression analysis. Tables 4 and
5 summarize the results obtained for females and males (non-significant predictors
have been excluded from the analyses).
Table 4 Significant predictors of selecting T/S versus H/SS majors among females
b S.E Wald t(395) exp(b) 95% CI for
exp(b)
*p < .05; **p < .01; Hosmer and Lemeshow χ2(8) = 9.991; p ns.; − 2 Log likelihood = 247.495; Cox and
Snell R2 = .537; Nagelkerke R2 = .716; AIC = 261.495
Table 5 Significant predictors of selecting T/S versus H/SS majors among males
b S.E. Wald t(388) exp(b) 95% CI for
exp(b)
*p < .05; **p < .01; Hosmer and Lemeshow χ2(8) = 2.643; p ns.; − 2 Log likelihood = 233.642; Cox and
Snell R2 = .548; Nagelkerke R2 = .731; AIC = 247.642
13
626 S. Trusz
The tested models fitted the data well. Predictions regarding changes in the prob-
ability of selecting study major based on the factors included in the analysis are
more accurate than those based only on the intercept. Among females, significant
predictors explained 73%, and among males, 71%, of the outcome variable variance.
Regardless of the participants’ gender, the factors affecting the selection of study
major were: the teacher’s expectancies regarding students’ achievement in mathe-
matics and their results in the matriculation exam in this subject. In contrast, specific
predictors among females were: parents’ and the teacher’s expectancies concern-
ing daughters’/female students’ achievement in literacy and their self-expectancies
referring to achievement in mathematics and literacy, and among males: their self-
concept of mathematical and literacy abilities and time spent on learning both of
these subjects. Finally, the factors that, contrary to assumptions, did not affect the
outcome variable independently in the models were: parents’ expectancies regard-
ing their children’s achievement in mathematics, results in the literacy section of the
matriculation test, and gender of the mathematics and literacy teacher in the final
grade of secondary school.
The strongest predictor in the group of females were the teacher’s expectancies regard-
ing female students’ achievement in mathematics (exp(b) = 5.388), and then their self-
expectancies and the teacher’s expectancies concerning female students’ achievement in
literacy (exp(b) = 3.622 and .134, respectively). In contrast, among males the three most
important factors were: their self-concept of mathematical abilities (exp(b) = 3.643),
time spent on learning literacy (exp(b) = .978), and mathematics (exp(b) = 1.019).
In terms of predictors of selecting the study major that are common to females
and males, the direction of their influence was the same but the strength was differ-
ent. The change of the teacher’s expectancies regarding students’ literacy achieve-
ment by 1 point of the scale resulted in decreasing probability of selecting a T/S
major by 55% among males and by as much as 87% among females. Similarly, the
increase in the result of the matriculation test in mathematics promoted to a greater
extent the selection of a T/S major among males than females. The change in the test
result by 10 points was associated with an increase in the probability by 135% in the
first group, and by 126% in the second group.
Data presented in Table 4 points out that T/S majors selected by females were
linked to the teacher’s expectancies regarding female students’ achievement in math-
ematics. Their change by 1 unit of the scale increased the chance of selecting T/S
majors almost 5.5 times, which was preceded by an increase in time spent on learn-
ing mathematics and the result of the matriculation test in these subjects (correla-
tions between these factors, r = .212 and .722; ps < .001, respectively).
Moreover, among the predictors specific for females were: parents’ expectancies
concerning their daughters’ achievement in mathematics and females’ self-expec-
tancies regarding achievement in mathematics and literacy with opposite vectors.
The improvement of the first factor by 1 point of the scale was associated with the
decrease in probability of selecting T/S majors by 73%. Paradoxically, a similar
change in the other two, led to a 58% decrease and a 166% increase in the probabil-
ity of selecting T/S majors.
Considering data in Table 5, the selection of T/S majors by males was signifi-
cantly correlated with their effort put into learning of mathematics and literacy. The
13
Why do females choose to study humanities or social sciences,… 627
increase in the time of learning mathematics by every 20 min increased the proba-
bility of studying T/S by 146%, which was preceded by an improvement in the result
of the matriculation test in this subject (correlation between indicated predictors was
r = .210; p < .01). In contrast, increasing time spent on learning literacy by 20 min
reduced the probability of selecting T/S majors by 155%, which was preceded by a
higher result in the matriculation test in this subject (correlation between indicated
predictors was r = .283; p < .01).
Finally, the improvement of male students’ self-concept of mathematical abili-
ties by 1 point of the scale increased the chance of studying T/S majors 3.5 times. A
similar change in self-concept of literacy abilities was related to a 84% decrease in
the probability of selecting T/S majors.
13
628 S. Trusz
The presented profiles allow the assumption that among students with low val-
ues of the analyzed predictors, the probability of selection of T/S majors was simi-
larly low regardless of the mathematics teacher’s gender (differences from 7% for
student’s self-expectancies to 8% for other factors). The probability of selection
increased with the improvement of the predictors’ value, and it was significantly
higher when the mathematics classes were taught by female than male teachers (dif-
ferences from 45% for students’ self-expectancies to 51% for their self-concept).
The other four interactions related the literacy teacher’s gender to the predictors
of students’ achievement in this subject, i.e.: (6) learning time (exp(b) = .984), (7)
the teacher’s expectancies (exp(b) = .413), (8) parents’ expectancies (exp(b) = .514),
and (9) students’ self-concept (exp(b) = .396). Figure 2 shows the listed interactions.
Regardless of the literacy teacher’s gender, the probability of selection of T/S
majors were similarly high among students with low values of indicated predic-
tors (differences from 1% for learning time to 5% for the teacher’s expectancies).
The probability of selection decreased with the decrease in the value of predic-
tors, and it was significantly lower when literacy classes were taught by female
than male teachers (differences from 25% for learning time to 69% for students’
self-concept).
Among females, out of 12 tested interactions, only five were significant. It
is worth emphasizing that all of them related the gender of a literacy teacher
to the factors of students’ achievement in this subject, i.e.: (1) learning time
(exp(b) = .982), (2) the teacher’s expectancies (exp(b) = .446), (3) parents’ expec-
tancies (exp(b) = .506), (4) students’ self-expectancies (exp(b) = .634), and (5)
their self-concept (exp(b) = .462). Figure 3 shows the first of these interactions.
The profiles presented in Fig. 3 indicated that greater involvement during liter-
acy classes was related to an increase in probability of selecting T/S majors when
this subject was taught by a male, and a decrease when it was taught by a female
13
Why do females choose to study humanities or social sciences,… 629
teacher (79% and 19%, respectively). No similar differences existed in the groups
which did not put much effort into learning literacy (56% and 55%, respectively).
Finally, the profiles of interactions from 2 to 5 were as shown in Fig. 2. There-
fore, they can be interpreted in the same way as the interactions from 6 to 9 that
were found in the group of male students. Differences among female students
with low versus high values of the analyzed predictors ranged from 1% for stu-
dents’ self-expectancies to 4% for the teacher’s expectancies, and from 14% for
students’ self-concept to 58% for the teacher’s expectancies, respectively.
5 Discussion of results
The goals of this study were: (1) to identify factors affecting the selection of T/S
versus H/SS majors by females and males, and (2) to assess mathematics and
literacy teacher gender as a moderator of relationships between the predictors
included in the models and the study majors selected by females and males.
Among females, the selection of H/SS, i.e., gender-stereotyped majors, was,
on the one hand, promoted by the teacher’s and parents’ high expectancies con-
cerning female students/daughters’ achievement in literacy and, paradoxically, by
their self-expectancies regarding achievement in mathematics; and on the other
hand, by the teacher’s low expectancies concerning female students achievement
in mathematics, the result of the matriculation test in this subject, and, surpris-
ingly, by their self-expectancies concerning achievement in literacy.
Among males, the selection of stereotypical, i.e.: T/S majors, on the one hand,
was enhanced by high values of self-concept of mathematical abilities, time spent
on learning mathematics and, consequently, the result of the matriculation test in
this subject; and on the other hand, by low values of the teacher’s expectancies
13
630 S. Trusz
13
Why do females choose to study humanities or social sciences,… 631
13
632 S. Trusz
13
Why do females choose to study humanities or social sciences,… 633
13
634 S. Trusz
However, such works are still missing. Assuming that the indicated factors, i.e., aca-
demic outcomes and the selection of specific study majors, are correlated, it seems
that the revealed regularities at least partially explain the results obtained in the cur-
rent study.
13
Why do females choose to study humanities or social sciences,… 635
Funding This work was supported by the National Science Centre in Poland (Grant No. 2012/05/D/
HS6/03350).
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License,
which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as
you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Com-
mons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article
are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the
material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is
not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission
directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licen
ses/by/4.0/.
References
Alon, S., & DiPrete, T. A. (2017). Gender differences in the formation of a field of study choice set.
Sociological Science, 2, 50–81.
13
636 S. Trusz
Aronson, J., & McGlone, M. S. (2009). Stereotype and social identity threat. In T. Nelson (Ed.), Hand-
book of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination (pp. 153–178). New York: Psychology Press.
Babad, E. (2009). The social psychology of the classroom. New York: Routledge.
Bernard, M. E., Keefauver, L. W., Elsworth, G., & Naylor, F. D. (1981). Sex-role behavior and gender in
teacher-student evaluations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 681–696.
Brophy J. E., & Good, T. L. (1974). Teacher-student relationships: Causes and consequences. New York:
Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Buckley, S. (2016). Gender and sex differences in student participation, achievement and engagement in
mathematics. http://research.acer.edu.au/learning_processes/18. Accessed August 29, 2018.
Burusic, J., Babarovic, T., & Seric, M. (2012). Differences in elementary school achievement between
girls and boys: Does the teacher gender play a role?”. European Journal of Psychology of Educa-
tion, 27, 523–538.
Byrne, M., Flood, B., Hassall, T., Joyce, J., Montaño, J. L., González, J. M., et al. (2012). Motivations,
expectations and preparedness for higher education: A study of accounting students in Ireland, the
UK, Spain and Greece. Accounting Forum, 36, 134–144.
Catalyst. (2018). Women in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM). http://www.
catalyst.org/knowledge/women-science-technology-engineering-and-mathematics-stem#footnote34
_8j0ubdw. Accessed August 29, 2018.
Cooper, H., & Hazelrigg, P. (1988). Personality moderators of interpersonal expectancy effects: An inte-
grative research review. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 937–949.
Cunningham, B. C., Hoyer, K. M., Sparks, D., & Ralph, J. (2015). Gender differences in science, tech-
nology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) interest, credits earned, and NAEP performance in
the 12th Grade. IES National Center for Education Sciences. https://nces.ed.gov/pubs2015/20150
75.pdf. Accessed August 29, 2018.
Cvencek, D., Meltzoff, A. N., & Greenwald, A. G. (2011). Math-gender stereotypes in elementary school
children. Child Development, 82, 766–779.
Darley, J. M., & Fazio, R. H. (1980). Expectancy confirmation processes arising in the social interaction
sequence. American Psychologist, 35, 867–881.
Darlington, R. B., & Hayes, A. F. (2016). Regression analysis and linear models: Concepts, application
and implementation. New York: The Guilford Press.
de Boer, H., Bosker, R. J., & van der Werf, M. P. C. (2010). Sustainability of teacher expectation bias
effects on long-term student performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 168–179.
Debacker, T. K., & Nelson, R. M. (2000). Motivation to learn science: Differences related to gender, class
type, and ability. Journal of Educational Research, 93, 245–254.
Driessen, G. (2007). The feminization of primary education: Effects of teachers’ sex on pupil achieve-
ment, attitudes and behaviour. Review of Education, 53, 183–203.
Eagly, A. H. (2009). The his and hers of prosocial behavior: An examination of the social psychology of
gender. American Psychologist, 64, 644–658.
Eagly, A. H., Johannesen-Schmidt, M. C., & Van Engen, M. L. (2003). Transformational, transactional,
and laissez-faire leadership styles: A meta-analysis comparing women and men. Psychological Bul-
letin, 129, 569–591.
Eccles, J., Jacobs, J. E., & Harold, R. D. (1990). Gender role stereotypes, expectancy effects, and parents’
socialization of gender differences. Journal of Social Issues, 46, 183–201.
Else-Quest, N. M., Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (2010). Cross-national patterns of gender differences in
mathematics: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 103–127.
Emerson, L., Fear, J., Fox, S., & Sanders, E. (2012). Parental engagement in learning and schooling. https
://www.aracy.org.au/publications-resources/command/download_file/id/7/filename/Parental_engag
ement_in_learning_and_schooling_Lessons_from_research_BUREAU_ARACY_August_2012.
pdf. Accessed August 29, 2018.
Eurostat. (2017). Tertiary education statistics. http://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/statistics-explained/index.php/
Tertiary_education_statistics. Accessed August 29, 2018.
Fan, W. (2011). Social influences, school motivation and gender differences: An application of the expec-
tancy-value theory. Educational Psychology, 31, 157–175.
Fiedler, K., Walther, E., Freytag, P., & Plessner, H. (2002). Judgment biases in a simulated classroom—A
cognitive-environmental approach. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 88,
527–561.
Garcia, J. N. (2014). Teacher and parent beliefs and expectations of parental involvement and how it
relates to student academic achievement. PhD diss.: Texas State University.
13
Why do females choose to study humanities or social sciences,… 637
Gentrup, S., & Rjosk, C. (2018). Pygmalion and the gender gap: Do teacher expectations contribute
to differences in achievement between boys and girls at the beginning of schooling? Educational
Research and Evaluation, 24, 295–323.
Gill, S., & Reynolds, A. J. (1999). Educational expectations and school achievement of urban African
American children. Journal of School Psychology, 37, 403–424.
Good, C., Aronson, J., & Harder, J. A. (2008). Problems in the pipeline: Stereotype threat and women’s
achievement in high-level math courses. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 29, 17–28.
Good, T. L., & Brophy, J. E. (2008). Looking in classrooms (10th ed.). New York: Pearson.
Hall, J. A., & Briton, N. J. (1993). Gender, nonverbal behavior, and expectations. In P. D. Blanck (Ed.),
Interpersonal expectations: Theory, research, and applications (pp. 276–295). New York: Cam-
bridge University Press.
Harris, M. J., & Rosenthal, R. (1985). Mediation of interpersonal expectancy effects. 31 meta-analyses.
Psychological Bulletin, 97, 363–386.
Hassett, J. M., Siebert, E. R., & Wallen, K. (2008). Sex differences in rhesus monkey toy preferences par-
allel those of children. Hormones and Behavior, 54, 359–364.
Hattie, J. (2009). Visible learning: A synthesis of over 800 meta-analyses relating to achievement. New
York: Routledge.
Helbig, M. (2012). Boys do not benefit from male teachers in their reading and mathematics skills:
Empirical evidence from 21 European Union and OECD countries. British Journal of Sociology of
Education, 33, 661–677.
Hongo, D. (2013). Gender differences in science, technology, engineering, mathematics and computer
science (STEM) programs at university. Statistics Canada. https://www150.statcan.gc.ca/n1/
pub/75-006-x/2013001/article/11874-eng.htm. Accessed August 29, 2018.
Huang, C. (2013). Gender differences in academic self-efficacy: A meta-analysis. European Journal of
Psychology of Education, 28, 1–35.
Hyde, J. S. (1990). Meta-analysis and the psychology of gender differences. Signs: Journal of Women in
Culture and Society, 16, 55–73.
Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychologist, 60, 581–592.
Hyde, J. S. (2014). Gender similarities and differences. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 373–398.
Hyde, J. S., Fennema, E., & Lamon, S. J. (1990). Gender differences in mathematics performance: A
meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 139–155.
Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (1988). Gender differences in verbal ability: A meta-analysis. Psychological
Bulletin, 104, 53–69.
Jacobs, J. E., Davis-Kean, P., Bleeker, M., Eccles, J., & Malanchuk, O. (2004). “I can, but I don’t want
to” The impact of parents, interests, and activities on gender differences in math. In A. M. Gallagher
& J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), Gender differences in mathematics: An integrative psychological approach
(pp. 246–263). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Jussim, L. (1986). Self-fulfilling prophecies. A theoretical and integrative review. Psychological Review,
93, 429–445.
Jussim, L., & Eccles, J. (1992). Teacher expectations II: Construction and reflection of student achieve-
ment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 947–961.
Jussim, L., & Eccles, J. (1995). Naturally occurring interpresonal expectancies. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.),
Social development: Review of personality and social psychology (pp. 74–108). Thousand Oaks,
CA: Sage.
Jussim, L., Eccles, J., & Madon, S. (1996). Social perception, social stereotypes, and teacher expecta-
tions: Accuracy and the quest for the powerful self-fulfilling prophecy. Advances in Experimental
Social Psychology, 28(C), 281–388.
Kite, M. E., & Whitley, B. E. (2016). Psychology of prejudice and discrimination (3rd ed.). New York:
Routledge.
Kling, K. C., Hyde, J. S., Showers, C. J., & Buswell, B. N. (1999). Gender differences in self-esteem: A
meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125(4), 470–500.
Legewie, J., & DiPrete, T. A. (2014). The high school environment and the gender gap in science and
engineering. Sociology of Education, 87, 259–280.
Li, Q. (1999). Teachers’ beliefs and gender differences in mathematics: A review. Educational Research,
41, 63–76.
Lindberg, S. M., Hyde, J. S., Petersen, J. L., & Linn, M. C. (2010). New trends in gender and mathemat-
ics performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 1123–1135.
13
638 S. Trusz
Machin, S., & Pekkarinen, T. (2008). Assessment: Global sex differences in test score variability. Sci-
ence, 322, 1331–1332.
McKown, C., & Weinstein, R. S. (2002). Modeling the role of child ethnicity and gender in children’s
differential response to teacher expectations. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32, 159–184.
Meece, J. L., Glienke, B. B., & Askew, K. (2009). Gender and motivation. In K. R. Wentzel & A. Wig-
field (Eds.), Handbook on motivation at school (pp. 411–432). New York: Routledge.
Nguyen, H. H., & Ryan, A. M. (2008). Does stereotype threat affect test performance of minorities and
women? A meta-analysis of experimental evidence. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 1314–1334.
Pajares, F., & Miller, M. D. (1994). Role of self-efficacy and self-concept beliefs in mathematical prob-
lem solving: A path analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 193–203.
Plante, I., O’Keefe, P. A., Aronson, J., Fréchette-Simard, C., & Goulet, M. (2019). The interest gap: How
gender stereotype endorsement about abilities predicts differences in academic interests. Social Psy-
chology of Education, 22, 227–245.
Reilly, D. (2012). Gender, culture, and sex-typed cognitive abilities. PLoS ONE. https://doi.org/10.1371/
journal.pone.0039904.
Rhodewalt, F. (2008). Self-handicapping: On the self-perpetuating nature of defensive behavior. Social
and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 1255–1268.
Ridgeway, C. L. (2011). Framed by gender: How gender inequality persists in the modern world. Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Robnett, R. D. (2016). Gender bias in stem fields: Variation in prevalence and links to stem self-concept.
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 40, 65–79.
Rosenthal, R. (1997). Interpersonal expectancy effects: A forty-year perspective (p. 16). Chicago, August:
Paper presented at The American Psychological Association convention.
Rubie-Davies, C. M. (2006). Teacher expectations and student self-perceptions: Exploring relationships.
Psychology in the Schools, 43, 537–552.
Rubie-Davies, C. M. (2015). Becoming a high expectation teacher: Raising the bar. New York:
Routledge.
Rudman, L. A., & Phelan, J. E. (2010). The effect of priming gender roles on women’s implicit gender
beliefs and career aspirations. Social Psychology, 41(3), 192–202.
Schmader, T. (2002). Gender identification moderates stereotype threat effects on women’s math perfor-
mance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 194–201.
Shapiro, J. R., & Williams, A. M. (2012). The role of stereotype threats in undermining girls’ and wom-
en’s performance and interest in stem fields. Sex Roles, 66, 175–183.
Simpkins, S. D., Davis-Kean, P. E., & Eccles, J. (2006). Math and science motivation: A longitudinal
examination of the links between choices and beliefs. Developmental Psychology, 42, 70–83.
Simpkins, S. D., Estrella, G., Gaskin, E., & Kloberdanz, E. (2018). Latino parents’ science beliefs and
support of high school students’ motivational beliefs: Do the relations vary across gender and
familism values? Social Psychology of Education, 21, 1203–1224.
Smith, A. E., Jussim, L., & Eccles, J. (1999). Do self-fulfilling prophecies accumulate, dissipate, or
remain stable over time? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 548–565.
Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M., & Quinn, D. M. (1999). Stereotype threat and women’ s math performance.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 4–28.
Stangor, C. (2009). The study of stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination within social psychology:
A quick history of theory and research. In T. D. Nelson (Ed.), Handbook of prejudice, stereotyping,
and discrimination (pp. 1–22). New York: Psychology Press.
Starr, C. R., & Leaper, C. (2019). Do adolescents’ self-concepts moderate the relationship between
STEM stereotypes and motivation? Social Psychology of Education, 22, 1109–1129.
Sullivan, A. (2009). Academic self-concept, gender and single-sex schooling. British Educational
Research Journal, 35, 259–288.
Szumski, G., & Karwowski, M. (2019). Exploring the Pygmalion effect: The role of teacher expectations,
academic self-concept, and class context in students’ math achievement. Contemporary Educational
Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2019.101787.
Tiedemann, J. (2000). Parents’ gender stereotypes and teachers’ beliefs as predictors of children’s con-
cept of their mathematical ability in elementary school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92,
144–151.
Tomasetto, C., Alparone, F. R., & Cadinu, M. (2011). Girls’ math performance under stereotype threat:
The moderating role of mothers’ gender stereotypes. Developmental Psychology, 47, 943–949.
13
Why do females choose to study humanities or social sciences,… 639
Tosto, M. G., Asbury, K., Mazzocco, M., Petrill, S. A., & Kovas, Y. (2016). From classroom environ-
ment to mathematics achievement: The mediating role of self-perceived ability and subject interest.
Learning and Individual Differences, 50, 260–269.
Trusz, S. (2018). Four mediation models of teacher expectancy effects on students’ outcomes in math-
ematics and literacy. Social Psychology of Education, 21, 257–287.
Trusz, S., & Bąbel, P. (Eds.). (2016). Intrapersonal and interpersonal expectancies. New York:
Routledge.
Voyer, D. (2011). Time limits and gender differences on paper-and-pencil tests of mental rotation: A
meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 267–277.
Voyer, D., & Voyer, S. D. (2014). Gender differences in scholastic achievement: A meta-analysis. Psycho-
logical Bulletin, 140, 1174–1204.
Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A meta-
analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 250–270.
Walkey, F. H., McClure, J., Meyer, L. H., & Weir, K. F. (2013). Low expectations equal no expectations:
Aspirations, motivation, and achievement in secondary school. Contemporary Educational Psychol-
ogy, 38, 306–315.
Wang, M.-T., & Degot, J. L. (2017). Gender gap in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics
(STEM): Current knowledge, implications for practice, policy, and future directions. Educational
Psychology Review, 29, 119–140.
Watson, P. W., Rubie-Davies, C. M., Meissel, K., Peterson, E. R., Flint, A., Garrett, L., et al. (2017).
Teacher gender, and expectation of reading achievement in New Zealand elementary school stu-
dents: Essentially a barrier? Gender and Education. https://doi.org/10.1080/09540253.2017.14101
08.
Weathington, B. L., Cunningham, C. J., & Pittenger, D. J. (2010). Research methods for the behavioral
and social sciences. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Wentzel, K. R., & Wigfield, A. (Eds.). (2009). Handbook on motivation at school. New York: Routledge.
Whitley, B. E. (1997). Gender differences in computer-related attitudes and behavior: A meta-analysis.
Computers in Human Behavior, 13, 1–22.
Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. (2000). Expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation. Contemporary
Educational Psychology, 25, 68–81.
Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published
maps and institutional affiliations.
Sławomir Trusz Ph.D., is a Professor of Pedagogy and Head of Educational Microprocesses Laboratory
at the Pedagogical University of Kraków, Poland. His leading areas of interest are the effects of intra-
and interpersonal expectancies and educational self-fulfilling prophecy mechanism. His recent studies
focused on stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination in relation to children with oncological disease.
He co-authored the book entitled Intrapersonal and interpersonal expectancies, Routledge, 2016 (with
Przemysław Bąbel, Jagiellonian University in Poland).
13