s11218 020 09551 5

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 25

Social Psychology of Education (2020) 23:615–639

https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-020-09551-5

Why do females choose to study humanities or social


sciences, while males prefer technology or science? Some
intrapersonal and interpersonal predictors

Sławomir Trusz1

Received: 4 October 2018 / Accepted: 12 March 2020 / Published online: 18 April 2020
© The Author(s) 2020

Abstract
The article attempts to answer the question why females prefer humanities/social
studies, whereas males opt for technology/science. For this purpose, the study
majors selected by 445 females and 431 males were linked by logit functions with:
(1) parents’ and (2) teachers’ expectancies, (3) students’ self-expectancies, (4) their
self-concepts of abilities and (5) time spent on learning mathematics and (6) lit-
eracy, (7) test results in mathematics and (8) literacy, (9) gender of mathematics,
and (10) literacy teacher in the 12th grade. Interaction effects of the mathematics
and literacy teacher’s gender with the abovementioned predictors were also quanti-
fied. Females’ selections were mostly influenced by teachers’ expectancies, while for
males, by their self-concepts. The teacher’s gender modified tested relations in five
(females) and nine (males) cases. The results were discussed on the grounds of the
theory of intra- and interpersonal expectancies as learning regulators.

Keywords Gender stereotype · Interpersonal and intrapersonal expectancies ·


Educational achievement of females and males

1 Introduction

Are girls actually more polite and hard-working than boys, and boys more aggres-
sive and pugnacious than girls? Feminists say not, arguing that gender stereotypes
are sources of significant differences in behavior, and later, in educational attain-
ment of students. Gender stereotypes, i.e., unjustified representations of various
groups of people, by definition omitting any differences between them (Kite and
Whitley 2016; Stangor 2009), may incline parents and teachers to treat their daugh-
ters (female students) and sons (male students) differently.

* Sławomir Trusz
trusz@up.krakow.pl
1
Institute of Educational Sciences, Pedagogical University of Kraków, 4 Ingardena St.,
30‑060 Kraków, Poland

13
Vol.:(0123456789)
616 S. Trusz

Teachers at school may encourage male students to learn science more, e.g., by
praising them for their correctly conducted physical experiments, whereas female stu-
dents might be motivated to analyze the beauty of Gabriele Mistral’s poetry, according
to socially shared schema of technically talented boys and humanistically predisposed
girls (Eccles et al. 1990; Jussim et al. 1996; Watson et al. 2017). Similarly, at home,
parents who keep their daughters away from screwdrivers or their sons away from dolls
may show them how to bake cakes and replace a bicycle hub, respectively (Jacobs et al.
2004; Simpkins et al. 2018).
The noticeable gap in educational experience and performance of schematically
male versus female subjects among students cannot be explained solely by their abili-
ties (individual differences), but to some extent also by the cultural transmission of gen-
der stereotypes (e.g., Li 1999; Nguyen and Ryan 2008; Robnett 2016; Spencer et al.
1999; Tiedemann 2000; Watson et al. 2017).
In terms of this gap, the importance of expectations of significant others on the typi-
cal behavior of girls and boys, and correlated children’s self-expectations (e.g., Pajares
and Miller 1994; Rudman and Phelan 2010; Trusz 2018), their self-concept of ability in
humanities/social studies (H/SS) versus technology/science (T/S) (e.g., Sullivan 2009;
Szumski and Karwowski 2019; Trusz 2018), and time spent on gender-stereotyped and
gender-neutral activities must be emphasized (e.g., Debacker and Nelson 2000; Hyde
2014; Plante et al. 2019).
As a result, initially inaccurate gender stereotypes may be the source of real differ-
ences in the functioning of females and males, sized according to a specific domain of
human activity. The reason for this is provided by the results of meta-analyses in which
the average sizes of the gender effect for mathematical performance were Cohen’s
d = − .11 (Else-Quest et al. 2010), .16 (Lindberg et al. 2010), .22 (Reilly 2012), .29 and
.32 (Hyde et al. 1990), .43 (Hyde 1990); for cognitive abilities, e.g., mental rotation,
were .51 (Voyer 2011), .56 (Voyer et al. 1995), 1.03 (Voyer 2011); for literacy perfor-
mance were − .11 (Hyde and Linn 1988), − .37 (Voyer and Voyer 2014), − .44 (Reilly
2012); for academic (Huang 2013) and global self-efficacy (Kling et al. 1999), were .08
and .21, respectively; for leadership were − .13, .16, and .27 (Eagly et al. 2003); and for
prosocial behavior was .37 (Eagly 2009). Negative values of Cohen’s d represent higher
female results than male ones in the tested domain.
An important mechanism of this transformation seems to be the stereotype threat
(e.g., Nguyen and Ryan 2008; Schmader 2002; Shapiro and Williams 2012) and self-
fulfilling prophecy (e.g., Fiedler et al. 2002; Gentrup and Rjosk 2018; Watson et al.
2017). The indicated phenomena, together with the regulatory function of students’
self-expectations and their self-concept of ability in T/S and H/SS (e.g., Fan 2011;
Rudman and Phelan 2010; Szumski and Karwowski 2019) are discussed in more detail
in the following sections of this paper.

1.1 Teachers’ and parents’ expectancies related to females’ and males’


achievement

Gender stereotypes are sources of varied expectancies concerning the social


behavior and educational achievement of females and males. In the case of social

13
Why do females choose to study humanities or social sciences,… 617

behavior, teachers and parents expect females to be more submissive, warm, and
protective than males. In contrast, a prototype male should be dynamic, success-
ful, and tough (Eccles et al. 1990; Kite and Whitley 2016). Hall and Briton (1993)
identified representative gender-stereotypical adjectives. The authors claim
that “in general masculine and feminine traits fall into two categories: agentic
or instrumental for the masculine and expressive or communal for feminine” (p.
278).
Different expectancies also apply to educational achievement of female and
male students (Jussim et al. 1996; Simpkins et al. 2006). If females are more emo-
tional and warmer, they should present higher scores in H/SS classes, the mas-
culine, analytical, and lively males’ minds allegedly predispose them to achieve
higher results in T/S classes.
Through the analysis of the achievements in mathematics of mid-grade female
students at primary school, Tiedemann (2000) showed that they are significantly
differentiated by their mothers’ and fathers’ gender stereotype-tinged expectan-
cies, as well as by the transformation of the children’s self-concept of mathemati-
cal ability caused by parents’ bias perception. Parents who believed in the ste-
reotype, as opposed to those immune to its impact, assigned lower mathematical
abilities (but not the effort put into learning) to their daughters than their sons.
Commenting on the obtained results, the author underlines: “although the effects
are not large, they are consistent, and they appear to influence children’s self-per-
ceptions in a manner consistent with the self-fulfilling prophecy hypothesis” (p.
149). Analogous results were revealed for gender stereotype-tinged expectancies.
In the next study, involving a similar age structure sample, McKown and Wein-
stein (2002) found that girls were more than 3.5 times as likely to confirm teach-
ers’ low expectancies on their mathematics ability compared to high expectan-
cies. In contrast, boys were only about 0.5 times as likely to confirm teachers’ low
compared to high expectancies. Furthermore, the power of the identified trend
intensified as the age of students increased. A similar impact of the gender stereo-
type did not occur in the case of students’ literary achievements.
Expectancies of others based on gender stereotypes may affect students’ edu-
cational achievement and subsequently their selection of T/S versus H/SS courses
and majors. Hattie (2009), based on meta-analysis of 674 studies, calculated that
the average effect size of expectancies for students’ educational achievement
was d = .43. This means that about 35% of students show higher or lower aca-
demic performance than would be predicted based on other factors than teachers’
expectancies.
It seems that consistent communication of teachers’ expectancies related to gen-
der stereotypes reflected in preferential treatment of females and males in arts and
mathematics classes, respectively, may lead the former to select H/SS majors and
the latter T/S ones (Harris and Rosenthal 1985; Rubie-Davies 2015).
However, it is hardly known to what extent the transmission of gender stereotype-
tinged expectancies of significant others occurs specifically in diades of the same
versus different sexes (i.e., from teacher/parent man/woman to girl/boy vs. from
teacher/parent man/woman to boy/girl). The available evidence is not conclusive
(Brophy and Good 1974; Bernard et al. 1981; Rubie-Davies 2015).

13
618 S. Trusz

1.2 Students’ self‑expectancies concerning typically female and male activities

Gender stereotypes are the source of interpersonal expectancies and, on the other
hand, of students’ expectancies about themselves. By asking questions such as
“What am I good at?” and “What subjects should I learn?” children seek answers
through the analysis of parents’ and teachers’ expectancy-tinged communications
(Babad 2009; Cvencek et al. 2011; Darley and Fazio 1980). Therefore, boys who
are socialized to fulfill roles typically attributed to males know that activities
including emotional involvement are appropriate for girls and that they should
stop themselves from crying even though they have a broken knee hurts a great
deal. On the other hand, girls are aware that climbing trees is not the kind of
activity that “ladies” do, whereas feeling sorry for people in need is.
For instance, Pajares and Miller (1994) found a strong link between students’ judg-
ments about their capability to solve math problems and their mathematics achievement.
Therefore, self-efficacy mediated the effect of gender and prior experience on math-
ematics self-concept, and then on mathematics problem-solving performance. Male and
female students differed in mathematics performance, self-efficacy, and self-concept,
and these differences were mediated by differences in the students’ self-efficacy percep-
tions. It should be emphasized that lower performance and more negative self-concept
of the female students were largely determined by lower judgments of their capabilities.
Similar results, in more recent studies, were obtained by Plante et al. (2019), Rudman
and Phelan (2010), Szumski and Karwowski (2019), and Trusz (2018).
To sum up, interpersonal expectancy effects can be regulated by students’
self-expectancies and self-concepts of abilities. The indicated factors connect the
expectancies of significant others with students’ achievement, and at the same
time, directly influence their educational careers (Trusz 2018; Rubie-Davies 2006;
Tosto et al. 2016; Wigfield and Eccles 2000). Consequently, inter and intraper-
sonal stereotype-tinged expectancies may influence the results of the matriculation
tests and subsequent selection of study majors made by females and males.

1.3 The self‑fulfilling prophecy mechanism and gender‑stereotype threat


in relation to educational achievement of females and males

Different educational and professional careers of females and males, despite


similar input level of intellectual abilities (Hyde 2005), can be explained by two
mechanisms: the self-fulfilling prophecy and the gender stereotype threat.
The first mechanism occurs when a parent or teacher creates false expectancies
concerning individual characteristics of a child (e.g., my daughter is sensitive so
in the future she will choose to study humanities vs. my son solves logical rebuses
easily, so in the future he will study at the university of technology). Afterwards,
these expectancies are communicated to the child through the parent’s/teacher’s
verbal and nonverbal behavior. Finally, despite the original inappropriateness, the
expectancies are confirmed—the female actually selects a H/SS major and the
male goes for a T/SS one (Trusz and Bąbel 2016; Rubie-Davies 2015).

13
Why do females choose to study humanities or social sciences,… 619

Realization of these false expectancies is facilitated if they are accepted by chil-


dren and, consequently, incorporated into the self-schemas system, i.e., self-expec-
tancies and self-concepts of abilities (Jussim 1986; Jussim and Eccles 1995; Rubie-
Davies 2006). This is possible as a result of the stereotype threat mechanism. In
the Spencer et al. experiment (1999), a group of females and males solved a math-
ematics test. Participants were informed either that the tasks were difficult and males
should perform better due to allegedly higher analytical reasoning abilities (study 1),
or that the tasks were easy (study 2).
It turned out that alleviating the stereotype threat in study 2 caused females to
perform at a similar level to males. In contrast, providing information that tasks are
difficult for females induced an aversive affect and intense intellectual overstimula-
tion, which paradoxically, as a result of emotional and cognitive overload, under-
mined females’ outcomes as opposed to relaxed males.
The discussed results were replicated in a series of studies (e.g., Aronson and
McGlone 2009; Schmader 2002; Shapiro and Williams 2012; Tomasetto et al.
2011). After all, in a Nguyen and Ryan’s (2008) meta-analysis, based on the results
of 116 studies, the average size of the stereotype threat effect for women’s mathe-
matical achievements was Cohen’s d = .26. Moreover, the authors demonstrated that
the negative impact of the analyzed phenomenon is strongest for moderately math-
ematics-identified women (.52), followed by highly mathematics-identified women
(.29) and low mathematics-identified women (.11).

1.4 Learning effort and earlier outcomes as predictors of educational


achievement

In addition to socially constructed factors of educational achievement, i.e., inter- and


intrapersonal expectancies (de Boer et al. 2010; Rubie-Davies 2015), the educational out-
comes, and consequently females’ and males’ selections of study majors are also influ-
enced by objective variables, i.e., time invested in learning and results obtained in particu-
lar subjects.
In a meta-analysis of 100 studies, Hattie (2009) calculated that the average effect
size of learning time on academic success was d = .38. This means that about 30%
of students present higher or lower results than would have been predicted based on
factors other than the various times they spent on learning. Similarly, based on 3607
studies, the same author calculated that average effect size of prior achievement
for academic success was d = .67. This shows that about 35% of students present a
higher increase or decrease in academic results than could have been expected based
on promises other than their prior achievement.
It seems that people who invest more time in learning literacy versus mathemat-
ics and obtain higher results in matriculation tests in these subjects should be more
likely to select H/SS versus T/S majors, respectively (Meece et al. 2009).
Eurostat data (2017) confirm a noticeable imbalance between males and females
studying T/S majors (from 61 to 74% of males) and between females and males study-
ing H/SS majors (from 58 to 78% of females). Similar results are reported for non-
European countries, e.g., Canada, USA, Australia, India, and Japan (cf. Catalyst 2018).

13
620 S. Trusz

The aforementioned difference can be explained partly by the results achieved


in standardized tests—for female students higher in literacy and for male students
higher in mathematics (Machin and Pekkarinen 2008)—as well as by the impact
of gender stereotype-tinged expectancies of parents and teachers, students’ self-
expectancies, and their self-concepts (Rudman and Phelan 2010; Starr and Leaper
2019; Szumski and Karwowski 2019; Tiedemann 2000; Watson et al. 2017). Moreo-
ver, relations between the indicated factors and the study majors selected by K − 12
students may be significantly modified by the gender of mathematics and literacy
teachers during the final year of secondary school (Brophy and Good 1974; Bernard
et al. 1981; Rubie-Davies 2015).

2 Objectives

In the current study, an attempt was made to: (1) establish factors that make female
students give preference to H/SS versus T/S majors, (2) establish factors that make
male students give preference to T/S versus H/SS majors, and (3) test whether rela-
tions between the established predictors and the study majors chosen by females and
males are modified by the gender of mathematics and literacy class teachers.
The study assumed that educational paths of females and males can be influenced
by two groups of factors: (1) variables related to stereotypical beliefs on which study
majors are appropriate for females and males, and (2) objective variables—with
opposite vector—affecting academic performance. The first group included: parents’
and the teacher’s expectancies regarding children/students’ achievement in mathe-
matics and literacy, students’ self-expectancies, and their self-concepts of abilities
and predicted successes in exams in these subjects.
The second group included: the results of mathematics and literacy sections of
the matriculation test (equivalent to the Scholastic Assessment Test or American
College Testing in the USA) taken five months earlier in secondary school (scale
from 0 to 100 points, with a 30-point grade threshold) and average daily time (in
minutes) spent on learning mathematics and literacy prior to taking the matricula-
tion test in these subjects. Test results and time of learning particular subjects reflect
the effort invested in learning and correlate with predispositions of people to study
them (Good and Brophy 2008; Wentzel and Wigfield 2009). Moreover, the math-
ematics and literacy tests results are critical for admission to university. The study
also assumed that the relations between factors from groups 1/2 and the outcome
variable (selection of T/S vs. H/SS majors) could be modified by the gender of
mathematics and literacy teacher in the final grade of secondary school.
To summarize, in the current study, two models were tested in which the out-
come variable was the study major selected by females (model 1) and males (model
2), and predictors were: (1) parents’ expectancies concerning their child achieve-
ment in mathematics and (2) literacy, (3) the teacher’s expectancies regarding their
students’ achievement in mathematics and (4) literacy, (5) students’ self-expectan-
cies concerning matriculation test results in mathematics and (6) literacy, (7) their
self-concept of mathematical and (8) literacy abilities, (9) the average declared time
spent on learning mathematics and (10) literacy per day, (11) declared results in the

13
Why do females choose to study humanities or social sciences,… 621

mathematics and (12) literacy sections of the matriculation test, and (13) gender of
the mathematics and (14) literacy teacher in the final grade of secondary school.

3 Method

3.1 Participants

The sample was organized using a convenience-voluntary sampling scheme (Weath-


ington et al. 2010). The sample included participants who: (1) provided a written
consent to participate in the study, (2) were first-year students of H/SS (e.g., sociol-
ogy) or T/S majors (e.g., biochemistry), and (3) graduated from secondary school
and successfully passed the matriculation exams in the same calendar year, i.e.,
2017, when the investigation was conducted.
The study involved 876 subjects (445 females), first-year students (average age was
19.492; SD = .621) of different faculties in H/SS (n = 438; 220 females) and exact T/S
(n = 438; 220 females) of universities located in a large southeast, well-industrialized
region of Poland. The aims, procedure, and materials used in the study were approved
by the Ethics Committee for Research at the Pedagogical University in Kraków.

3.2 Materials

Variables included in the tested models were controlled by means of a questionnaire


developed by the paper’s author. The tool consists of four parts containing state-
ments assessed on a 4-point scale coded 1 (definitely inaccurate) to 4 (definitely
accurate). Table 1 summarizes the assessment of questionnaire reliability.
Furthermore, the questionnaire included the following respondent details: (1)
gender (coded: Male = 1; Female = 2) and (2) age of participant, (3) study majors
(coded: H/SS = 0; T/S = 1), (4) year of college, (5) result in the mathematics and (6)
literacy section of the matriculation test, (7) time spent on learning mathematics and
(8) literacy (reported in minutes per day), and (9) gender of the mathematics and
(10) literacy teacher in the final grade of secondary school.

3.3 Research procedure

The study was conducted individually. On the campuses of the universities where
the subjects studied, e.g., in libraries, canteens, university corridors, during breaks
between classes, in dormitories etc., the assistants of the principal investigator asked
the subjects to complete the questionnaires. Having obtained the participant’s writ-
ten consent, the interviewer provided the respondent with appropriate forms and
waited for him or her to complete them. If the participant requested additional infor-
mation on the aims of the study, the interviewer provided a standard answer pointing
out that “it concerns circumstances related to the subject’s selection of study major,
e.g., their parents and the teacher’s beliefs on their mathematical and literacy abili-
ties, the time spent on learning various subjects, their own interests, etc.”.

13
622

Table 1  Psychometric characteristics of the questionnaire

13
Part of tool Number of Content of illustrative items Cronbach’s
items alpha value

Parents’ expectancies
Mathematics 7 My parents told me that I am good at mathematics .911
My parents are convinced that I cannot deal well with science
Literacy 7 My parents have always told me that I am skilled in humanities .899
My parents think I am weak at arts
Teacher’s expectancies
Mathematics 7 Teachers were convinced that I would select technology or science majors .928
At secondary school, teachers stressed that I am good at mathematics
Literacy 7 Teachers were convinced that I would successfully pass the literacy section of the matriculation test .911
Teachers believed in my humanistic abilities
Students’ self-expectancies
Mathematics 7 I was convinced that I would pass the mathematics section of the matriculation test better than most of my .857
friends
In the final year of secondary school, I was more concerned about the mathematics than the literacy section
of the matriculation test
Literacy 7 I believed that I would be able to complete most of the literacy section of the matriculation test .800
I assumed that I would not be able to pass the literacy section of the matriculation test
Students’ self-concept
Mathematics 6 I am very good at mathematics .852
Solving physics problems has always been my weak point
Literacy 6 I could successfully publish my own texts in various magazines or on the Internet .840
The grammar of my mother tongue makes me confused
Total 54 .779
S. Trusz
Why do females choose to study humanities or social sciences,… 623

4 Results

4.1 Preliminary data analysis

Basic descriptive statistics of predictors included in the models are summarized in


Table 2, and Table 3 shows correlations among them.
Before quantifying the models, outliers were excluded from the sample. Consid-
ering leverage values (above .085) and Cook’s distances (above 1.00), data of 38

Table 2  Descriptive statistics of predictors included in models within the group of females and males
Predictors Descriptive statistics
Min Max Mean SEM SD Kurtosis Skewness

Females
Parents’ expectancies—mathematics 1 4 2.815 .040 .834 − .214 − 1.056
Parents’ expectancies—literacy 1 4 2.922 .037 .775 − .255 − .904
Teacher’s expectancies—mathematics 1 4 2.536 .041 .865 .052 − 1.040
Teacher’s expectancies—literacy 1 4 2.668 .039 .813 .004 − .993
Students’ self-expectancies—mathemat- 1 4 2.693 .037 .769 − .148 − .856
ics
Students’ self-expectancies—literacy 1 4 2.953 .030 .638 − .472 − .221
Students’ self-concept—mathematics 1 4 2.536 .038 .795 .029 − .954
Students’ self-concept—literacy 1 4 2.554 .029 .609 − .101 − .843
Time spent on learning—mathematics 0 250 65.236 2.211 46.387 1.664 3.246
Time spent on learning—literacy 0 360 45.972 1.957 41.049 2.589 10.971
Results of the matriculation test—math- 30 100 69.769 .990 20.758 − .251 − 1.086
ematics
Results of the matriculation test—lit- 30 100 73.034 .670 14.063 − .471 .047
eracy
Males
Parents’ expectancies—mathematics 1 4 2.782 .040 .837 − .242 − 1.005
Parents’ expectancies—literacy 1 4 2.736 .037 .769 − .003 − .872
Teacher’s expectancies—mathematics 1 4 2.534 .043 .889 .074 − 1.096
Teacher’s expectancies—literacy 1 4 2.524 .039 .808 .147 − .916
Students’ self-expectancies—mathemat- 1 4 2.733 .036 .750 − .091 − .897
ics
Students’ self-expectancies—literacy 1 4 2.908 .029 .605 − .149 − .517
Students’ self-concept—mathematics 1 4 2.641 .039 .812 − .024 − 1.097
Students’ self-concept—literacy 1 4 2.497 .030 .630 − .017 − 1.017
Time spent on learning—mathematics 0 270 52.782 1.994 41.630 1.591 3.977
Time spent on learning—literacy 0 240 36.501 1.725 36.011 1.993 5.362
Results of the matriculation test—math- 30 100 69.311 1.002 20.920 − .201 − 1.218
ematics
Results of the matriculation test—lit- 30 100 70.731 .730 15.248 − .224 − .563
eracy

All statistics are calculated on the basis of data collected from 876 subjects; SE of the kurtosis and skew-
ness for all predictors = .233 and .117, respectively

13
624

Table 3  Correlations of predictors within the group of females and males


(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9) (10) (11) (12)

13
(1) Parents’ expectancies—mathemat- – − .605** .861** − .648** .759** − .441** .805** − .646** .205** − .242** .737** − .235**
ics − .516** .856** − .629** .788** − .420** .799** − .591** .203** − .255** .740** − .302**
(2) Parents’ expectancies—literacy – − .621** .865** − .533** .716** − .646** .816** − .157** .311** − .525** .466**
− .587** .841** − .523** .708** − .615** .794** − .063 .370** − .483** .532**
(3) Teacher’s expectancies—math- – − .636** .764** − .480** .814** − .659** .212** − .214** .722** − .229**
ematics − .652** .756** − .455** .850** − .653** .277** − .261** .771** − .319**
(4) Teacher’s expectancies—literacy – − .546** .683** − .678** .863** − .159** .311** − .556** .454**
− .594** .663** − .706** .811** .108* .384** − .598** .516**
(5) Students’ self-expectancies—math- – − .439** .718** − .556** .142** − .186** .710** − .193**
ematics − .393** .706** − .566** .131** − .272** .768** − .236**
(6) Students’ self-expectancies—lit- – − .465** .678** − .177** .147** − .368** .479**
eracy − .486** .683** − .067 .291** − .367** .595**
(7) Students’ self-concept—mathemat- – − .661** .162** − .243** .687** − .256**
ics − .696** .204** − .298** .723** − .344**
(8) Students’ self-concept—literacy – − .195** .270** − .536** .410**
− .132** .335** − .568** .498**
(9) Time spent on learning—math- – .285** .189** − .151**
ematics .265** .210** .001
(10) Time spent on learning—literacy – − .193** .143**
− .269** .283**
(11) Results of the matriculation – − .010
test—mathematics − .184**
(12) Results of the matriculation –
test—literacy

All statistics are calculated on the basis of data collected from 876 subjects; in the cells, the first value refers to females and the second one refers to males; *p < .05;
**p < .01; two-tailed test
S. Trusz
Why do females choose to study humanities or social sciences,… 625

females and 41 males were excluded from the analysis. Ultimately, the models were
tested based on materials obtained from 402 females and 395 males.
The relationships between the predictors and the outcome variable were linear
(RESET test value for females was 1.288, df1 = 28, df2 = 365, p > .05 and for males
was 1.410, df1 = 28, df2 = 352, p > .05). Tested models met the assumption that the
predictors were not collinear (VIF statistics for females below 6.135 and tolerance
above .163; and for males below 6.198 and above .161, respectively). All statistics
presented in the paper were conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics 22 and R version
3.1.0 software.

4.2 Predictors of the selection of study majors among females and males

The assumed models were tested using logistic regression analysis. Tables 4 and
5 summarize the results obtained for females and males (non-significant predictors
have been excluded from the analyses).

Table 4  Significant predictors of selecting T/S versus H/SS majors among females
b S.E Wald t(395) exp(b) 95% CI for
exp(b)

Intercept 1.861 1.472 1.597 6.428


Parents’ expectancies—literacy − 1.314** .451 8.475 2.911 .269 .111 .651
Teacher’s expectancies—mathematics 1.684** .350 23.178 4.813 5.388 2.714 10.695
Teacher’s expectancies—literacy − 2.008** .445 20.370 4.512 .134 .056 .321
Students’ self-expectancies—mathematics − .871* .371 5.518 2.348 .418 .202 .866
Students’ self-expectancies—literacy 1.287** .396 10.536 3.245 3.622 1.665 7.879
Results of the matriculation test—math- .023* .012 3.836 1.958 1.024 .999 1.049
ematics

*p < .05; **p < .01; Hosmer and Lemeshow χ2(8) = 9.991; p ns.; − 2 Log likelihood = 247.495; Cox and
Snell ­R2 = .537; Nagelkerke ­R2 = .716; AIC = 261.495

Table 5  Significant predictors of selecting T/S versus H/SS majors among males
b S.E. Wald t(388) exp(b) 95% CI for
exp(b)

Intercept − 1.209 1.647 .539 .298


Teacher’s expectancies—literacy − .804* .374 4.618 2.149 .448 .215 .932
Students’ self-concept—mathematics 1.293** .342 14.321 3.784 3.643 1.865 7.118
Students’ self-concept—literacy − .950* .445 4.545 2.132 .387 .161 .926
Time spent learning—mathematics .019** .006 10.871 3.297 1.019 1.008 1.030
Time spent learning—literacy − .022** .007 10.011 3.164 .978 .965 .992
Results of the matriculation test—mathemat- .030** .011 7.933 2.816 1.031 1.009 1.053
ics

*p < .05; **p < .01; Hosmer and Lemeshow χ2(8) = 2.643; p ns.; − 2 Log likelihood = 233.642; Cox and
Snell ­R2 = .548; Nagelkerke ­R2 = .731; AIC = 247.642

13
626 S. Trusz

The tested models fitted the data well. Predictions regarding changes in the prob-
ability of selecting study major based on the factors included in the analysis are
more accurate than those based only on the intercept. Among females, significant
predictors explained 73%, and among males, 71%, of the outcome variable variance.
Regardless of the participants’ gender, the factors affecting the selection of study
major were: the teacher’s expectancies regarding students’ achievement in mathe-
matics and their results in the matriculation exam in this subject. In contrast, specific
predictors among females were: parents’ and the teacher’s expectancies concern-
ing daughters’/female students’ achievement in literacy and their self-expectancies
referring to achievement in mathematics and literacy, and among males: their self-
concept of mathematical and literacy abilities and time spent on learning both of
these subjects. Finally, the factors that, contrary to assumptions, did not affect the
outcome variable independently in the models were: parents’ expectancies regard-
ing their children’s achievement in mathematics, results in the literacy section of the
matriculation test, and gender of the mathematics and literacy teacher in the final
grade of secondary school.
The strongest predictor in the group of females were the teacher’s expectancies regard-
ing female students’ achievement in mathematics (exp(b) = 5.388), and then their self-
expectancies and the teacher’s expectancies concerning female students’ achievement in
literacy (exp(b) = 3.622 and .134, respectively). In contrast, among males the three most
important factors were: their self-concept of mathematical abilities (exp(b) = 3.643),
time spent on learning literacy (exp(b) = .978), and mathematics (exp(b) = 1.019).
In terms of predictors of selecting the study major that are common to females
and males, the direction of their influence was the same but the strength was differ-
ent. The change of the teacher’s expectancies regarding students’ literacy achieve-
ment by 1 point of the scale resulted in decreasing probability of selecting a T/S
major by 55% among males and by as much as 87% among females. Similarly, the
increase in the result of the matriculation test in mathematics promoted to a greater
extent the selection of a T/S major among males than females. The change in the test
result by 10 points was associated with an increase in the probability by 135% in the
first group, and by 126% in the second group.
Data presented in Table 4 points out that T/S majors selected by females were
linked to the teacher’s expectancies regarding female students’ achievement in math-
ematics. Their change by 1 unit of the scale increased the chance of selecting T/S
majors almost 5.5 times, which was preceded by an increase in time spent on learn-
ing mathematics and the result of the matriculation test in these subjects (correla-
tions between these factors, r = .212 and .722; ps < .001, respectively).
Moreover, among the predictors specific for females were: parents’ expectancies
concerning their daughters’ achievement in mathematics and females’ self-expec-
tancies regarding achievement in mathematics and literacy with opposite vectors.
The improvement of the first factor by 1 point of the scale was associated with the
decrease in probability of selecting T/S majors by 73%. Paradoxically, a similar
change in the other two, led to a 58% decrease and a 166% increase in the probabil-
ity of selecting T/S majors.
Considering data in Table 5, the selection of T/S majors by males was signifi-
cantly correlated with their effort put into learning of mathematics and literacy. The

13
Why do females choose to study humanities or social sciences,… 627

increase in the time of learning mathematics by every 20 min increased the proba-
bility of studying T/S by 146%, which was preceded by an improvement in the result
of the matriculation test in this subject (correlation between indicated predictors was
r = .210; p < .01). In contrast, increasing time spent on learning literacy by 20 min
reduced the probability of selecting T/S majors by 155%, which was preceded by a
higher result in the matriculation test in this subject (correlation between indicated
predictors was r = .283; p < .01).
Finally, the improvement of male students’ self-concept of mathematical abili-
ties by 1 point of the scale increased the chance of studying T/S majors 3.5 times. A
similar change in self-concept of literacy abilities was related to a 84% decrease in
the probability of selecting T/S majors.

4.3 The teacher’s gender as a moderator of study major selected by K − 12


students

The teacher’s gender as a moderator of study major selected by K − 12 students was


also tested. For this purpose, the product of the mathematics or literacy teacher’s
gender and particular predictors included in the tested models was calculated. It was
then introduced to the regression equations, and eventually used to quantify to what
extent it is related to the outcome variable.
Among males, out of 12 tested interactions, nine turned out to be significant. Five
of them related the mathematics teacher’s gender to achievement factors in this sub-
ject, i.e.: (1) the result of the matriculation test (exp(b) = 1.025), (2) the teacher’s
expectancies (exp(b) = 2.167), (3) parents’ expectancies (exp(b) = 1.868), (4) stu-
dents’ self-expectancies (exp(b) = 1.773), and (5) their self-concept (exp(b) = 2.208).
Figure 1 shows the revealed regularities.

Fig. 1  Predicted probability of


selecting a T/S major dependent
on the gender of the mathemat-
ics teacher and the value of the
quantified predictor (males).
By reference to the example
of the mathematics section of
the matriculation test factor.
Similar function sequences were
obtained for other factors related
to male students’ achieve-
ment in this subject, i.e.: (1)
the teacher’s and (2) parents’
expectancies, (3) students’
self-expectancies, and (4) their
self-concept

13
628 S. Trusz

Fig. 2  Predicted probability of


selecting a T/S major depend-
ent on the gender of the literacy
teacher and the value of the
quantified predictor (males).
By reference to the example
of the student self-concept of
literacy abilities. Similar func-
tion sequences were obtained
for other factors related to male
students’ achievement in this
subject, i.e.: (1) time spent on
learning, (2) the teacher’s and
(3) parents’ expectancies, and
(4) students’ self-concept

The presented profiles allow the assumption that among students with low val-
ues of the analyzed predictors, the probability of selection of T/S majors was simi-
larly low regardless of the mathematics teacher’s gender (differences from 7% for
student’s self-expectancies to 8% for other factors). The probability of selection
increased with the improvement of the predictors’ value, and it was significantly
higher when the mathematics classes were taught by female than male teachers (dif-
ferences from 45% for students’ self-expectancies to 51% for their self-concept).
The other four interactions related the literacy teacher’s gender to the predictors
of students’ achievement in this subject, i.e.: (6) learning time (exp(b) = .984), (7)
the teacher’s expectancies (exp(b) = .413), (8) parents’ expectancies (exp(b) = .514),
and (9) students’ self-concept (exp(b) = .396). Figure 2 shows the listed interactions.
Regardless of the literacy teacher’s gender, the probability of selection of T/S
majors were similarly high among students with low values of indicated predic-
tors (differences from 1% for learning time to 5% for the teacher’s expectancies).
The probability of selection decreased with the decrease in the value of predic-
tors, and it was significantly lower when literacy classes were taught by female
than male teachers (differences from 25% for learning time to 69% for students’
self-concept).
Among females, out of 12 tested interactions, only five were significant. It
is worth emphasizing that all of them related the gender of a literacy teacher
to the factors of students’ achievement in this subject, i.e.: (1) learning time
(exp(b) = .982), (2) the teacher’s expectancies (exp(b) = .446), (3) parents’ expec-
tancies (exp(b) = .506), (4) students’ self-expectancies (exp(b) = .634), and (5)
their self-concept (exp(b) = .462). Figure 3 shows the first of these interactions.
The profiles presented in Fig. 3 indicated that greater involvement during liter-
acy classes was related to an increase in probability of selecting T/S majors when
this subject was taught by a male, and a decrease when it was taught by a female

13
Why do females choose to study humanities or social sciences,… 629

Fig. 3  Predicted probability of


selecting T/S majors dependent
on the gender of the literacy
teacher and the time spent on
learning literacy (females)

teacher (79% and 19%, respectively). No similar differences existed in the groups
which did not put much effort into learning literacy (56% and 55%, respectively).
Finally, the profiles of interactions from 2 to 5 were as shown in Fig. 2. There-
fore, they can be interpreted in the same way as the interactions from 6 to 9 that
were found in the group of male students. Differences among female students
with low versus high values of the analyzed predictors ranged from 1% for stu-
dents’ self-expectancies to 4% for the teacher’s expectancies, and from 14% for
students’ self-concept to 58% for the teacher’s expectancies, respectively.

5 Discussion of results

The goals of this study were: (1) to identify factors affecting the selection of T/S
versus H/SS majors by females and males, and (2) to assess mathematics and
literacy teacher gender as a moderator of relationships between the predictors
included in the models and the study majors selected by females and males.
Among females, the selection of H/SS, i.e., gender-stereotyped majors, was,
on the one hand, promoted by the teacher’s and parents’ high expectancies con-
cerning female students/daughters’ achievement in literacy and, paradoxically, by
their self-expectancies regarding achievement in mathematics; and on the other
hand, by the teacher’s low expectancies concerning female students achievement
in mathematics, the result of the matriculation test in this subject, and, surpris-
ingly, by their self-expectancies concerning achievement in literacy.
Among males, the selection of stereotypical, i.e.: T/S majors, on the one hand,
was enhanced by high values of self-concept of mathematical abilities, time spent
on learning mathematics and, consequently, the result of the matriculation test in
this subject; and on the other hand, by low values of the teacher’s expectancies

13
630 S. Trusz

regarding male students’ achievement in literacy, their self-concept of literacy


abilities, and time spent on learning this subject.
It appears that study major selection among males was determined by a greater
number of objective factors than it was among females. Considering the time
spent on learning mathematics and literacy as well as the results of the matricula-
tion test in mathematics correlated with them, male students decided to select T/S
majors (reasoning: since I invested more effort in learning mathematics than lit-
eracy, which resulted in a satisfactory outcome of the matriculation test in math-
ematics, then T/S major will be a better choice).
On the other hand, among female students one objective predictor of study major
selection was the result of the matriculation test in mathematics, although its impact
was weaker than among males. Other factors were more subjective and partly gen-
der stereotype-tinged (Jussim et al. 1996; Meece et al. 2009; Rubie-Davies 2015).
Among them, the most important were the teacher’s expectancies concerning female
students’ achievement in literacy and parents’ expectancies similar in terms of the
content (reasoning: since teacher and parents systematically communicate to me that
I am good at literacy, then for my own sake I should select a H/SS major instead of a
T/S one). Contrary to the indicated forces were the teacher’s expectancies regarding
female students’ outcomes in mathematics (reasoning: since the teacher claims I am
good at mathematics, I will select a T/S major).
In addition to the indicated factors, the following were important among
females: their self-expectancies concerning future results in mathematics and
literacy. However, their impact was paradoxical and difficult to explain (bizarre
reasoning: I predict that my result in the matriculation test in mathematics will
be high, which tells me that I should select a H/SS major, and I think that I will
easily pass the matriculation test in literacy, so I am going to select a T/S major).
In contrast, among male students the subjective factors were: self-concept of
mathematical and literacy abilities. These factors, developed on the basis of per-
sonal experiences when learning specific contents, seem to be concrete, and thus,
more accurate predictors of educational careers than female students’ self-expec-
tancies concerning unspecified possibilities to deal with learning mathematics or
literacy in the future (Trusz 2018; Jussim 1986; Wigfield and Eccles 2000) (rea-
soning: I am more talented at mathematics than literacy, so I give up on a H/SS
major and select a T/S one).
Finally, considering the common predictors for the selection of study majors with
the same signs in the groups of females and males, the first one, i.e., the teacher’s
expectancies on students’ achievement in literacy, was perceived by participants as a
signal confirming their predisposition to study H/SS. In turn, it had a positive impact
on the selection of H/SS majors (reasoning: since the teacher communicates to me
that I am gifted at literacy, it is worthwhile studying H/SS). However, their influ-
ence, according to the cultural gender-stereotype transmission model (Legewie and
DiPrete 2014; Ridgeway 2011; Wang and Degot 2017; Whitley 1997) was stronger
among females.
The second factor, representing the effort put into learning mathematics, was
more reasonably discounted by males. After obtaining a satisfactory result in the
matriculation test in this subject, they studied a similar (or related) major, being less

13
Why do females choose to study humanities or social sciences,… 631

prone—compared to females—to radically changing their interests (reasoning: I


obtained a high result in the matriculation test in mathematics, so it would be more
sensible to select a T/S than a H/SS major).
To the best knowledge of the author, so far surprisingly few studies have been
conducted on the relationship between the expectancies of significant others, self-
regulated learning by students’ personality traits, and females’ and males’ prefer-
ences for H/SS or T/S majors. Existing works (e.g., Alon and DiPrete 2017; Byrne
et al. 2012; Emerson et al. 2012; Walkey et al. 2013) confirm that parents’ and
teachers’ expectancies as well as students’ self-concepts, their self-expectancies, and
motivation to learn are critical for educational careers of secondary school gradu-
ates, including the choice of satisfactory study majors and successful completion of
the programs.
Emerson et al. (2012) point out “outside of the school, parents can contribute to
their children’s educational outcomes by creating a rich learning environment. Par-
ents can discuss options for post-secondary education, stress the value of education
in general, provide learning resources (…), help their children maintain positive atti-
tudes towards their own abilities and support them through problems at school” (p.
46).
Byrne et al. (2012) add “students’ expectations of higher education often influ-
ence their choice of academic discipline as they typically seek to align their degree
program with their perceived abilities, interests and personality (…) students’ expec-
tations can also impact on the learning process and their success and satisfaction
within higher education” (p. 136).
Alon and DiPrete (2017) assessed gender-related selection pressure in the majors
preferred by females and males. Based on data of 27,000 students the authors
revealed that the egalitarian orientations of young females and males in selecting
STEM majors were moderated by cultural beliefs about gender identity and differ-
ences in competencies, part of which come from the family and school environment
in the form of stereotype-tinged expectancies. The indicated factors “curb the aspira-
tions and career choices of women with the highest levels of academic competence
and quantitative skills and, over time, can outweigh the effect of egalitarian forces”
(p. 67).
Other studies quantified the link between the discussed factors and the students’
outcomes, which in turn, are an important predictor of educational careers. Jussim
and his colleagues found that the power of relationship between teachers’ expec-
tancies and students’ marks in mathematics ranged from β = .10 to .49 (Smith et al.
1999), while for mathematics test results it ranged from β = .09 (Smith et al. 1999)
to .21 (Jussim and Eccles 1992). Other authors reported even higher results than val-
ues quantified in meta-analyses (Hattie 2009; Rosenthal 1997), e.g., β = .40 (Gill and
Reynolds 1999) and .54 (Trusz 2018).
Parents’ expectancies have a similar function. Tomasetto et al. (2011) found lower
mathematics achievement among female students aged 5–7 years old, if their moth-
ers endorsed negative gender stereotypes about mathematics. Eccles et al. (1990)
analyzed the relationship between parents’ expectancies concerning their chil-
dren’s mathematics and sport competences and their cognitive and physical activity.

13
632 S. Trusz

Children’s competences in these areas have been influenced by parents’ stereotype


colored beliefs about which gender is more talented in these areas.
Moreover, Garcia (2014) documented that the relationship between parents’
expectancies and their children’s academic achievement is modified by parents’ edu-
cational/socio-economic level. As the indicated moderators increased, the strength
of the link between parents’ expectancies and academic achievements of children
increased (β = .005; p ns and .116; p < .05 for parents with less than and at least a
high school diploma, respectively).
Considering students’ intrapersonal expectancies, their self-concepts, and motiva-
tion to learn mathematics versus literacy, research results (Buckley 2016; Cunning-
ham et al. 2015) have shown that compared to males, lower percentages of female
secondary school graduates reported that they liked mathematics or science and
that mathematics or science was their favorite subjects. Moreover, higher percent-
ages of males than females took advanced courses in science or formal sciences.
Consequently, males had higher average results in the mathematics and science part
of National Assessment of Educational Progress than females (Cunningham et al.
2015).
As the age of students increases, the gender gap referring to self-efficacy and the
assessment of the importance of STEM majors exacerbates (Meece et al. 2009), and
therefore secondary school female graduates with high outcomes in mathematics are
less likely to opt for a STEM university program than male graduates (Hongo 2013).
On the other hand, male graduates are more likely to select a STEM program, even
in lower-mark categories. Moreover, female college students had negative beliefs
about mathematics if the college environment maintained or reinforced gender ste-
reotypes about this subject (Good et al. 2008). Finally, female students reported
significantly higher levels of mathematics anxiety than male students, which may
encourage them to select H/SS rather than T/S majors (Buckley 2016).
In summary, the obtained results are generally consistent with those reported by
other authors and show that parents and teachers tend to perceive females to be less
competent than males in mathematics-oriented majors that are perceived as mas-
culine, and to assess as less important for female students’ future educational paths
than male students. These gender stereotype-tinged communications could be inter-
nalized by students and shape gender identities, that in turn may contribute to gen-
der differences in academic interests of females and males. Furthermore, the indi-
cated relations can be modified by teachers’ gender.
In the current study, the number of significant interactions of the teacher’s gen-
der factor with predictors included in the models was higher among males (9 out
of 12) than females (5 out of 12). Apparently, the educational choices of the former
were more sensitive to teachers’ gender than in the case of the latter. Further, among
female students, the gender of the mathematics teacher was not a significant modera-
tor in any of the tested settings, as opposed to the gender of literacy teachers (five
significant interactions).
In the case of male students’ relations with teachers of a different gender, and tak-
ing into account the high values of specific predictors, the probability of selecting
T/S majors was higher than for relations with teachers of the same gender. Among
them were (1) the teacher’s and (2) parents’ expectancies concerning male students/

13
Why do females choose to study humanities or social sciences,… 633

sons’ achievement in mathematics, (3) students’ self-expectancies, (4) self-concept,


and (5) the result in the matriculation test in the subject. In these settings, female
teachers were more involved in the transmission of gender stereotypes as compared
to male teachers.
Similarly, in the case of female students’ relations with teachers of the oppo-
site gender, and taking into account the high values of specific factors, the odds of
selecting H/SS majors were higher than for relations with teachers of the same gen-
der. Among them were (1) the teacher’s and (2) parents’ expectancies concerning
female students/daughters achievement in literacy, (3) their self-expectancies, (4)
self-concept, and (5) time spent on learning the subject. Again, in the indicated cir-
cumstances, female teachers were more involved in the transmission of gender ste-
reotypes as compared to male teachers.
Finally, considering male students’ relations with female teachers, and taking
into account the high values of specific predictors, the probability of selecting H/SS
majors was higher than for male teachers. Among them were (1) the teacher’s and (2)
parents’ expectancies concerning male students’ achievement in literacy, (3) students’
self-concept, and (4) time spent on learning literacy. In this case, female teachers
engaged more in the transmission of beliefs incompatible with the gender stereotype.
While discussing the obtained findings it should be stressed that the results of
other studies on the effects of teacher-student gender interaction on students’ school
achievement are mixed and sometimes contradictory. Many of them (e.g., Burusic
et al. 2012; Driessen 2007; Helbig 2012; Watson et al. 2017) reported no effects of
the teacher’s gender on students’ academic outcomes. For example, Driessen (2007),
using data from over 5000 elementary school students and their teachers, found a
non-significant effect of teacher gender on the achievement, attitude, and behavior
of girls and boys. Helbig (2012), based on the PIRLS data with a sample of 146,315
elementary school students from 21 countries, found that boys did not benefit from
having male teachers, and only in two countries, i.e., in Austria and Romania, girls
performed better if their teachers were female.
However, other studies show that the teacher’s gender interacting with the stu-
dents’ gender has significant impact on their achievement (Good and Brophy 2008;
Watson et al. 2017). Li (1999) reported that male teachers taught mathematics more
effectively than female teachers, and female students attain higher scores in math-
ematics when they are taught by female than male teachers.
Referring to studies in which quantitative and qualitative differences in teachers’
interactions with male and female students were identified, Good and Brophy (2008)
note that “words of encouragement or feedback directed to boys tend to focus exclu-
sively on their achievement striving and accomplishments, but some of what is said
to girls in parallel situations focuses instead on neatness, following directions, ask
more thought-provoking questions of, or provide more extensive feedback to boys
in mathematics or science classes but to girls in language arts classes” (p. 266). The
indicated teachers’ behavior can differentiate the variables correlated with students’
outcomes, and in the long run affect the choices of educational paths consistent with
gender stereotypes.
In the discussed studies, analysis covered the impact of teachers’ and students’
gender on students’ achievement rather than on the selection of study majors.

13
634 S. Trusz

However, such works are still missing. Assuming that the indicated factors, i.e., aca-
demic outcomes and the selection of specific study majors, are correlated, it seems
that the revealed regularities at least partially explain the results obtained in the cur-
rent study.

6 Limitations, conclusions, and recommendations

The variables in the study were controlled in a retrospective, questionnaire measure-


ment. Therefore, the considered relations did not have to be causal as was assumed.
This limitation is pointed out by many investigators conducting surveys in the social
sciences (Darlington and Hayes 2016).
The indicated limitation leads to another one—namely, time spent on learning
mathematics and literacy was estimated based on participants’ declarations, and a
few months after taking the matriculation test in both subjects. Certainly, data would
have been more reliable if time spent on learning was measured daily by students
over the eight-month period of the final grade in secondary school. However, even
in such methodologically inflated conditions, students could overestimate/underesti-
mate the assessment of learning time, e.g., to protect their self-esteem by means of
the strategy of presenting oneself in a positive light (Rhodewalt 2008).
Third, the study did not involve any control of confounding variables that could
be important for the direction or effects size of the predictors of the study majors
selected by students. As Garcia (2014) points out, this kind of variable can be
the level of parents’ education (or more general—the cultural capital of the fam-
ily environment) or personal/cognitive determinants of individual susceptibility
to induce incorrect interpersonal expectancies (Cooper and Hazelrigg 1988), and
consequently, the formation of the educational self-fulfilling prophecies (Trusz
and Bąbel 2016; Rubie-Davies 2015).
Fourth, the paradoxical and difficult to explain results obtained for females’
self-expectancies indicate that they could be incidental and reflect an uncon-
trolled bias of non-probability sampling. Assuming this is the case, the remaining
results should be interpreted with great caution since they may be an artifact of
the sampling method.
Fifth, it is difficult to discuss the results by reference to findings of other authors,
because there is no research in which: the selections of study majors made by
females and males are explained based on the influence of objective factors (time
spent on learning mathematics and literacy, and the results of the matriculation tests
in both subjects) and subjective ones (the parents’ and teachers’ expectancies con-
cerning their children’s/students’ achievement in mathematics and literacy, students’
self-expectancies, and their self-concept of mathematical and literacy abilities),
considering the potentially moderating impact of mathematics and literacy teacher
gender. Thus, the obtained results may reflect universal regularities occurring in the
population of K − 12 students or result from the applied method of data collection,
analysis, and interpretation.

13
Why do females choose to study humanities or social sciences,… 635

Therefore, it cannot be excluded that the source of differences in the selection


of study majors among females and males were not stereotype-tinged factors, as
assumed, but rather biological/evolutionary determinants. Indeed, in the impressive
experiment with rhesus monkeys as participants, Hassett et al. (2008) demonstrated
that the sex of monkeys significantly diversified preferences concerning their selec-
tions of “typically male” toys. The vast majority of males preferred contact with
wheeled vehicles, and a minority chose plush toys. However, the reversed result
pattern was not observed in female monkeys. Perhaps, as a result of biologically
defined preferences of exploratory activities, human children shape the family and
school environment in a way that in turn offers stimuli encouraging females and
male students to select H/SS and T/S majors, respectively.
Most likely, the truth about the sources of educational preferences of females and
males is somewhere in the middle. Nevertheless, these choices have serious conse-
quences, as study major selection (e.g., engineering vs. anthropology) translates into
eventual outcomes on the market in terms of employment, job match, and satisfac-
tory earnings.
Finally, biologically or culturally conditioned transmission of the gender stere-
otype can be effectively overcome. This is proven by the biographies of eminent
people. Michael Faraday, for example, who came from a lower social class, still
managed to begin his studies in physics and then conducted intensive research on
electricity and magnetism, culminating in the discovery of electromagnetic induc-
tion. Similarly, Maria Skłodowska-Curie, despite the active opposition of the
authorities of her home university being reluctant to the academic development of
women, conducted successful research on radioactivity, which culminated in her
being awarded two (sic!) Nobel Prizes, i.e., in chemistry and physics.

Funding This work was supported by the National Science Centre in Poland (Grant No. 2012/05/D/
HS6/03350).

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest The author declare that there is no conflict of interest.

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License,
which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as
you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Com-
mons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article
are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the
material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is
not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission
directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creat​iveco​mmons​.org/licen​
ses/by/4.0/.

References
Alon, S., & DiPrete, T. A. (2017). Gender differences in the formation of a field of study choice set.
Sociological Science, 2, 50–81.

13
636 S. Trusz

Aronson, J., & McGlone, M. S. (2009). Stereotype and social identity threat. In T. Nelson (Ed.), Hand-
book of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination (pp. 153–178). New York: Psychology Press.
Babad, E. (2009). The social psychology of the classroom. New York: Routledge.
Bernard, M. E., Keefauver, L. W., Elsworth, G., & Naylor, F. D. (1981). Sex-role behavior and gender in
teacher-student evaluations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 681–696.
Brophy J. E., & Good, T. L. (1974). Teacher-student relationships: Causes and consequences. New York:
Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Buckley, S. (2016). Gender and sex differences in student participation, achievement and engagement in
mathematics. http://resea​rch.acer.edu.au/learn​ing_proce​sses/18. Accessed August 29, 2018.
Burusic, J., Babarovic, T., & Seric, M. (2012). Differences in elementary school achievement between
girls and boys: Does the teacher gender play a role?”. European Journal of Psychology of Educa-
tion, 27, 523–538.
Byrne, M., Flood, B., Hassall, T., Joyce, J., Montaño, J. L., González, J. M., et al. (2012). Motivations,
expectations and preparedness for higher education: A study of accounting students in Ireland, the
UK, Spain and Greece. Accounting Forum, 36, 134–144.
Catalyst. (2018). Women in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM). http://www.
catal​yst.org/knowl​edge/women​-scien​ce-techn​ology​-engin​eerin​g-and-mathe​matic​s-stem#footn​ote34​
_8j0ub​dw. Accessed August 29, 2018.
Cooper, H., & Hazelrigg, P. (1988). Personality moderators of interpersonal expectancy effects: An inte-
grative research review. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 937–949.
Cunningham, B. C., Hoyer, K. M., Sparks, D., & Ralph, J. (2015). Gender differences in science, tech-
nology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) interest, credits earned, and NAEP performance in
the 12th Grade. IES National Center for Education Sciences. https​://nces.ed.gov/pubs2​015/20150​
75.pdf. Accessed August 29, 2018.
Cvencek, D., Meltzoff, A. N., & Greenwald, A. G. (2011). Math-gender stereotypes in elementary school
children. Child Development, 82, 766–779.
Darley, J. M., & Fazio, R. H. (1980). Expectancy confirmation processes arising in the social interaction
sequence. American Psychologist, 35, 867–881.
Darlington, R. B., & Hayes, A. F. (2016). Regression analysis and linear models: Concepts, application
and implementation. New York: The Guilford Press.
de Boer, H., Bosker, R. J., & van der Werf, M. P. C. (2010). Sustainability of teacher expectation bias
effects on long-term student performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 168–179.
Debacker, T. K., & Nelson, R. M. (2000). Motivation to learn science: Differences related to gender, class
type, and ability. Journal of Educational Research, 93, 245–254.
Driessen, G. (2007). The feminization of primary education: Effects of teachers’ sex on pupil achieve-
ment, attitudes and behaviour. Review of Education, 53, 183–203.
Eagly, A. H. (2009). The his and hers of prosocial behavior: An examination of the social psychology of
gender. American Psychologist, 64, 644–658.
Eagly, A. H., Johannesen-Schmidt, M. C., & Van Engen, M. L. (2003). Transformational, transactional,
and laissez-faire leadership styles: A meta-analysis comparing women and men. Psychological Bul-
letin, 129, 569–591.
Eccles, J., Jacobs, J. E., & Harold, R. D. (1990). Gender role stereotypes, expectancy effects, and parents’
socialization of gender differences. Journal of Social Issues, 46, 183–201.
Else-Quest, N. M., Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (2010). Cross-national patterns of gender differences in
mathematics: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 103–127.
Emerson, L., Fear, J., Fox, S., & Sanders, E. (2012). Parental engagement in learning and schooling. https​
://www.aracy​.org.au/publi​catio​ns-resou​rces/comma​nd/downl​oad_file/id/7/filen​ame/Paren​tal_engag​
ement​_in_learn​ing_and_schoo​ling_Lesso​ns_from_resea​rch_BUREA​U_ARACY​_Augus​t_2012.
pdf. Accessed August 29, 2018.
Eurostat. (2017). Tertiary education statistics. http://ec.europ​a.eu/euros​tat/stati​stics​-expla​ined/index​.php/
Terti​ary_educa​tion_stati​stics​. Accessed August 29, 2018.
Fan, W. (2011). Social influences, school motivation and gender differences: An application of the expec-
tancy-value theory. Educational Psychology, 31, 157–175.
Fiedler, K., Walther, E., Freytag, P., & Plessner, H. (2002). Judgment biases in a simulated classroom—A
cognitive-environmental approach. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 88,
527–561.
Garcia, J. N. (2014). Teacher and parent beliefs and expectations of parental involvement and how it
relates to student academic achievement. PhD diss.: Texas State University.

13
Why do females choose to study humanities or social sciences,… 637

Gentrup, S., & Rjosk, C. (2018). Pygmalion and the gender gap: Do teacher expectations contribute
to differences in achievement between boys and girls at the beginning of schooling? Educational
Research and Evaluation, 24, 295–323.
Gill, S., & Reynolds, A. J. (1999). Educational expectations and school achievement of urban African
American children. Journal of School Psychology, 37, 403–424.
Good, C., Aronson, J., & Harder, J. A. (2008). Problems in the pipeline: Stereotype threat and women’s
achievement in high-level math courses. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 29, 17–28.
Good, T. L., & Brophy, J. E. (2008). Looking in classrooms (10th ed.). New York: Pearson.
Hall, J. A., & Briton, N. J. (1993). Gender, nonverbal behavior, and expectations. In P. D. Blanck (Ed.),
Interpersonal expectations: Theory, research, and applications (pp. 276–295). New York: Cam-
bridge University Press.
Harris, M. J., & Rosenthal, R. (1985). Mediation of interpersonal expectancy effects. 31 meta-analyses.
Psychological Bulletin, 97, 363–386.
Hassett, J. M., Siebert, E. R., & Wallen, K. (2008). Sex differences in rhesus monkey toy preferences par-
allel those of children. Hormones and Behavior, 54, 359–364.
Hattie, J. (2009). Visible learning: A synthesis of over 800 meta-analyses relating to achievement. New
York: Routledge.
Helbig, M. (2012). Boys do not benefit from male teachers in their reading and mathematics skills:
Empirical evidence from 21 European Union and OECD countries. British Journal of Sociology of
Education, 33, 661–677.
Hongo, D. (2013). Gender differences in science, technology, engineering, mathematics and computer
science (STEM) programs at university. Statistics Canada. https​://www15​0.statc​an.gc.ca/n1/
pub/75-006-x/20130​01/artic​le/11874​-eng.htm. Accessed August 29, 2018.
Huang, C. (2013). Gender differences in academic self-efficacy: A meta-analysis. European Journal of
Psychology of Education, 28, 1–35.
Hyde, J. S. (1990). Meta-analysis and the psychology of gender differences. Signs: Journal of Women in
Culture and Society, 16, 55–73.
Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychologist, 60, 581–592.
Hyde, J. S. (2014). Gender similarities and differences. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 373–398.
Hyde, J. S., Fennema, E., & Lamon, S. J. (1990). Gender differences in mathematics performance: A
meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 139–155.
Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (1988). Gender differences in verbal ability: A meta-analysis. Psychological
Bulletin, 104, 53–69.
Jacobs, J. E., Davis-Kean, P., Bleeker, M., Eccles, J., & Malanchuk, O. (2004). “I can, but I don’t want
to” The impact of parents, interests, and activities on gender differences in math. In A. M. Gallagher
& J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), Gender differences in mathematics: An integrative psychological approach
(pp. 246–263). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Jussim, L. (1986). Self-fulfilling prophecies. A theoretical and integrative review. Psychological Review,
93, 429–445.
Jussim, L., & Eccles, J. (1992). Teacher expectations II: Construction and reflection of student achieve-
ment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 947–961.
Jussim, L., & Eccles, J. (1995). Naturally occurring interpresonal expectancies. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.),
Social development: Review of personality and social psychology (pp. 74–108). Thousand Oaks,
CA: Sage.
Jussim, L., Eccles, J., & Madon, S. (1996). Social perception, social stereotypes, and teacher expecta-
tions: Accuracy and the quest for the powerful self-fulfilling prophecy. Advances in Experimental
Social Psychology, 28(C), 281–388.
Kite, M. E., & Whitley, B. E. (2016). Psychology of prejudice and discrimination (3rd ed.). New York:
Routledge.
Kling, K. C., Hyde, J. S., Showers, C. J., & Buswell, B. N. (1999). Gender differences in self-esteem: A
meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125(4), 470–500.
Legewie, J., & DiPrete, T. A. (2014). The high school environment and the gender gap in science and
engineering. Sociology of Education, 87, 259–280.
Li, Q. (1999). Teachers’ beliefs and gender differences in mathematics: A review. Educational Research,
41, 63–76.
Lindberg, S. M., Hyde, J. S., Petersen, J. L., & Linn, M. C. (2010). New trends in gender and mathemat-
ics performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 1123–1135.

13
638 S. Trusz

Machin, S., & Pekkarinen, T. (2008). Assessment: Global sex differences in test score variability. Sci-
ence, 322, 1331–1332.
McKown, C., & Weinstein, R. S. (2002). Modeling the role of child ethnicity and gender in children’s
differential response to teacher expectations. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32, 159–184.
Meece, J. L., Glienke, B. B., & Askew, K. (2009). Gender and motivation. In K. R. Wentzel & A. Wig-
field (Eds.), Handbook on motivation at school (pp. 411–432). New York: Routledge.
Nguyen, H. H., & Ryan, A. M. (2008). Does stereotype threat affect test performance of minorities and
women? A meta-analysis of experimental evidence. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 1314–1334.
Pajares, F., & Miller, M. D. (1994). Role of self-efficacy and self-concept beliefs in mathematical prob-
lem solving: A path analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 193–203.
Plante, I., O’Keefe, P. A., Aronson, J., Fréchette-Simard, C., & Goulet, M. (2019). The interest gap: How
gender stereotype endorsement about abilities predicts differences in academic interests. Social Psy-
chology of Education, 22, 227–245.
Reilly, D. (2012). Gender, culture, and sex-typed cognitive abilities. PLoS ONE. https​://doi.org/10.1371/
journ​al.pone.00399​04.
Rhodewalt, F. (2008). Self-handicapping: On the self-perpetuating nature of defensive behavior. Social
and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 1255–1268.
Ridgeway, C. L. (2011). Framed by gender: How gender inequality persists in the modern world. Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Robnett, R. D. (2016). Gender bias in stem fields: Variation in prevalence and links to stem self-concept.
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 40, 65–79.
Rosenthal, R. (1997). Interpersonal expectancy effects: A forty-year perspective (p. 16). Chicago, August:
Paper presented at The American Psychological Association convention.
Rubie-Davies, C. M. (2006). Teacher expectations and student self-perceptions: Exploring relationships.
Psychology in the Schools, 43, 537–552.
Rubie-Davies, C. M. (2015). Becoming a high expectation teacher: Raising the bar. New York:
Routledge.
Rudman, L. A., & Phelan, J. E. (2010). The effect of priming gender roles on women’s implicit gender
beliefs and career aspirations. Social Psychology, 41(3), 192–202.
Schmader, T. (2002). Gender identification moderates stereotype threat effects on women’s math perfor-
mance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 194–201.
Shapiro, J. R., & Williams, A. M. (2012). The role of stereotype threats in undermining girls’ and wom-
en’s performance and interest in stem fields. Sex Roles, 66, 175–183.
Simpkins, S. D., Davis-Kean, P. E., & Eccles, J. (2006). Math and science motivation: A longitudinal
examination of the links between choices and beliefs. Developmental Psychology, 42, 70–83.
Simpkins, S. D., Estrella, G., Gaskin, E., & Kloberdanz, E. (2018). Latino parents’ science beliefs and
support of high school students’ motivational beliefs: Do the relations vary across gender and
familism values? Social Psychology of Education, 21, 1203–1224.
Smith, A. E., Jussim, L., & Eccles, J. (1999). Do self-fulfilling prophecies accumulate, dissipate, or
remain stable over time? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 548–565.
Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M., & Quinn, D. M. (1999). Stereotype threat and women’ s math performance.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 4–28.
Stangor, C. (2009). The study of stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination within social psychology:
A quick history of theory and research. In T. D. Nelson (Ed.), Handbook of prejudice, stereotyping,
and discrimination (pp. 1–22). New York: Psychology Press.
Starr, C. R., & Leaper, C. (2019). Do adolescents’ self-concepts moderate the relationship between
STEM stereotypes and motivation? Social Psychology of Education, 22, 1109–1129.
Sullivan, A. (2009). Academic self-concept, gender and single-sex schooling. British Educational
Research Journal, 35, 259–288.
Szumski, G., & Karwowski, M. (2019). Exploring the Pygmalion effect: The role of teacher expectations,
academic self-concept, and class context in students’ math achievement. Contemporary Educational
Psychology. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedps​ych.2019.10178​7.
Tiedemann, J. (2000). Parents’ gender stereotypes and teachers’ beliefs as predictors of children’s con-
cept of their mathematical ability in elementary school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92,
144–151.
Tomasetto, C., Alparone, F. R., & Cadinu, M. (2011). Girls’ math performance under stereotype threat:
The moderating role of mothers’ gender stereotypes. Developmental Psychology, 47, 943–949.

13
Why do females choose to study humanities or social sciences,… 639

Tosto, M. G., Asbury, K., Mazzocco, M., Petrill, S. A., & Kovas, Y. (2016). From classroom environ-
ment to mathematics achievement: The mediating role of self-perceived ability and subject interest.
Learning and Individual Differences, 50, 260–269.
Trusz, S. (2018). Four mediation models of teacher expectancy effects on students’ outcomes in math-
ematics and literacy. Social Psychology of Education, 21, 257–287.
Trusz, S., & Bąbel, P. (Eds.). (2016). Intrapersonal and interpersonal expectancies. New York:
Routledge.
Voyer, D. (2011). Time limits and gender differences on paper-and-pencil tests of mental rotation: A
meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 267–277.
Voyer, D., & Voyer, S. D. (2014). Gender differences in scholastic achievement: A meta-analysis. Psycho-
logical Bulletin, 140, 1174–1204.
Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A meta-
analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 250–270.
Walkey, F. H., McClure, J., Meyer, L. H., & Weir, K. F. (2013). Low expectations equal no expectations:
Aspirations, motivation, and achievement in secondary school. Contemporary Educational Psychol-
ogy, 38, 306–315.
Wang, M.-T., & Degot, J. L. (2017). Gender gap in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics
(STEM): Current knowledge, implications for practice, policy, and future directions. Educational
Psychology Review, 29, 119–140.
Watson, P. W., Rubie-Davies, C. M., Meissel, K., Peterson, E. R., Flint, A., Garrett, L., et al. (2017).
Teacher gender, and expectation of reading achievement in New Zealand elementary school stu-
dents: Essentially a barrier? Gender and Education. https​://doi.org/10.1080/09540​253.2017.14101​
08.
Weathington, B. L., Cunningham, C. J., & Pittenger, D. J. (2010). Research methods for the behavioral
and social sciences. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Wentzel, K. R., & Wigfield, A. (Eds.). (2009). Handbook on motivation at school. New York: Routledge.
Whitley, B. E. (1997). Gender differences in computer-related attitudes and behavior: A meta-analysis.
Computers in Human Behavior, 13, 1–22.
Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. (2000). Expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation. Contemporary
Educational Psychology, 25, 68–81.

Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published
maps and institutional affiliations.

Sławomir Trusz Ph.D., is a Professor of Pedagogy and Head of Educational Microprocesses Laboratory
at the Pedagogical University of Kraków, Poland. His leading areas of interest are the effects of intra-
and interpersonal expectancies and educational self-fulfilling prophecy mechanism. His recent studies
focused on stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination in relation to children with oncological disease.
He co-authored the book entitled Intrapersonal and interpersonal expectancies, Routledge, 2016 (with
Przemysław Bąbel, Jagiellonian University in Poland).

13

You might also like