Senior Thesis - Leo Borg
Senior Thesis - Leo Borg
Senior Thesis - Leo Borg
Leo Borg
University of California, Berkeley
December 2023
Advisors:
Dr. Amm Quamruzzaman
Professor Oliver O’Reilly
Table of Contents
2 — Abstract
3 — Introduction
4 — Review of Literature
8 — Theoretical Framework
9 — Educational and Pedagogical Theory
11 — Cognitive and Mathematical Theory
12 — Liberatory Theory
14 — Overview
15 — Data and Methods
24 — Results
33 — Discussion and Conclusion
40 — Appendix
41 — References
1
Abstract
There is a large gap in research that specifically investigates how the cognitive and
intellectual content of curriculum may relate to equity. Such an investigation, conducted in this
study, is crucial to grasp the full impact of pedagogical materials upon equity in learning
determine if there is a correlation between equity and the depth of learning made available to
students. This study focuses on two classrooms from different schools in the Bay Area that serve
analysis of pedagogy and curriculum, along with interviews with teachers. The body of research
that pertains to equity in education would predict that the school that is located in an affluent area
and has a less diverse and more socioeconomically privileged student body is likely to have
superior curriculum and pedagogy. However, in this case, the converse holds. Critical content
analysis reveals a significant difference in mathematical integrity between the textbooks. These
findings also imply the amount of time spent in group work scenarios is not necessarily
indicative of the depth of learning that students experience. Moreover, whether a problem
indicator of the actual depth of knowledge it requires of a student. Finally, both teachers
expressed a desire to improve their familiarity with and expertise in equity-based pedagogy, but
indicated that time and money were often the primary limiting factors that prevented them from
doing so. The experiences of these teachers suggest that paid professional development
opportunities for teachers could be promising for the proliferation of equity-based pedagogy.
2
Introduction
Over the last few decades, educators and educational researchers have sought a clear
understanding of the ways that large-scale societal dynamics are reflected within and reproduced
by the education system. In particular, a major theme of educational research in the U.S. has
been the study of inequity, both from a systemic and an interpersonal viewpoint. This has
resulted in a wealth of research that evidences deep structural connections between social
inequity, educational institutions, and pedagogical strategies (Hammond 2015; Rogers et al.
research that addresses the constraints that teachers face and the obstacles they encounter with
regard to implementing equity-based pedagogy. This presents a problem because any attempt to
understanding of the practical limitations that teachers experience. With this knowledge,
developers of pedagogy can design more effective interventions that are logistically feasible for
educators.
Moreover, research that seeks to understand the relationship between pedagogy and
equity often fixates primarily on teaching techniques at the cost of examining curricular
materials. While interpersonal dynamics and pedagogical interventions are both fundamental
determinants of equity in the classroom, they alone do not comprise a complete image of a
student’s educational experience. There is a large gap in research that specifically investigates
how the cognitive and intellectual content of curriculum may relate to equity. This investigation,
which I will conduct in this study, is crucial to grasp the full impact of pedagogical materials
3
curricular materials, I seek to determine if there is a correlation between equity- both in the
relational sense and the socioeconomic sense- and the depth of learning made available to
students.
Review of Literature
There is a large body of research that analyzes the U.S. education system as a social
institution. In this academic tradition, it has long been known that the resources available to
students in educational settings are directly correlated with their socioeconomic status. Some
studies focus on this correlation with respect to the variety of pedagogy students experience,
while others focus the academic rigor of instruction. Still others investigate facets of the
One seminal work in this area, by Jean Anyon, draws comparisons between schools
according to the demographics of their students (Anyon 1980). In this work, she demonstrates
that student demographics are directly correlated not only with the educational opportunities they
receive but also with the quality and philosophical orientation of the pedagogy they are exposed
to. As a result, upper-class students are more likely to be supported to develop a variety of skills,
while lower class students are more likely to be restricted to rote learning environments. Other
studies have illuminated a persistent pattern known as learning time loss, in which high-poverty
schools tend to experience “absences, delays, disruptions, and interruptions that reduce
instructional time or divert time away from instructional purposes”at higher rates than
low-poverty schools. In particular, a study by John Rogers, et al. (Rogers et al. 2014) compared
available learning time across a breadth of California public high schools. This investigation
found that teachers, students, and administrators in high-poverty schools face a large array of
4
obstacles, including issues with school facilities, increased teacher obligations, and external life
stressors, that their low-poverty counterparts experience at much lower rates. This suggests that,
in addition to lacking tangible physical resources, students who attend high-poverty schools are
afforded significantly less instructional time over the course of their academic career than
socioeconomic lines is present within individual schools. In their influential case study of
Berkeley High, Beth Rubin et al. investigate the common practice of tracking, by which students
are funneled into different academic pathways (Rubin et al. 2006). These pathways are organized
by rigor, ranging from remedial to advanced, and are ostensibly designed to sort students by skill
level. However, as Rubin et al. argue, this practice has the effect of reifying inequity because
students who are already socioeconomically disadvantaged are more likely to be placed in a
lower track than their privileged peers. Moreover, there is a disparity in the quality of instruction
between higher and lower tracks. Rubin et al. demonstrate that this practice introduces additional
logistical and bureaucratic obstacles for students who are placed in a lower track but seek
upward academic mobility. Consequently, the ultimate effect is a deliberate and institutionally
sanctioned type of segregation in which claims of meritocracy eclipse the material needs of
students.
education system in which policy decisions intersect with pedagogy to dictate the dynamics of
the classroom. In this regard, there is a strong body of research that evidences deep relationships
between pedagogy and equitable learning outcomes. One illustrative example is a study
conducted by Jo Boaler which found that a heterogeneous classroom (in other words, a
5
classroom composed of students of varying abilities) provided not only positive outcomes in
terms of students’ academic achievement, but also created a classroom culture of mutual respect
and equity (Boaler 2006). This work in particular is important because it contradicts the
assumption that students learn best in similar-ability groups- the very assumption that undergirds
educators might cultivate relational equity in their classrooms without sacrificing academic
project-based learning, are also well-known to educators and educational researchers within the
realm of equity-based pedagogy. These strategies are explained in more detail in the Theoretical
Framework.
Much of this research is grounded in a rich tradition of educational and social theory,
which encompasses a variety of equity-based pedagogies. Many of these pedagogies have been
designed to address a specific marginalized position within society, such as class, or were
developed using existing frameworks in the social sciences, such as feminist theory. For
example, class-sensitive pedagogy, put forth by Stephanie Jones and Mark Vagle, is designed to
combat bias on the basis of social class by intentionally disrupting classist assumptions built into
the discourse and structure of the classroom (Jones and Vagle 2013). Red pedagogy, developed
by Sandy Grande, draws from both Indigenous epistemologies and critical pedagogy to
formulate an explicitly decolonial pedagogy (Grande 2014). The most foundational of these
pedagogies is Paulo Friere’s theory of critical pedagogy (Freire 2018), which I elucidate in my
inequality is manifested and perpetuated within the school system: institutional circumstances
6
(including internal administrative decisions and external social factors) and classroom
interactions. However, the relationship between the nature of written curricular materials and
Many studies are quick to note that lower-income schools typically have older textbooks
than higher-income schools, but this is generally mentioned as an aside, treated as self-evident or
assumed to be common knowledge. However, surely the year in which a textbook was published
cannot be the best metric for its quality. This is especially true in light of the recent widespread
Common Core State Standards (CCSS) reform. The curriculum was developed through a
deliberate research process and yet, a decade into its implementation, it has become clear that
something has gone fundamentally awry (for a discussion, see Wu 2020). The content of the
Common Core Standards represents, on the whole, a considerable departure from previously
adopted state standards (Porter et al. 2011), and at the time of their adoption in the early 2010’s,
many researchers were optimistic about the potential of CCSS to dramatically improve the
quality of mathematics education in the U.S. (Schmidt & Houang 2012). However, the standards
were implemented to varying degrees of success. This prompted a flurry of debate and
speculation in the popular media about the efficacy of CCSS, but there has been a noticeable lack
Finally, there exists a small but significant body of literature on the methodology of
educational research. Because of the large size of the U.S. education system and the copious
social dynamics at play within it, it is extremely difficult to obtain a data sample that is
sufficiently large and random to allow for generalizability, even when limiting the scope of a
study to a certain geographic area. Several key papers provide insights into the possibilities for
educational research in the modern age (Lemke & Sabelli 2008; Jacobson, Kapur, & Reimann.
7
2016; Jacobson, Levin, & Kapur 2019). In particular, these authors argue for viewing the
education system through the lens of a complex systems conceptual framework, in which the
interactions of a large number of moving pieces can be modeled and traced using data analysis
tools. The concept of emergence from complexity theory is particularly relevant to educational
research that seeks to understand patterns and causal relationships. In their analysis, Jacobson et
al. demonstrate that traditional methods of research, either qualitative or quantitative, are
unfortunately not fully equipped to employ the depth of data gathering and processing needed to
make generalizable conclusions about such a complicated system (Jacobson, Levin, & Kapur
2019). As a result of this recent development in the theory, many educational researchers may be
acknowledging the inherent un-generalizability of their findings. This philosophical question was
Theoretical Framework
The theoretical framework used in this study is based on the connections between the
author’s perception of four broadly defined areas: education (as a social institution), pedagogy
(as a practice), social inequity, and mathematics. We use the term “area” rather than “discipline”
because these categories are established in formal academia to varying degrees. However, we
argue that, to pursue the research question, it is necessary to name these areas and view them as
discrete. Despite this, each of these areas correspond to one or more disciplines. For example,
both education as a social institution and social inequity are generally studied through a
sociological or historical lens. Pedagogy is studied within the realm of cognitive science as well
8
Educational and Pedagogical Theory
Firstly, the theoretical framework used in this study conceptualizes education as a social
institution and pedagogy as a practice. This choice deliberately narrows the scope of the study
and enables specific connections to be drawn between the areas of focus . This also enables us to
clearly distinguish between education and pedagogy. These terms are often conflated in
colloquial language and academia alike. In contrast to the social and systemic terms on which
education is often studied, pedagogy consists of concrete teaching strategies and specific
embodied experiences that occur within the structures of the education system. Social inequity,
To establish connections between social inequity and education, we use both Pierre
1990) and Gloria Ladson-Billings’ theory of education debt (Ladson-Billings 2006). Bordieu’s
theory is based on the broader theory of sociocultural reproduction, which is prevalent in the
study of social institutions in general. Sociocultural reproduction theory is concerned with how
social institutions perpetuate and preserve existing social dynamics, with a particular emphasis
on social stratification. Bordieu applies this theory to education as a social institution by focusing
on how the structures of the education system support and uplift upper-class students to the
exclusion of lower-class students (Nash 1990, page 436). According to Bordieu, the education
system does this by valuing the social and cultural characteristics, habits, and worldviews of
students who are already privileged, thus granting them more opportunity to succeed. This
systematic effect results in more negative educational outcomes for students who are already
socially disadvantaged, which, in turn, reproduces their disadvantage by reducing their social
9
mobility. Interestingly, scholar Roy Nash (Nash 1990, page 436) argues that one of Bourdieu's
main critiques of the education system is “its structured refusal to develop a universal pedagogy-
a pedagogy that takes nothing for granted- able to succeed with relatively unprepared working
class pupils”.
“education debt” in response to widespread discourse about “achievement gaps” along racial and
socioeconomic lines (Ladson-Billings 2006). She argues that, while perhaps well-meaning, the
“achievement gap” discourse implies that students of color and lower-class students are
somehow deficient. This discourse is situated firmly in the present and refuses to critically
understand the history of education in the United States. Ladson-Billings’ theory of education
debt, on the other hand, is predicated on this history and reveals the ways that these marginalized
groups have been systematically excluded. The theory of education debt asserts that this
knowledge, sociopolitical power, and economic status. By analyzing education in this way, it
becomes clear that the “achievement gaps” between privileged and marginalized groups are
The theoretical framework used in this study conceptualizes pedagogy as related to, but
fundamentally different from, education. Whereas education can be easily viewed from a
systemic perspective, pedagogy occurs at the individual and small-group scale. It is dependent on
the social dynamics within a classroom, the attitude and aptitude of the teacher, and the specific
context in which instruction occurs. Many scholars of education have demonstrated that this
context cannot be separated from broader social institutions (see, for example, Nasir et al 2013).
10
While this is undeniable, we emphasize that pedagogy is also directly derived from the actual
academic content that is being taught. To demonstrate this, we will use the theory of knowledge
structures (such as those elaborated in Redish 2003) and Hung-Hsi Wu’s theory of Textbook
The theory of knowledge structures has been a mainstay of cognitive and learning science
for decades (for example, Kuhn 1993). This theory is used to understand how the brain stores
specifically apply this theory to math education in a similar manner to Edward Redish (Redish
2003). Redish uses the theory of knowledge structures to explain how students reason in the
knowledge elements”, which are small and potentially isolated pieces of information (Redish
2003, page 24). Depending on the educational context, these patterns may be probed to activate
together if the instructor provides the tools for students to strengthen the association between
their knowledge elements. This theory implies that a primary goal of instruction should be
assisting students in fortifying their knowledge structures. While this theory is abstract, its
applicability for pedagogy lies in its focus on the coherence and consistency of both the
instructive methods and the content that is being taught. This theory serves as an abstract
“Textbook School Mathematics” is a term coined by Hung-Hsi Wu (Wu 2020, page xiv)
to describe the particular characteristics of the modern mathematics curriculum in the United
States. This theory posits that the K-12 mathematics curriculum generally “lacks clarity…
11
mostly asks for rote memorization… neglect[s] the inherent logical structure of mathematics…
and fail[s] to recognize the mathematical purpose behind each topic” (Wu 2020, page xiv). Wu
emphasizes that using such a curriculum results in students severely lacking a coherent
instructors must receive better training, both in the specific content they teach and in pedagogical
strategies. This theory proposes improved pedagogy as a partial remedy to the phenomenon of
Textbook School Mathematics. While Wu does not draw any explicit connections to the broader
mechanisms of society, I believe that his theory fruitfully intersects with the study of social
inequity. In particular, Wu’s theory implies that students whose teachers are more pedagogically
adept have a better chance at surmounting the intellectual incoherence of Textbook School
Mathematics. Studies of educational inequity have demonstrated that wealthier schools tend to
have better-trained teachers who are equipped to employ a wider variety of instruction (an
illustration of this can be found in Anyon (1980)). With this in mind, my theoretical framework
aims to draw connections between the quality of mathematics curriculum, the quality of
Liberatory Theory
Finally, the theoretical framework for this study is informed by theories of liberatory
pedagogy. The most general of these is Paulo Freire’s theory of critical pedagogy (Freire 2018).
Freire argues that a central goal of education for marginalized people should be the development
of “critical consciousness”, which is a clear awareness of their situation and how they may best
take liberatory action (Freire 2018, page 109). This development takes place through
12
“problem-posing education”, which serves as a direct contrast to rote learning (Freire 2018, page
83). This theory allows me to draw more explicit connections between instruction, societal
inequity, and equity within the classroom. In particular, it highlights the mutual exclusivity
between rote schooling and the development of a holistic understanding of the world. In addition,
this theory emphasizes that equity-focused education must prepare marginalized people to act
upon their position in society in order to improve it. Critical pedagogy has been massively
influential to educational research and has served as the foundation for many subsequently
developed equity-based pedagogies. While these pedagogies often take inspiration from Freire’s
critical pedagogy, they tend to be narrower in scope (for instance, Grande 2008). Consequently,
we do not anticipate directly using them in this research, but their existence is notable.
pedagogy which have wider applicability (Cohen et al. 1999; Boutte et al. 2010). Both of these
theories have been central to recent pedagogical approaches to equity in science and
communication is central and multiple abilities are both valued and necessary. This theory posits
that organizing the classroom in this way increases relational equity by encouraging students to
assist each other and engage in positive and productive communication (Cohen et al. 1999). In
develop their cognitive abilities. The theory of Culturally Responsive Teaching asserts that
students are more engaged in learning and have better educational outcomes when pedagogy
deliberately respects and invites their culture into the classroom (Boutte et al. 2010). Importantly,
both of these theories translate into actionable teaching strategies that, with guidance, could be
13
accessible for most K-12 teachers. These theories draw the final connection between pedagogy
14
Data and Methods
In order to answer the research question, we collected several types of data. The first of
these were obtained through classroom observations. We attended a total of eight hours of class,
approximately four from each of two schools. Additionally, for each of the lessons observed, we
collected the corresponding curricular material. This includes the textbook section, any relevant
worksheets, and a lesson plan, if the teacher has made one. We also conducted in-depth
interviews with two teachers, one from each school, whose lessons we observed.
In our outreach to secondary school teachers in the Bay Area, we wrote via email to over
100 secondary school administrators in the East Bay area. This is because most schools do not
post teacher emails on their website, so the administrator emails are often the only external point
of contact with the school. We asked that they forward our request for observations and
affirmative responses from teachers from two schools. This indicates that the majority of
administrators did not forward the survey to the mathematics teachers at their school or that the
majority of teachers who received the message were not willing to participate. With this
limitation, the study gravitated more naturally toward an approach bearing more resemblance to
a case study than a broad study. The benefits and limitations inherent to this approach are
Our analysis of the classroom observations focuses on two variables: the degree of
student participation and the degree to which a variety of learners are supported in the classroom.
standardized dimensions by which to assess them. Firstly, we measure the degree of student
15
(1) teacher lecture and student individual work,
(4) student group work on a complex task (such as a project or creative endeavor).
For each class period, we calculated what percentage of the total instructional time was
spent in each mode of participation. In addition, we gave each class period a total score, which
we will refer to as a pedagogical index, which is simply the weighted average over the total
minutes in the class period. This pedagogical index is intended to reflect the “average” type of
variety of learners by noting the abilities that are required to participate, perform, or benefit fully.
OnceweI have identified the abilities required of the students in each intervention, we also note
when the pedagogy and/or curriculum provides opportunities for students to substitute one ability
for another or to use multiple abilities to different degrees. This concept can be thought of as the
textbooks, worksheets, and lesson plans. In a similar fashion to our pedagogical analysis, we
categorize curricular materials along both a quantitative and qualitative dimension. The
qualitative dimension measures the degree to which curricular materials support a variety of
learners. This is done by taking note of the abilities that are necessary for students to benefit fully
from the curricular materials, and categorize the degree of flexibility of the curriculum by noting
16
We also measure the level of thinking that the materials require of students. For example,
a worksheet might require simple factual recall, an original analysis into a problem, or the
practice of a newly introduced method. Specifically, this study utilizes the metric established by
Anderson et al. in their revision of Bloom’s Taxonomy, entitled A Taxonomy for Teaching,
Learning, and Assessing (2001) This taxonomy consists of three figures. The first of these
explains how to categorize a given task according to its knowledge demand (Anderson et al.
2001, page 29). It is encapsulated in the figure entitled “The Knowledge Dimension” on the
following page. The second figure provides a framework for categorizing a given task according
to its cognitive demand (Anderson et al. 2001, page 31). The final figure combines both of these
dimensions of classification into one table, which provides an overview of The Taxonomy for
17
The Knowledge Dimension
A. FACTUAL KNOWLEDGE – The basic elements students must know to be acquainted with a
discipline or solve problems in it
Ab. Knowledge of specific details and elements Major natural resources, reliable sources of information
Ba. Knowledge of classifications and categories Periods of geological time, forms of business
ownership
Bb. Knowledge of principles and generalizations Pythagorean theorem, law of supply and demand
Bc. Knowledge of theories, models and structures Theory of evolution, structure of Congress
Ca. Knowledge of subject-specific skills and Skills used in painting with watercolors, whole-number
algorithms division algorithm
Cc. Knowledge of criteria for determining when to Criteria used to determine when to apply a procedure
use appropriate procedures using Newton’s second law, criteria used to judge the
feasibility of using a particular method to estimate
business costs
Db. Knowledge about cognitive tasks, including Knowledge of the types of tests particular teachers
appropriate contextual and conditional knowledge administer, knowledge of the cognitive demands of
different tasks
18
The Cognitive Process Dimension
1. REMEMBER 1.1 Recognizing e.g. Recognize the dates of important events in U.S.
Retrieve relevant history
knowledge from
long-term memory 1.2 Recalling e.g. Recall the dates of important events in U.S. history
3. APPLY 3.1 Executing e.g. Divide one whole number by another whole number,
Carry out or use a both with multiple digits
procedure in a given
situation 3.2 Implementing e.g. Determine in which situations Newton’s second law is
appropriate
4. ANALYZE 4.1 Differentiating e.g. Distinguish between relevant and irrelevant numbers
Break material into in a mathematical word problem
constituent parts and
determine how parts 4.2 Organizing e.g. Structure evidence in a historical description into
evidence for and against a particular historical explanation
relate to one another
and to an overall 4.3 Attributing e.g. Determine the point of view of the author in an essay
structure or purpose in terms of his or her political perspective
19
This taxonomy is an expansion of an established and canonical model in educational
research, and Anderson et al. describe how to categorize pedagogical interventions according to
this framework. This taxonomy also has the advantage of illuminating the degree to which the
dimension of the taxonomy, which corresponds to the numerical scale of 1-6 and is a
students. The knowledge dimension, on the other hand, corresponds to the depth of expertise that
is required of students. The lower levels of this dimension involve simple factual recall and
knowledge of the relationships between concepts. The next highest dimension involves the
implementation of a procedure, which requires more active involvement from the student than
previous levels because they are required to construct something, even if it is by rote. Finally, the
20
highest dimension of knowledge corresponds to metacognition, or the process of thinking about
In order to classify the curricular materials according to this taxonomy, we assess each
practice exercise in the relevant textbook sections and place it in the appropriate category in the
taxonomy. We can then ascertain the levels of thinking the curricula require of students by
counting how many problems from each textbook fall into each category and analyzing the
The final measurement of the curriculum is the degree to which the explanation of the
mathematics aligns with an axiomatically correct understanding of the concepts. This is crucial
because it directly determines whether students have the opportunity to gain an accurate
understanding of mathematics from reading the book or participating in a lesson. The concept of
will use this term in the same sense as Hung-Hsi Wu in his series of books of mathematics of the
secondary school curriculum (Wu, 2020). For a textbook to inspire an axiomatically correct
understanding of mathematics, it must begin with a set of basic assumptions, known as axioms,
and build each subsequent result by proving it using the axioms and any other results that have
already been proven using those axioms. In the U.S., books that fit this description are usually
not accessible until university mathematics, predominantly because a certain level of intellectual
maturity is required to grasp the purpose and rigor of an axiomatic development. However, as
Wu argues, secondary school students could benefit from a closer approximation of this
21
Therefore, we will measure the “correctness” of the mathematics, in an axiomatic sense,
by noting to what degree the theorems, equations, and formulas are explicitly justified in the
curriculum. These theorems, equations, and formulas are usually called results in the
which each follow from previous arguments in the sequence in conjunction with a set of basic
axioms. Hence, a proof is a definitive, water-tight demonstration that a particular result is true.
At worst, results are stated outright with no proof or reference to prior material, while at best,
they are proven rigorously with reference to previous results, which were also proven rigorously.
In between these two extremes are results that are not proven entirely rigorously, but
which the curriculum makes attempts to justify or provide motivation for. Keeping with the
mathematical terminology, justification is taken to mean a line of reasoning that is not a proof
but rather a non-rigorous explanation. The aim of justification in a mathematical text is to help
mathematical text may provide justification in the form of an example of a special case in which
the result is true or a heuristic explanation that may serve as an outline for a potential proof.
Motivation, on the other hand, is a term used for an exposition of the result. When a
mathematical text provides motivation for a result, it explains why the result is important or
helpful, often alluding to future applications or deeper consequences that the result may have.
To this end, each result that appears in the curricular material is coded according to the
following scale:
22
(3) Stated with no proof but some justification;
(4) Stated with partial proof (including if the results used in the partial proof were not
themselves proven; and
Finally, this study compares the qualities of the pedagogy and curriculum with the
the data referenced are the racial makeup of the neighborhoods, average family income, per
capita income, and percentage of families with children under the age of eighteen that are living
below the poverty line. The data used for this study is taken from the 2021 American
Community Survey. The data was drawn from each census tract that corresponds to the school
district’s area of service, then the data points from each tract were averaged to obtain the final
statistic for each school. In the case of one school, this approach offers a higher level of precision
than simply taking the demographic data from the city as a whole, since the city is relatively
small geographically and greater precision was required to demarcate it from its three closely
neighboring cities. In contrast, the area served by the other school is geographically much more
consolidated. However, for consistency, we used the same approach of averaging the data from
There are a variety of ways to measure the resources available to the school and its
teachers. On one hand, the quantitative measurements pertaining to income and wealth
correspond heavily to the financial state of the school. This is because schools are predominantly
funded through property taxes, while parents are also asked to contribute to their schools through
fundraising, which brings parental income and wealth into relevance. A final indicator of the
resources available to teachers is the degree of professional development support that teachers
report receiving from their school, which is also measured in a question in our interviews. In
23
addition, we ask teachers to report the degree to which they feel external factors outside their
control impede their ability to teach, and give them an opportunity to describe these factors. We
classify these responses according to whether the factors teachers report are related to social and
economic conditions. With these measurements, we were equipped to analyze whether the nature
of the curriculum used and/or the nature of the pedagogy employed at a certain school is
Results
This study focuses on two high school classrooms in the Bay Area. For the purpose of
anonymity, the schools will be referred to as “School A” and “School B”, and the teachers will
be referred to as “Teacher A” and “Teacher B”, respectively. Both School A and School B are
public high schools that serve between 1,000 and 2,000 students. Teacher A teaches three
different classes, which include a “regular” third year math class, a remedial third year math
class, and an Advanced Placement Calculus class. For this study, I observed both the “regular”
and remedial third year classes. The instructor has been teaching for 13 years in total, all of
which have been at School A. Teacher B also teaches three classes, including Advanced
Placement Statistics, “regular” statistics, and geometry. For this study, we observed both the
Advanced Placement and “regular” statistics classes. Teacher B has been teaching for 18 years,
and previously taught at two charter schools before taking their current position at School B.
Interestingly, and perhaps fortunately for this study, Teacher A was teaching an introductory
statistics module at the time of my observation. Thus, all of the classes we observed were
focusing on statistics. This allows for a more direct comparison of the pedagogy and the
24
curricular materials by controlling for one important variable: the actual content that is being
taught.
We begin with an overview of the basic socioeconomic data of the surrounding areas of
each school. As the chart below demonstrates, the racial makeup of City B is significantly more
diverse than that of City A. Where City A is majority-white (about 61%), only about 46% of City
B is white. Moreover, within every non-white racial demographic, City B has a higher
percentage than City A. In economic terms, while both cities have a relatively high average
family income and per-capita income, City A is notably wealthier by these metrics. Additionally,
after correcting for outliers, City B has more than double the percentage of families with children
CITY A CITY B
Race (pct.) White 60.98 46.40
25
**with correction for two outlier tracts. 5.54 without outlier correction.
***with correction for one outlier tract. 6.38 without outlier correction.
Data from American Community Survey, 2021.
SCHOOL A SCHOOL B
Level of Student Involvement Pct. of Instructional Time
Additionally, each of the two teachers we observed taught two types of classes. At School
A, we observed a regular class and a remedial class, while at School B we observed an Advanced
Placement class and a regular class. The table below splits the data above according to this:
26
Table 3: Level of Student Involvement as Percentage of Instructional Time, By Class
SCHOOL A SCHOOL B
Level of Student Involvement Pct. of Instructional Time
The qualitative data extracted from the classroom observations falls into several
categories. In particular, we focused on the abilities required for students to perform the activities
at hand, along with the actual behavior of the students. The latter proved to be particularly
relevant, given that the majority of the time in both classrooms was spent in group work. The
abilities required for students to perform fully in the classroom did not significantly differ from
the abilities required by the respective textbook. This is because the activities that took place in
the classroom were generally at or below level (3) on the participation scale described above.
Consequently, students in both classrooms spent the vast majority, if not the entirety, of their
time engaging at a level that did not require much originality. In other words, the activities that
students did in class aligned very closely with the problems given in the textbooks for both
27
courses. Thus, the degree of flexibility of the pedagogy was tightly tied with the degree of
SCHOOL A SCHOOL B
Abilities required to Reading and interpreting charts Creating charts on a graphing
perform fully Applying probability formulas calculator
Drawing charts Interpreting graphs and charts
Assessing statistical Determine cause and effect
significance Assessing statistical significance
Solving equations and Distinguish the strengths and
inequalities weaknesses of different visual
Drawing and interpreting graphs data displays
Working in teams to collect data Comment on trends in data and
Deciding the most appropriate offer possible explanations for
chart to use for a given data set them
Manipulating exponential
functions
Simplifying expressions
Calculating area and perimeter
Teacher positioning Sitting in desk in the corner of Walking around the classroom
during group work the room Checking in with each group
Not getting up unless a student individually over the course of
raises their hand or calls out the class
Student activity during Conversing with each other, Conversing with each other,
group work mostly about the activity both about the activity and about
Room was often silent for other things
several minutes at a time Room was never silent
Several students were outwardly Frequently raising their hands to
exasperated call teacher over
Occasionally raising their hands
to call teacher over
Student activity during Very little time spent in this Many students (at least 7)
teacher-directed activity mode raising hands in response to
Several students (between 2 and questioning from the teacher
5) raising hands in response to Some students shouting out
questioning from the teacher responses
28
The curricular materials were first classified according to the Taxonomy for Teaching and
Learning by Anderson, et al (Anderson et al. 2001). Due to the cumbersome nature of the tables,
they will be provided in the appendix and a summary will be provided here.
We analyzed 118 problems from School A’s textbooks. 67 of these were taken from the
textbook for the remedial class (Dietiker, 2014) and 51 of these were taken from the textbook for
the regular class (Kysh, 2015). The chapters for curricular analysis were those that directly
correspond to the material students were learning in the class periods that I observed. Of these
problems, nearly half (57 of 118) fell into the category C3 in the taxonomy, which is Apply
Procedural Knowledge. Questions that fall into this category are those that ask students to
perform a computation by applying a formula or procedure that they had previously been given.
The remainder of the questions were relatively scattered across other categories, but with no
more than 10 in any other category, and with several categories having none at all. These
categories were A5, A6, B1, B6, D1, D2, and D3. Interestingly, when analyzed separately, the
regular and remedial curriculum displayed some differences. Specifically, none of the 51
problems from the regular class book were in category D (which is the metacognitive dimension)
or category 6 (which is the “create” dimension). The remedial curriculum, however, had three
problems in category D and two in category 6, including one question that was in category D6.
While both the regular and remedial curriculum had a drastic spike in questions in category C3,
the spike was slightly more pronounced in the remedial curriculum than in the regular
curriculum. 55.2% (37 of 67) of the remedial problems fell into this category, while 39.2% (20 of
Teacher B used the same textbook (Rossman et al. 2008) for their Advanced Placement
and their regular statistics classes. Both classes were covering the same material at the time of
29
my observation. The corresponding section in Textbook B contained 44 problems. Within these
problems, no one category in the taxonomy was as dominant as the C3 category was in Textbook
A. The most prominent categories were B2 (27.3%) and B4 (25%), followed closely by C4
(20.4%) and then C3 (15.9%). While the spread was more even, it was also narrower, as only
seven of the 24 categories were represented. In particular, there were no questions that fell into
category A (factual knowledge) or category 6 (create). Only one of the 44 problems fell into
emerged. The School A textbook section was composed entirely of practice exercises, with little
actual text beyond what was included in the problems. The chapter contains several blocks of
text which provide key definitions that are necessary to do the problems, but no results (again,
this term is used in the mathematical sense of a theorem, equation, or formula). Moreover, there
behind any of the exercises. On the other hand, the section from Textbook B contained three
results. Each of these results was stated with no proof but with some justification, which classify
The chart below reveals the key features of each textbook that emerged during the
30
Table 5: Qualitative Comparison of Textbooks
SCHOOL A SCHOOL B
Abilities required to Reading and interpreting charts Creating charts on a graphing
perform adequately Applying probability formulas calculator
Drawing charts Interpreting graphs and charts
Assessing statistical significance Determine cause and effect
Solving equations and Assessing statistical significance
inequalities Distinguish the strengths and
Drawing and interpreting graphs weaknesses of different visual
Working in teams to collect data data displays
Deciding the most appropriate Comment on trends in data and
chart to use for a given data set offer possible explanations for
Manipulating exponential them
functions
Simplifying expressions
Calculating area and perimeter
Substitution of abilities Only possible in the question that Many broad questions which
requires students to work in could allow for multiple
teams to collect data and create a interpretations of the same data
visual representation of it set to be correct
Assistance for those Several paragraphs contain key Several paragraphs contain key
who are confused definitions definitions
Only one example Questions that ask students to
create graphs often provide
detailed instructions
Several pages are dedicated to
describing the process of
creating and interpreting a graph
Several results provide guidance
in interpreting graphs and warn
students of potential pitfalls
Consistency of topic Not every question focused on Every question focused on the
the topic that the section claims topic that the section claims to
to cover. cover
Completely unrelated questions
sometimes appear with no
reasoning behind it (or even a
“review” label)
31
The final set of data to report is that from the in-depth interviews. The primary purpose of
these interviews was to get a sense of the resources available to the teachers and the limitations
that they face. We also took the opportunity to ask some questions about their general teaching
philosophy and approach to pedagogy in general. When asked if they feel that they have the
freedom to explore new teaching strategies, both Teacher A and Teacher B responded
affirmatively. Interestingly, when asked if they feel that their ability to explore new teaching
strategies is limited by factors outside their control, both Teacher A and Teacher B gave strong
affirmative responses. “I want to do these things, but I can’t do the entire cognitive lift myself,”
explains Teacher B. They go on to state that they feel limited by “[p]arent expectations,
resistance from students, and sometimes there’s a lack of incentive from my fellow colleagues.”
Teacher A expressed a similar sentiment, mentioning “parents, students, and money” as the
When asked for their thoughts on their textbooks, Teacher A and Teacher B had notably
different attitudes. Teacher A said, “It is what it is. You just kind of go with it. Maybe it’s not the
best all the time, but it gives you at least a starting point to build your lesson from.” This
philosophy seemed to be reflected in their approach to lesson planning, as each of the classes that
I observed revolved heavily around the textbook. Specifically, the group work that students were
On the other hand, Teacher B expressed a sense of thoughtful separation from the
textbooks they used. “I'm a huge proponent of the reminder that textbooks are not the
curriculum,” they told me. “Sometimes I have to tell my colleagues: It doesn't matter what you
“covered” [gesturing with air quotes]. That’s a statement of how you spent your time, but did the
students actually understand it?” As with Teacher A, their philosophy also seemed apparent in
32
their lesson planning, as the group work that students were assigned consisted primarily of
Advanced Placement- level practice exam questions (in the Advanced Placement class) or a
Both Teacher A and Teacher B felt that they could benefit from more professional
development opportunities from their schools. In particular, Teacher B remarked that their
professional development is dictated by “self motivation, which is cool, but also weird that it's
not being asked or expected of you.” Both indicated that they would feel more able to implement
equity-based pedagogy in their work if they had greater access to training and resources to help
them do so.
We begin with the analysis of the classroom observations. One implication of these
findings is that the amount of time spent in group work scenarios is not necessarily indicative of
the depth of learning that students experience. In Teacher A’s classroom, the average pedagogical
index was 2.87, whereas the average index in Teacher B’s classroom was 2.41. This score
reflects the “average” type of activity students are involved in over the course of a class period,
and therefore this contrast indicates that students in Teacher B’s classroom spent, on average,
less time working in groups than Teacher A’s students. The overall dataset indicates that this was
Based on this data, one might assume that the depth of student involvement in Teacher
A’s classroom was significantly higher than that in Teacher B’s classroom. Yet, the qualitative
observations of the classroom environments tell a largely different story. Although students were
33
assigned group work in Teacher A’s classroom, the attitudes with which they approached the
group work were markedly more detached than those of Teacher B’s students, and the level of
interaction between group members in Teacher A’s classroom was lower than the level of
interaction between students in Teacher B’s classroom. Moreover, Teacher B took a much more
active role in the facilitation of group work than Teacher A. The overall effect was that students
in Teacher B’s classroom were more actively engaged while they were participating in group
work than those in Teacher A’s classroom, even though they spent less time doing so.
This suggests that group work must be effectively designed in order to engender
meaningful collaboration, and that instructor involvement in group work may play a role in the
involvement of the students with each other. Neither of these are particularly new findings (see
Boaler 2006, for example), nor are they likely to be surprising to anyone who is familiar with
educational settings. Despite this, it is still interesting to note the large discrepancy in the quality
and quantity of student-to-student interaction that existed between the two classrooms. It is also
crucial to note the incongruence between the qualitative and quantitative measurements that were
taken in the same environment at the same time. Indeed, however carefully designed,
quantitative metrics can encapsulate only so much of the dynamic environment of a high school
classroom.
Another interesting and perhaps unexpected finding of this case study pertains to the
curricular materials. School A is located in an affluent area and has a student body which is, on
the whole, less diverse and more socioeconomically privileged than the student body of School
B. Consequently, the body of research would predict that School A is likely to have superior
curricular materials to School B. While its textbook is newer (it was published in 2015, whereas
34
the textbook from School B was published in 2008), the content analysis reveals that the newer
standpoint because it eschewed axioms entirely. The book consists almost entirely of problems,
with some sparse paragraphs providing the bare minimum definitions that one would need to
even understand what the questions were asking. Furthermore, the qualitative analysis reveals
that the book provides very little support for students who may be struggling and very few
opportunities to approach a problem from multiple angles. This has critical implications for
equity. Most obviously, the curriculum privileges students who already have a good grasp on the
subject. For those students who have a fundamental lack of understanding, the lack of actual
exposition means that their misconceptions may never be addressed. Worse, they may even be
reinforced, since the procedural nature of the vast majority of the problems necessitates little
conceptual understanding.
Another notable feature of Textbook A is its extreme focus on C3 questions (Recall that
this category in the Taxonomy for Teaching and Learning is called Applying Procedural
Knowledge). These questions have the merit of asking students to construct their own solution to
a problem, and in this sense they require a level of active involvement from the student beyond
factual recall. However, this active involvement does not necessitate original thought. Oftentimes
C3 questions can be solved by referencing a similar problem as an example and using the same
exact steps while replacing the old numbers with new ones. Therefore, it is possible to get
through these problems by algorithmically applying a procedure while not having a deep
understanding of why one is applying this procedure, where the procedure originates from, or
35
Moreover, the section from Textbook A contained a variety of problems that were largely
unrelated to what the section was supposedly about. Ostensibly, this could be done for the
purpose of refreshing students’ memory of past topics, but the lack of transparency around why
these problems are included is worrisome. This may give students the impression that
individually. Not only does this not contribute to students gaining a clear understanding of
mathematics, but it actively undermines it. Although it does indeed contain a vast array of
Textbook B also fell prey to some of the same pitfalls as Textbook A. Specifically, the
range of questions it asks is relatively narrow, according to the quantitative classification. Thus,
the range of abilities required for students to fully participate is also relatively narrow. However,
Textbook B is overall much more mathematically sound. Although it is still relatively inflexible,
it has a significantly higher degree of flexibility than Textbook A. This is because, as the
qualitative analysis shows, it generally asks broader questions than Textbook A. These questions
allow for students to create multiple distinct but still valid interpretations of the same problem.
This is a more accurate representation of the practice of statistics than that given by Textbook A.
Additionally, Textbook B contains several results that bear resemblance to theorems, even
though they are not stated as such, and it provides some justification for these results.
Consequently, Textbook B has a higher level of mathematical integrity than Textbook A, and is
lower on the Taxonomy for Teaching and Learning than the questions in Textbook A.
36
while the majority of Textbook A questions were at level C (Procedural Knowledge).
Nonetheless, the questions asked by Textbook B tended to be broader and more conducive to
students creating their own interpretations and thoughts than those asked by Textbook A. This
suggests that, in this case, whether a problem requires a student to demonstrate conceptual or
procedural knowledge may not be an accurate indicator of the actual depth of knowledge it
requires of a student.
The presence of complete proofs in any high school curriculum is likely sparse at best,
due to the limitations imposed by the cognitive demand of an axiomatic approach. However, we
argue that the high school curriculum does have the bandwidth to provide at least some
justification and motivation for the results that it expects students to develop mastery of. When
schools and teachers use books that lack mathematical integrity, such as Textbook A, they do
their students a severe disservice and rob them of the opportunity to develop an understanding of
Ultimately, these patterns in the quality of student interaction and the quality of the
curriculum run somewhat contrary to what educational theory and research would suggest. As
the socioeconomic data show, the population of School B is, on the whole, less racially
privileged and less economically secure than the population of School A. Thus, theories such as
Friere’s banking model of education (Friere 2018) and research such as Anyon’s work on the
correlation between education and social class (Anyon 1980) would predict that the pedagogy
and curriculum at School B would focus more on rote procedural learning than the pedagogy and
pedagogical index between schools. However, Teacher B’s pedagogy was often more interactive
than Teacher A’s pedagogy, and Textbook B is generally less focused on procedural learning than
37
Textbook A. Indeed, Textbook B provides much more of an opportunity for students to develop
Teacher B appears more motivated to ensure her students understand the material, and exposes
In our interviews, both Teacher A and Teacher B indicated that they would feel more able
to incorporate more equity-based pedagogy into their own work if they had greater access to paid
taught in teacher preparation programs, and it is often somewhat disjoint from existing
curriculum and standard pedagogy. Hence, if teachers want to become well-versed in it, they
must either learn it either through an official professional development program or on their own
time and money. Therefore, teachers who serve socioeconomically disadvantaged populations-
the very teachers who would do well to be trained in equity-based pedagogy- may be less likely
to have access to that training. Teacher A and Teacher B both expressed a desire to improve their
familiarity with and expertise in equity-based pedagogy, but indicated that time and money were
often the primary limiting factors that prevented them from doing so. The experiences of Teacher
A and Teacher B suggest that paid professional development opportunities for teachers could be
Inherent to any study of the mechanisms of a system as broad as the education system are
a number of limitations. Indeed, as indicated in our review of recent literature, the field of
educational research is currently reckoning with the difficulty of producing generalizable results
through conventional means of investigation. Consequently, this study makes no attempt to assert
its findings to be indicative of any pattern in the education system at large. Rather, we argue that
the main contribution of this study lies in its design. In particular, it combines a social-scientific
38
investigation into the intersection of equity and education with a technical approach to curricular
analysis. This is somewhat unique because the majority of research that focuses on education
from a social justice standpoint does not seek to analyze the mathematical content of curriculum,
whereas research that focuses on the mathematical content of the curriculum tends to address
equity parenthetically, if at all. By doing these in parallel, the approach used in this study
in a rigorous way, with the socioeconomic factors that are a familiar staple in the majority of
39
40
References
Anderson, Lorin W., et al. 2001. A Taxonomy for Teaching, Learning, and Assessing: A Revision
of Bloom’s Taxonomy of Educational Objectives. New York: Longman.
Anyon, Jean. 1980. “Social Class and the Hidden Curriculum of Work.” The Journal of
Education 162 (1): 67-92.
Cohen, Elizabeth G., Rachel A. Lotan, Beth A. Scarloss, and Adele R Arellano. 1999. “Complex
Instruction: Equity in Cooperative Learning Classrooms.” Theory Into Practice 38 (2):
80–86. https://doi.org/10.1080/00405849909543836.
Boutte, Gloria, and Charlease Kelly-Jackson. 2010. “Culturally Relevant Teaching in Science
Classrooms: Addressing Academic Achievement, Cultural Competence, and Critical
Consciousness.” International Journal of Multicultural Education 12 (2): 20.
Boaler, Jo. 2006. “How a Detracked Mathematics Approach Promoted Respect, Responsibility,
and High Achievement.” Theory Into Practice 45 (1): 40–46.
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15430421tip4501_6.
Dietiker, Leslie. Core Connections Integrated II. Sacramento: CPM Educational Program, 2014.
Freire, Paulo. 2018. Pedagogy of the Oppressed: 50th Anniversary Edition. New York:
Bloomsburg Publishing Inc.
Grande, Sandy. 2014. “Red Pedagogy: The Un-Methodology”. In Handbook of Critical and
Indigenous Methodologies. Ed. Norman K. Denzin, Yvonna S. Lincoln & Linda Tuhiwai
Smith, 233-254. Thousand Oaks: SAGE Publications, Inc., 2014.
https://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9781483385686.
Hammond, Zaretta. 2015. “Climbing out of the Gap” and “What’s Culture Got to Do with It?” in
Culturally Responsive Teaching and the Brain: Promoting Authentic Engagement and
Rigor Among Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Students, 12-34. California: Corwin.
Jacobson, Michael J., Kapur, Manu, and Reimann, Peter. 2016. “Conceptualizing Debates in
Learning and Educational Research: Toward a Complex Systems Conceptual Framework
of Learning.” Educational Psychologist 0(0): 1-9.
https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2016.1166963.
41
Jacobson, Michael J., Levin, James A., and Kapur, Manu. 2019. “Education as a Complex
System: Conceptual and Methodological Implications. Educational Researcher 48 (2):
112-119. https://www.jstor.org/stable/45280846.
Jones, Stephanie Michelle, and Vagle, Mark. 2013. “Living Contradictions and Working for
Change: Toward a Theory of Social Class-Sensitive Pedagogy.” Educational Researcher
42, no. 3 (April): 129-141. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189x13481381.
Kuhn, Deanna. 1993. “Science as Argument: Implications for Teaching and Learning Scientific
Thinking.” Science Education 77 (3): 319–337. https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.3730770306.
Kysh, Judith. Core Connections Integrated III. Sacramento: CPM Educational Program, 2015.
Ladson-Billings, Gloria. 2006. “From the Achievement Gap to the Education Debt:
Understanding Achievement in U.S. Schools.” Educational Researcher 35 (7): 3–12.
https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X035007003.
Lemke, Jay L., and Sabelli, Nora H. 2008. “Complex Systems and Educational Change: Towards
a new research agenda.” Educational Philosophy and Theory 40 (1): 118-129.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-5812.2007.00401.
Nash, Roy. 1990. “Bourdieu on Education and Social and Cultural Reproduction.” British
Journal of Sociology of Education 11 (4): 431–47.
https://doi.org/10.1080/0142569900110405.
Nasir, Na’ilah Suad, and Maxine McKinney de Royston. 2013. “Power, Identity, and
Mathematical Practices Outside and Inside School.” Journal for Research in Mathematics
Education 44 (1): 264–87. https://doi.org/10.5951/jresematheduc.44.1.0264.
Porter, Andrew, et al. 2011. “Common Core Standards: The New U.S. Intended Curriculum.”
Educational Researcher 40 (3): 103-116. https://www.jstor.org/stable/41238917.
Redish, Edward F. 2003. “Cognitive Principles and Guidelines for Instruction.” In Teaching
Physics with the Physics Suite, 17-50. Somerset: John Wiley & Sons Inc.
Rogers, J., Mirra, N., Seltzer, M., & Jun, J. 2014. “It’s About Time: Learning Time and
Educational Opportunity in California High Schools.” Los Angeles: UCLA IDEA.
Rossman, Allan J., et al. Workshop Statistics: Discovery with Data and the Graphing Calculator.
Wiley, 2008.
42
Rubin, Beth C., et al. 2006. “Structuring Inequality at Berkeley High.” In Unfinished Business:
Closing the Racial Achievement Gap in our Schools, edited by Pedro Noguera and Jean
Yonemura Wing, 29-86, Jossey-Bass.
Schmidt, William H., and Houang, Richard T. 2012. “Curricular Coherence and the Common
Core State Standards for Mathematics.” Educational Researcher 41 (8): 294-308.
https://www.jstor.org/stable/23272323.
Wu, Hung-Hsi. 2020. Rational Numbers to Linear Equations. Rhode Island: American
Mathematical Society.
43