Britton HomophobiaHomosocialityAnalysis 1990
Britton HomophobiaHomosocialityAnalysis 1990
Britton HomophobiaHomosocialityAnalysis 1990
REFERENCES
Linked references are available on JSTOR for this article:
https://www.jstor.org/stable/4120971?seq=1&cid=pdf-
reference#references_tab_contents
You may need to log in to JSTOR to access the linked references.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms
Taylor & Francis, Ltd. is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to
The Sociological Quarterly
Dana M. Britton*
University of Texas
George Weinberg first coined the term "homophobia" in 1967. He conceives of homo-
phobia as an "irrational revulsion." More specifically, he defines it as the "irrational
condemnation of homosexuals" which results in "violence, deprivation, and separa-
tion" (1972, p. vii). Since Weinberg's definition, other researchers have developed
different labels in an effort to more precisely describe the phenomenon, such as "homo-
sexophobia" (Levitt and Klassen 1974), the fear of homosexuality; and "homonegati-
vism" (Hudson and Ricketts 1980), a general affective and behavioral dislike of
homosexuals. Though these terms' histories are rather short, the feeling described has
existed for thousands of years. The present research uses the more general term "homo-
phobia," taken to mean the fear and dislike of lesbians and gay men.
A dramatic shift in the study of minority/majority relations fostered the relatively
recent research focus on homophobia. Early literature on homosexuality sought the
behavioral "defect" in the personalities of homosexuals that made them "different."
Indeed, homosexuality was the topic of many psychological studies on pathological
deviance. The change from the study of the homosexual minority to the heterosexual
majority represents a fundamental paradigm shift. The "organism deficiency" model
has given way to a "social deficiency" model (MacDonald and Games 1974). Research-
ers are now exploring the effects of the majority on an oppressed minority (Herek
1984b).
*Direct all correspondence to: Dana M. Britton, Department of Sociology, University of Texas, Austin, TX 78712.
LITERATURE REVIEW
Efforts to understand homophobia have been, at best, sporadic. The first empirical
research did not appear until 1971 (Smith 1971). Subsequent empirical and theoretical
approaches have been discrete entities. As Herek (1984b, p. 4) notes, "Social scientists
attempting to explain why so many people hold negative feelings toward homosexual
persons have tended to offer either theoretical speculations or empirical data, with little
synthesis of the two."
One prominent explanation of homophobia focuses on the link between religion and
homosexuality, from the standpoint of Biblical prohibitions. Since Western Christian
theology condemns homosexuality as a sin, homophobia is a reflection of religiosity.
In this vein, Greenberg and Bystyrn suggest that a cornerstone of the development of
the modern church was "the crystallization of a distinctly Christian intolerance of
homosexuality" (1982, p. 517). Indeed, one study finds that "rejection of homosexuality
is an integral part of the American Christian heritage" (Nyberg and Alston 1976).
Psychologists, primarily those of the Freudian school, proffer another position.
Freud holds that while everyone has homoerotic feelings, they are repressed in the
course of "normal" sexual development: those who successfully resolve the Oedipal
conflict become normal adults. Inherent in this position is the belief that homosexuality
Empirical Studies
2. Age and education. The older and less educated are more hom
and Ricketts 1980; Nyberg and Alston 1976; Thompson, Grisa
1985; West 1979).
6. Marital status. Those who are single are more homophobic (Hu
etts 1980).
Though past research correlates homophobia with many different variables, several
shortcomings are apparent. Most treat homophobia as unidimensional, assessing it with
one or two questions (Alston 1974; Levitt and Klassen 1974; Nyberg and Alston 1976;
West 1979). Only very recently has its multidimensional nature been emphasized and
scales constructed accordingly (Herek 1984a; Millham, San Miguel, and Kellogg 1976;
Weinberger and Millham 1979).
HYPOTHESES
METHODS
The data draw from a survey of 332 adults (ages 18 and above) residing in a medium
sized southwestern city. Respondents are randomly selected from the Polk Directo
the most comprehensive sampling frame available for this city. Members of the ta
sample who did not participate are replaced by random selection. The sample's dem
graphic characteristics do not significantly differ from those of the study popula
Items assessing homophobia directed at gay men derive from Herek (1984a)
accordance with his recommendations, items load on his "Condemnation-Toler
factor. A subset of 6 items from his scale are reported in Table 1, recoded, with h
scores indicating a high degree of homophobia.
Only one factor emerges using principal axis factoring on these items. This fact
(eigenvalue = 4.28) accounts for 71.3% of total common variance.2 As all comm
ties are above .58, all items share more than 58% of their variance with the comm
factor. Factor loadings, all above .75, indicate that all items highly correlate with
common factor (Table 1). A summed Z-score scale was created and a reliabili
analysis of the composite reveals an alpha of .96. These results replicate Herek's (19
finding that a single, bipolar condemnation-tolerance factor is appropriate in asses
homophobia.
Items measuring attitudes toward women, reported in Table 2, come from Mac-
Donald (1974), selected from two factors. The first treats sex-appropriate behavior
(masculinity/femininity); the second, roles for women in business and the professions.
Again, responses are recoded, high scores indicating a conservative attitude.
Table 1
Univariate Statistics: Scale Items-Male Homophobia (N = 330)
Standard Factor
Item Mean Deviation Loading**
The growing number of male homosex
decline in American morals.*
Male homosexuality is merely a different kind of lifestyle 2.864 1.083 .773
that should not be condemned.
Male homosexuality is a threat to many of our basic 2.867 1.095 .776
social institutions.*
If a man has homosexual feelings, he should do 3.216 0.955 .878
everything he can to overcome them.*
Male homosexuality is an inferior form of sexuality.* 3.034 1.122 .799
Male homosexuality is a sin.* 3.093 1.168 .866
Notes. * Reverse-coded items.
?*One-factor solution.
Table 2
Univariate Statistics: Scale Items--Attitudes toward Women (N = 329)
Standard Factor
Item Mean Deviation Loading*
On the average women are less capable o
economic production than are men.*
Women should be concerned with their duties of 2.176 1.135 .805
child-rearing and house-tending, rather than with
desires for business and professional careers.*
Men are naturally more capable than women in financial 1.709 0.916 .732
matters.*
A woman who refuses to bear children has failed in her 1.800 1.009 .593
duty to her husband.*
There is hardly anything more revolting than seeing a 3.124 1.089
woman dress, act, and cuss like a man.*
A woman on the average needs protection and guidance.* 2.419 1.088 .721
Notes: * Reverse-coded items.
*One-factor solution.
Upon factor analysis, one factor (eigenvalue = 2.93) emerges and explains 58.6%
the total common variance. It accounts for more than 30% of the variance in each of
the items, as communalities range from .30 to .51. Factor loadings, all above .59,
indicate good correlation with the common factor (Table 2). The items have high
internal consistency; after one item, which deals with gender-inappropriate behavior,
is excluded, the scale alpha is .82. Results of this analysis are used to create a summed
Z-score scale.
Table 3 reports items assessing attitudes toward men, drawn from Brannon and Juni
(1983). In accordance with their results items load on the "avoiding feminity" and the
"toughness/violence" factors. Scale items are recoded, with high scores indicating
conservative attitudes.
As expected, factor analysis of these items reveals two factors. The first, "anti-
femininity," accounts for 40% of total common variance (eigenvalue = 2.4); the sec-
ond, "violence," for 23% (eigenvalue = 1.4). As all items have communalties above
.18, more than 18% of their variance is shared with the factors. Results of rotations
utilizing both Varimax and Oblimin methods are very similar, so the former are
reported here. Three items load on the anti-femininity factor at .4 or above, indicat-
ing their high correlation with the factor. Three items also load on the violence fac-
tor from .6 to .8 (Table 3). Two summed Z-score scales are used and the items are
also assessed for reliability. After testing the items loading on Brannon's "anti-femi-
ninity" factor separately for internal consistency, three items remain, with an alpha
of .63. Similar analysis of the items assessing violence derives three items kept for
analysis, with an alpha of .76. These results show less internal consistency for the
scale than that demonstrated by its creators, who studied a highly homogeneous
population.
Items measuring religiosity focus on personal religious commitment, rather than
denominational conservatism. High scores on the recoded scale indicate high religi-
osity.
Table 3
Univariate Statistics: Scale Items-Attitudes toward Men (N = 330)
Standard Factor
Item Mean Deviation Loading**
1 2
I think it's very good for a boy to be taught how to 1.421 0.720
cook, sew, clean the house, and take care of younger
children.
I don't think a husband should usually have to wash the 2.285 1.096 .041 .574
dishes, unless his wife is busy and he wants to help
out.*
A young man who was not interested in sports would 1.645 0.867 .193 .387
be a little odd in my opinion.*
As a general rule, a man should not have to do the 2.121 1.056 .124 .860
family laundry.*
It annoys me a little to see a man sitting with his legs 1.745 0.918 - -
crossed like a woman.*
A man must never let anyone push him around no 2.570 1.047 -
matter who they are.*
Fists are sometimes the only way to get out of a bad 1.858 0.974 .578 .148
situation. *
In some kinds of situations, a man should be ready to 2.273 1.042 .765 .138
use his fists, even if his wife or girlfriend objects.*
A man must sometimes be willing to use violence.* 2.336 1.085 .791 .097
Noters * Reverse-coded items.
Factor analysis results appear in Table 4. As expected, one factor emerges from the
six items (eigenvalue = 3.56), accounting for 59.3% of total common variance. Upo
a reliability analysis, two items were excluded; the resulting composite has high interna
consistency (alpha = .87). A final factor analysis is conclusive, as the remaining items
all have communalities above .5, sharing more than 50% of their variance with t
common factor. All factor loadings exceed .75, indicating high correlation with t
common factor. A summed Z-score scale was created for use in the analysis.
Finally, items assessing homosociality, reported in Table 5, accord with the theoret
cal implications of Lipman-Blumen (1976). The scale measures two aspects: strict
social preference for members of one's own gender and preference for the maintenanc
of sex-segregated institutions. The four-category Likert scale is used, high scores in-
dicating high homosociality.
Factor analysis reveals the two expected dimensions. The first factor (eigen-
value = 2.24) explains 37.3% of total common variance; the second (eigen
value = 1.58) explains 26.4%. Communalities range from .18 to .58, so all items sha
18% or more variance with the factors. Results using both Varimax and Oblimin fact
rotation are similar, so the former are reported. Three items load on the "interpersona
factor at .71 or above, indicating high correlation with the factor. Three load on the
"institutional" factor at .43 or above, also showing significant correlation. Table
depicts the final loadings. The three items measuring interpersonal preference f
members of one's own sex are reliable, with high internal consistency (alpha = .7
Table 4
Univariate Statistics: Scale Items-Religiosity (N = 329)
Standard Factor
Item Mean Deviation Loading**
Religion is a very important part of my life.* 3.367 0.852 .874
I would describe myself as very religious.* 2.830 0.978 .790
I believe a commitment to traditional values is important 3.497 0.694 -
in one's life.*
I believe in obeying the decisions of religious leaders 2.561 1.021 -
concerning moral issues.*
I believe it is important for me to attend religious services 2.942 1.097 .782
frequently.*
Religion should influence how I live my life.* 3.261 0.941 .759
Notes: * Reverse-coded items.
?*One-factor solution.
Table 5
Univariate Statistics: Scale Items-Homosociality (N = 329)
Standard Factor
Item Mean Deviation Loading**
1 2
In general, I feel most comfortable with members of 2.669 0.977 .753 .137
my own sex.*
I prefer to have close friends who are members of my 2.766 0.945 .762 .076
own sex.*
In general, I feel that my own sex understands me 2.711 0.962 .717 -.001
better.*
It is wrong to have girls in the Boy Scouts.* 2.791 1.123 .067 .727
It is good to have little boys in the Girl Scouts. 2.918 1.063 .029 .432
I have no objection to all-male organizations such as 3.045 1.014 .069 .514
lodges and clubs which purposely exclude women.*
Notes: * Reverse-coded items.
**Two-factor solution-Varimax rotation.
Table 6
Univariate Statistics: Demographic Variables (N = 327)
Standard
Item Mean Deviation
ANALYSIS
Correlations
Table 7 reports the correlations between the homophobia scale and all independent
variables. All correlations, with the exception of gender, are statistically significant
(p < .05). Several aspects of this table are important. First, the largest correlations
are between homophobia and religiosity (.485) and homophobia and institutional
homosociality (.485). Attitudinal variables (attitudes toward women, both factors of the
attitudes toward men scale) also positively correlate. The "anti-feminity" factor, as
expected, is more strongly correlated with homophobia than is the violence factor.
Among demographic variables, age is positively correlated (.272); education, negatively
(-.296). Homophobia's smallest significant correlation is with the interpersonal
homosociality factor (.134), though in the anticipated direction.
Item correlations with the homosociality scales are also in the anticipated directions.
All items, with the exception of gender and the "violence" factor of the attitudes toward
men scale, correlate significantly with the "interpersonal" homosociality factor. The
other scale factor, "institutional" homosociality, correlates significantly, but at a weak
level (. 137). The strongest positive correlation is with attitudes on women (.334). Other
variables correlate positively in varying degrees, excepting education (-.177).
All items correlate significantly with the "institutional" factor of the homosociality
scale. All attitudinal scales positively correlate, attitudes toward women most strongly
(.393). Among the demographic variables age correlates with the factor positively
(.210); and education and "femaleness" negatively (--.140 and -.190, respectively).
Correlations between other attitudinal and demographic variables, on the whole
significant and in the anticipated directions, are reported in Table 7.
Path Analysis
Results of multivariate path analysis on all variables in the model serve to test the
hypotheses and are reported in Table 8 and Figure 1.
Hypothesis 7
Analysis affirms the first hypothesis, that support for traditional roles for women
increases homophobia. The correlation between attitudes toward women and homo-
phobia (Table 7) is strong, positive (.449), and in fact the second largest of any in the
model. This scale also has a significant direct effect on homophobia, reported in Table
8 (.159), and an indirect effect through the institutional homophobia factor
[(.153)(.264) = .040].
Hypothesis 2
Analysis also supports the second hypothesis, that support for traditional roles for
men increases homophobia. The correlation of homophobia with the "anti-femininity"
scale is significant and positive (.388), as is that with the "violence" factor (.184). In
view of the general identification of homosexual men as effeminate, this difference in
Attitudes
on Women
Femaleness
's Homosociality-
Interpersonal
SAnti-femininity
1b _.o'h Attitudes on Men- .25 -.io3S Homophobia-
Ae-.123 Homosociality--
Age - Institutional
Religiosity
Figure 7. Path diagram: All variables.
Note: Only significant paths shown.
strength of correlation is not surprising: Only the "anti-femininity" factor of the scale
significantly directly affects homophobia (.125). This factor also has an indirect effect
through institutional homosociality [(.205)(.264) = .054]. The "violence" factor,
though its direct effect on homophobia is insignificant (.068), indirectly affects it
through institutional homosociality [(. 125)(.264) = .033].
Hypothesis 3
Analysis also supports the hypothesis that religiosity increases homophobia. The
correlation coefficient between religiosity and homophobia (Table 7) is significant,
positive, and at .485 the largest of any in the matrix. Religiosity also produces the
greatest direct effect on homophobia (.340) and has an indirect effect through institu-
tional homosociality [(.231)(.264) = .061]. As expected, religiosity is a prime determi-
nant of homophobia.
Hypothesis 4
The fourth hypothesis, that homosociality increases homophobia, also finds sup-
port. The "interpersonal" factor is significantly positively correlated with homo-
phobia (.134). It has an insignificant direct effect on homophobia, which is not in
the anticipated direction (-.076). Due to that insignificance, indirect effects are not
assessed. The only variable significantly related to this factor is attitudes toward
women (.275).
Results for the "institutional" factor are more definitive. The scale correlates posi-
tively with homophobia (.485) at a level matched only by religiosity and has the
second-largest direct effect (.264). This result establishes that homosociality, at least
the "institutional" variety, is an important concept in explaining homophobia.
Hypothesis 5
Overall, all proposed hypotheses are supported in this research investigating the
petuation of homophobia. As expected, general religious and social conservatism
to homophobia. Conservatism about the proper roles of men and women seem
the source of this relationship. This is borne out by the finding that the "anti-fem
ity" factor of the attitudes toward men scale engenders a stronger direct effect t
"violence" factor. Respondents' fear of homosexual males appears to reflect c
for the maintenance of "proper" male and female roles. Unlike in many pr
studies, which only deal with the attitudes of college students, these relationshi
support in a survey of an adult sample.
The second major finding of the research is based on the introduction of t
variable, homosociality. Of the two dimensions of this concept tested, only the "in
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
A version of this article was presented at the annual meetings of the Southw
Social Science Association, Little Rock, Arkansas, March 29-April 1, 1989. The aut
thanks the College of Arts and Sciences, University of Oklahoma, for fundi
survey from which these data were taken, and Professors Robert Bursik and Ha
Grasmick for directing the survey. Thanks also to Dr. Carolyn Morgan, Dr. Gras
Dr. Christine Williams, and three anonymous reviewers for their comments on
drafts of this article.
NOTES
1. Parallel questions directed specifically at lesbians were asked but excluded from the present
report. While the theory of homosociality has great explanatory power for homophobia in
all-male institutions, it does little to explain homophobia directed at lesbians. The results from
those items are retained for future analysis.
2. I performed factor analysis on the entire scale to investigate whether two factors might
exist, one measuring homophobia directed at males, one at females. This was not the case.
However, only the male-targeted items are included for analysis due to the constraints implied
by the theory of homosociality.
REFERENCES
Aguero, J.E., L. Bloch, and D. Byrne. 1984. "The Relationships among Sexual Beliefs, Attitudes,
Experience, and Homophobia." Journal of Homosexuality 10(1/2): 95-107.
Alston, J.P. 1974. "Attitudes toward Extramarital and Homosexual Relations." Journal for the
Scientific Study of Religion 13: 479-481.
Black, K.N., and M.R. Stevenson. 1984. "The Relationship of Self-reported Sex-role Characteris-
tics and Attitudes toward Homosexuality." Journal of Homosexuality 10(2): 83-93.
Brannon, R., and S. Juni. 1983. "A Scale for Measuring Attitudes about Mascul
ical Documents 14(1): 6.
Bullough, V. 1976. Sexual Variance in Society and History. New York: Wil
Davies, C. 1982. "Sexual Taboos and Social Boundaries." American Journal of
1032-1063.
Gallup Poll. 1986. "Sharp Decline Found in Support for Legalizing Gay Relations." The Gallup
Report 254.
Grahn, J. 1984. Another Mother Tongue: Gay Words, Gay Worlds. Boston: Beacon Press.
Greenberg, D.F., and M.H. Bystyrn. 1982. "Christian Intolerance of Homosexuality." American
Journal of Sociology 88(3): 515-548.
Herek, G.M. 1984a. "Attitudes toward Lesbians and Gay Men: A Factor-analytic Study."
Journal of Homosexuality 10(1/2): 39-51.
- . 1984b. "Beyond Homophobia: A Social Psychological Perspective on Attitudes toward
Lesbians and Gay Men." Journal of Homosexuality 10(1/2): 1-21.
Hudson, W.W., and W.A. Ricketts. 1980. "A Strategy for the Measurement of Homophobia."
Journal of Homosexuality 5(4): 357-372.
Kite, M.E. 1984. "Sex Differences in Attitudes toward Homosexuals: A Meta-analytic Review."
Journal of Homosexuality 10(1/2): 69-81.
Laner, M.R., and R.H. Laner. 1980. "Sexual Preference or Personal Style? Why Lesbians are
Disliked." Journal of Homosexuality 5(4): 339-356.
Levitt, E.E., and A.D. Klassen. 1974. "Public Attitudes toward Homosexuality: Part of the 1970
National Survey by the Institute for Sex Research." Journal of Homosexuality 1(1):
29-43.
Lipman-Blumen, J. 1975. "Changing Sex Roles in American Culture: Future Directions for
Research." Archives of Sexual Behavior 4(4): 433-446.
-- . 1976.
tion"Toward
of Sociala Institutions."
Homosocial Theory of 15-31.
Signs 1(3): Sex Roles: An Explanation of the Sex Segrega-
Lumby, M.E. 1976. "Homophobia: The Quest for a Valid Scale." Journal ofHomosexuality 2(1):
39-47.
MacDonald, A.P., Jr. 1974. "Identification and Measurement of Multidimensional Attitudes
toward Equality Between the Sexes." Journal of Homosexuality 1(2): 165-182.
MacDonald, A.P., Jr., and R.G. Games. 1974. "Some Characteristics of Those Who Hold
Positive and Negative Attitudes toward Homosexuals." Journal of Homosexuality 1(1):
9-27.
Millham, J., C.L. San Miguel, and R. Kellogg. 1976. "A Factor-analytic Conceptualization of
Attitudes toward Male and Female Homosexuals." Journal of Homosexuality 2(1): 3-10.
Millham, J., and L.E. Weinberger. 1977. "Sexual Preference, Sex-role Appropriateness and
Restriction of Social Access." Journal of Homosexuality 3(2): 343-357.
Minnigerode, F.A. 1976. "Attitudes toward Homosexuality: Feminist Attitudes and Sexual
Conservatism." Sex Roles 2(4): 347-352.
Morin, S.F., and E.M. Garfinkle. 1978. "Male Homophobia." Journal of Social Issues 34(1):
29-47.
Nyberg, K.L., and J.P. Alston. 1976. "Analysis of Public Attitudes toward Homosexual Behav-
ior." Journal of Homosexuality 2(2): 99-107.
Patoglun-an, I., and J.M. Clair. 1986. "An Experimental Study of Attitudes toward Homosexu-
als." Deviant Behavior 7: 121-135.
Plasek, J.W., and J. Allard. 1984. "Misconceptions of Homophobia." Journal of Homosexuality
10(1/2): 23-37.
San Miguel, C., and J. Millham. 1976. "The Role of Cognitive and Situational Variables in
Aggression toward Homosexuals." Journal of Homosexuality 2(1): 11-27.
Staats, G.R. 1978. "Stereotype Content and Social Distance: Changing Views of Homosexual-
ity." Journal of Homosexuality 4(1): 15-27.
Thompson, E.H., C. Grisanti, and J.H. Pleck. 1985. "Attitudes toward the Male Role and Their
Correlates." Sex Roles 13(7/8): 413-427.
Weinberg, G. 1972. Society and the Healthy Homosexual. New York: St. Martin's Press.
Weinberger, L.E., and J. Millham. 1979. "Attitudinal Homophobia and Support of Traditional
Sex Roles." Journal of Homosexuality 4(3): 237-246.
West, W. 1979. "Public Tolerance of Homosexual Behavior." Cornell Journal ofSocial Relations
12: 25-36.