Envisioning Trust Trust Metaphors
Envisioning Trust Trust Metaphors
Envisioning Trust Trust Metaphors
info
Bjarne Espedal
Kjell Grønhaug
Marcus Selart
A large and rapidly growing trust-literature attests to the importance of trust in relationships
and organizations. Trust is a precondition for effective leadership, for well-functioning groups
and for organizational integration and commitment. As a result, a large literature has sought to
describe the relationship between trust and social outcomes as well as show how trust forms,
develops or in some cases breaks down(Fulmer & Gelfand, 2012; Burke et al. 2007; Dirks &
Trust has been defined in different ways, as a belief, an attitude, as intent or as a choice
(Kramer, 1999). An often used definition sees trust as “..a psychological state comprising the
1
intention to accept vulnerability based upon positive expectations of the intentions or behavior
Trust means different things to people in different situations. The trust or distrust a potential
ways from the trust a coach or teacher experiences towards a team-member or student, are
associated with different expectations toward a trustee and the situation, and suggest different
understandings manifest themselves in proverbs and citations such as “The only way to make
a man trustworthy is to trust him” (Henry Lewis Stimson) or “trust but verify” (Russian
proverb).
Existing contributions on trust however largely ignore people’s own understanding of trust –
instead, trust is assumed to exist, independently of the trustor’s own understanding of trust.
Seminal models like that of Mayer et al. (1995) build on a traditional input-throughput-output
in which “external influences operate mechanistically on action, but it does not itself have any
1989:1175). The self-system here becomes merely repository for environmental forces
As a result, existing theories of trust fail at describing significant variation with respect to
trust, variation relating to the various ways people construe and explain their trust in other
people. Not only does the existing research literature fail to account for variation but the
literature also intercepts itself from valuable insights about the underlying causal mechanisms
2
that shape trust and relationships. This is particularly noticeable with respect to our
understanding of the performative aspects of trust – how people shape and transform social
Here we argue that the experience and effects of trust are influenced by how people construe
trust in specific situations – people are not merely passive receptacles of information but bring
their own understanding of trust to social situations (Bandura, 1989). Drawing on the
metaphors we suggest have important ramifications for how people experience trust, how
people go about developing trust and peoples’ reactions to trust or changes in trust (trust-
breaches). Different trust-metaphors mediate the link between situational contingencies and
demands on the one hand and peoples’ strategies for managing different social situations.
Conceptual metaphors
Metaphor involves understanding and experiencing one thing in light of another (Lakoff &
Metaphors importantly are not merely linguistic phenomena but a fundamental feature of the
way our cognitive system works. People think in metaphors (Bowdle & Gentner, 2005;
Lakoff & Johnson, 2003; Lakoff, 1987). Conceptual metaphors then may or may not have
linguistic equivalents. One of the most prevalent and important metaphor, the box-metaphor
in which elements are seen as belonging either to one category or the other, has no
3
corresponding name, yet has a profound influences on how people think and reason (Lakoff,
1987).
The effects of metaphors on social information processing have been demonstrated through a
series of experiments using the metaphoric transfer strategy which assesses whether
influences the way people process information related to a dissimilar concepts in a way that
correspond to their metaphoric relationship (Landau, Meier & Keefer, 2010: 1047). Often
used metaphors in these studies include metaphors using the source concept of verticality for
positive and negative moods, or spatial distance for time. Metaphors thus have been found to
influence attention and memory processes (Robinson, Zabelina, Ode & Moeller, 2008), how
we perceive other people (Schubert, 2005), the perception of social symbols and
environments (Meier, Sellbom & Wygant, 2007) as well as attitudes (Meier & Robinson,
2004).
Understanding argue Lakoff & Johnson (2003) takes place within domains of experience
based on experiential gestalts that are experientially basic. Such basic experiences typically
involve our bodies and the interaction between our bodies with the natural environment or
with other people. Such experiences moreover because they reflect common human
experiences (we all have similar bodies) will be recurrent in all cultures. Examples include the
experienced relationship between health and an upright position which leads to the
metaphorical association between good and up. People grasp increasingly abstract constructs
and phenomena through the means of metaphors grounded in a more immediate and tangible
experience (Lakoff & Johnson, 2003, 1999). The term scaffolding is used to describe the
processes through which metaphoric connections develop from early experiences with non-
4
Metaphors are generative and inferential – they frame problems and suggest some solutions
while masking others. To say that a debate is a war implies transferring our knowledge of war
(e.g. war is about winning and inflicting wounds on the opponent) to the domain of a debate.
consequence talked about. To talk about a debate as a war thus highlights the combative,
tactical aspects of a debate. At the same time metaphors also hide other possible aspects of a
phenomenon, aspects that may be highlighted through alternative metaphors. Thus viewing a
debate as a dance highlights the cooperative aspects of a debate and suggests other goals
(cooperating to bring forth a new and enriched understanding of the debate-topic) (Lakoff,
1987; Schön, 1993). Metaphors (like intelligence is a fixed entity) thus form the foundation of
implicit theories that enable people to explain and form expectations about past and future
outcomes (Dweck & Leggett, 1988, Knee, Patrick & Lonsbary, 2003). As a result metaphors
also influence peoples’ motivation to pursue different actions, tactics or strategies (Dweck &
Leggett, 1988; Knee et al. 2003; Hong, Chiu, Dweck, Lin & Wan, 1999).
Here we focus on three conceptual metaphors for trust. These represent very different ways
of thinking about trust with very different implications as with respect how people go about
developing trust, how they respond to breaches of trust or interpret and experience trust. The
conceptual metaphors as of today have no name or are rarely or ever explicitly formulated.
They do however influence the way people think about trust and are at times reflected in
The first conceptual metaphor we refer to as “trust as a decision”. Here trust is essentially
seen as a reflection of properties of the benefactor of trust, the trustee. Developing trust here
5
involves the task of unpacking the true and assumed unchanging identity of the trustee as
either trustworthy or not. Trust here is often seen as developing over time «largely as a
function of the parties having a history of interaction that allows them to develop a
generalized expectancy that the other’s behavior is predictable and that he or she will act
constitutes a capacity of the trustor as opposed to a property of the trustee or the benefactor of
trust. The performance metaphor sees trust as a chore that involves effort and perseverance.
Unlike the former case in which trust is seen as reflective of a hidden property of the trustee,
here trust can be seen as a property of a process that the trustor initiates and supports.
The third conceptual metaphor sees «trust as an uncontrollable force». Here a trustor has little
or no control or insight into why he or she trusts someone. This third metaphor finds
expression in expressions like I cannot help but trust him “you just have to trust her”. Trust
here is attributed to psychological processes that are only partially open for introspection, a
force to be reckoned with or for which trustors will need to make precautions but that
otherwise leaves the trustor with little choice or opportunity for agency. This can be likened to
similar metaphors which see creativity as a divine revelation that cannot be forced.
The metaphors suggest different explanations for trust: Drawing on Weiner’s attributional
theory of achievement motivation and emotion (1995), the different trust-metaphors and their
accompanying explanations for trust cam be seen as varying with respect to two dimensions;
locus or the trustors’ perception of what drives the formation and development of trust and
control or how much control the trustor sees herself as exercising over the formation and
development of trust.
6
In the “trust as decision metaphor” – the locus of trust is in the trustee – trust reflects an
trust lies in the trustor – trust reflects a decision or effort on the part of the trustor. Finally in
the “trust as an uncontrollable force”-metaphor, the locus of trust resides outside the trustor
and the trustee. The “trust as a decision”-metaphor leaves the trustor with low to moderate
control; the trustor can protect herself against untrustworthy individuals but not alter the
suggests considerable control on behalf of the trustor in that a trustor is assumed to influence
leaves the trustor with the least control in that a trustor is assumed unable to control the
experience of trust. Different combinations of locus and control should produce distinct sets
of emotions. Hence, an internal locus (trustor) combined with high control (“trust as
alternatively shame (when trust is breached or falters). An external locus (focusing on the
trustee) when combined with controllability (“trust as decision”) should be associated with
gratitude or alternatively anger (Weiner, 1995). The “trust as an uncontrollable force” with
the locus residing neither in the trustor nor the trustee and low control should be associated
Finally, different metaphors suggest different implications for how people go about
developing trust or respond to breaches of trust. A “trust as a decision” metaphor suggests that
trust forms on the basis of verifiable knowledge – suggesting a distanced measured approach
to relationships. A breach of trust here is likely to be fatal as it reflects a fatal flaw in the
trustee that is not easily repaired. The “trust as performance” metaphor on the other hand
emphasizes the involvement of the trustor in making trust happen. Breaches here are not fatal
but mendable. Finally the “trust as an uncontrollable force”-metaphor suggest a passive yet
7
accepting approach in which people accept their experience of trust in the situation yet do
In order to fully capitalize on the insight that people have different understandings of trust or
employ different metaphors for trust, we also need to understand where and when different
metaphors and trust-types come to the fore. What do different metaphors really signify and
how do they relate to social situations and contexts? Here we suggest that different metaphors
can be seen as adaptations to different social situations with different structural properties.
These metaphors with their corresponding cognitions and emotions reflect and address the
specific opportunities and challenges inherent in that situation (Rusbult & Van Lange, 2003).
Different trust-metaphors thus can be seen as adaptive in that they prepare and facilitate
metaphor prepare people to shape and in some cases transform where needed and possible, a
«trust as decision» metaphor suggests prudence and care in situations where a trustor is likely
to have little influence and where misplaced trust can have a large negative downside. A
people have little or no control or information and need to align themselves to that situation.
Situations have structural properties that can be divided into two main categories of
situational features that we suggest influence the activation of different trust-metaphors: The
first is value; to what extent do people see a potential for a constructive relationship with the
trustee? This is likely to reflect the perceived value of outcomes that can be accessed through
the relationship – some relationships are more attractive than others because other people
8
The second category of situational features, are features that influences a trustor expectation
that she can actually influence the views and motivations of a trustee and the situation
involve some area of common interests, that extends over time, provide the trustor with ample
information and that allows free communication between the partners thus suggest a greater
potential for influencing a trustee and a relationship than situations that involve little common
ground, are one-off encounters, provide the trustor with little or no information and provide
Situations in which relationships are seen as potentially valuable and in which the partners see
themselves as capable of influencing the other we suggest should be associated with the
as less valuable and/ or are seen as offering less room for influence then should be associated
with the activation of a «trust as decision» metaphor leaving the trustor with little other
options than simply observing and learning about a trustee as opposed to influencing.
The effect of the situation on the activation of different metaphors moreover is likely to be
Thus situations moderated by personality and culture influence the activation of trust-
metaphors that in turn influence peoples’ responses, strategies and experience of trust. The
relationship between situations and metaphors however is not one-directional. Metaphors also
influence peoples’ construal of social situations. Thus “the trust as performance”- metaphor
once activated is likely to influence how people see the trustee and social situations,
features (Sedikides & Skowronski, 1991). Finally, different metaphors we suggest guide
behavior that shape and align situations to the activated metaphor. A «trust as decision»-
9
metaphor emphasizing distance and objectivity is likely to produce behavior that helps
Structural
characteristics
(value, control)
Personality
Culture
References
Bandura, A. (1989). Human agency in social cognitive theory. American Psychologist, 44,
1175-1184.
Burke, C. S, Sims, D. E., Lazzara E. H., & Salas, E. (2007). Trust in leadership: A multi-level
10
Dirks, K. T., & Ferrin, D.L. (2001). The role of trust in organizational settings. Organization
Dirks, K. T., & Ferrin, D.L. (2002). Trust in leadership: Meta-analytic findings and
implications for research and practice. Journal of Applied Psychology. 87, 611-628.
Fulmer, C. A., & Gelfand, M. J. (2012). At what level (and in whom) we trust: Trust across
Hong, Y., Chiu, C., Dweck C. S., Lin, D. M., & Wan, W. (1999). Implicit theories,
attributions, and coping: A meaning system approach. Journal of Personality and Social
Kelley, H.H., Holmes, J. G., Kerr, N. L., Reis, H. T., Rusbult, C. E., Van Lange, P. A. M.,
Knee, C. R., Patrick, H., & Lonsbary, C. (2003). Implicit theories of relationships:
Orientations toward evaluation and cultivation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7,
41-55.
Lakoff, G., (1987). Women, Fire and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal About the
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (2003). Metaphors We Live By. Chicago: University of Chicago
Press.
11
Mayer, R. C., Davis, J. H., & Schoorman, F. D. (1995). An integrative model of
Robinson, M. D., Zabelina, D. L. Ode, S., & Moeller, S. K., (2008). The vertical nature of
Rousseau, D. M., Sitkin, S. B., Burt, R.S. & Camerer, C. (1998). Not so different after all: A
Rusbult, C. E., & Van Lange, P. a. M. (2003). Interdependence, interaction, and relationships.
Sedikides, C., & Skowronski, J. J., (1991). The law of cognitive structure activation.
Szerszynski, B. (1999). Risk and trust: The performative dimension. Environmental Values, 8:
239-252.
12