Gender Role Stereotyping and Career Aspirations
Gender Role Stereotyping and Career Aspirations
Gender Role Stereotyping and Career Aspirations
net/publication/232417499
CITATIONS READS
69 1,768
2 authors, including:
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Linda Raffaele Mendez on 24 May 2017.
Gender-Role Stereotyping
and Career Aspirations:
A Comparison of Gifted Early Adolescent Boys and Girls
G
iven the underrepresentation of women in high-sta-
tus, high-pay occupations (e.g., medicine, engineer- women in high-pay, high-status professions is related to the
ing, the natural sciences, law), there has been consid- continued gender-role stereotyping of careers. Children learn
erable interest in understanding how gifted young women from a young age that, for example, secretaries are female,
come to choose their vocations. Certainly, in the past, there was while business executives are male (Berk, 2000). Examples of
much less choice for women in the vocational domain. Factors men in positions of power and women in supportive roles
The Journal of Secondary Gifted Education, Vol. XIII, No. 3, Spring 2002, pp. 96–107. Copyright ©2002 Prufrock Press, P.O. Box 8813, Waco, TX 76714.
96 ▼ Spring 2002
Gender-Role Stereotyping
abound in children’s books, movies, television socially valued (Galambos, Almeida, & Petersen,
programming, and children’s actual life experi- 1990). Certainly, with regard to careers, those that
ences. Many researchers have noted that limited are the highest in earning potential and prestige
exposure to women in nontraditional careers may are male-dominated. Thus, it makes sense that tal-
limit the occupational aspirations of gifted girls ented young women would be more attracted to
who have the potential to pursue education lead- male dominated careers than their male counter-
ing to a prestigious career, but may not perceive parts would be to female-dominated careers.
it as being within their realm of options (see, in
particular, Kerr, 1995). Overview of the Current Study
Recent research examining differences in
career aspirations among gifted boys and girls is The primary purpose of the current investiga-
relatively limited. Those studies that do exist, tion was to further examine gen-
however, suggest that strong adherence to gen-
der-role stereotypes in career aspirations may be
der-role stereotyping in the
career aspirations of gifted boys
▼
diminishing among gifted girls. Leung, Conoley, and girls. The study focused on
and Scheel (1994) suggested that the “social and students in the early adolescent Examples of men in positions
cultural changes occurring the past two decades years because, according to
have gradually and successfully resulted in some Gottfredson’s (1981) theory of of power and women
changes in women’s attitudes about careers” (p. circumscription and compro-
302). For example, a 1988 study conducted by mise, by the time youngsters in supportive roles abound
Kerr and Colangelo found that business had reach early adolescence, they
replaced education as the top choice of college have ruled out a number of
majors for gifted girls. Additionally, Reis, potential occupations that they
in children’s books, movies,
Callahan, and Goldsmith (1996) found the top believe are inconsistent with their
four career choices for gifted early adolescent gender role, their social class, or television programming,
males and females to be identical (i.e., doctor, sci- their intellectual potential. Thus,
entist, lawyer, and business owner), although they the typical early adolescent is and children’s actual life
were ranked differently for boys and girls. likely to have narrowed his or her
It appears from the literature that the career options, but not actually experiences
increased similarity in the career aspirations of selected a career. This narrowing
gifted boys and girls is attributable to girls becom- process is important because,
ing more interested in male-dominated occupa- according to Gottfredson, once
tions, rather than vice versa. For example, Leung,
Conoley, and Scheel (1994), who retrospectively
certain types of careers have been
eliminated, they are not likely to
▼
examined the career aspirations of gifted high be considered in the future. The aim of this inves-
school juniors and seniors, found that, although tigation was to learn more about the types of
the boys in their study were more likely to have careers (in terms of sex-typing, education required,
considered only traditionally masculine occupa- and prestige level) that were still in the pool of
tions, girls had considered both feminine and options for gifted early adolescent boys and girls.
masculine career options. They found no differ- The measure of career aspirations that was select-
ences between gifted boys and girls in the pres- ed was designed to examine specifically those
tige level of career aspirations. Similarly, Dunnell options that were still being considered in con-
and Bakken (1991) found that 9th- and 12th- trast to those that had been ruled out.
grade gifted females were significantly more like- Three other variables with theoretical links to
ly than same-aged gifted males to choose occupa- both career aspirations and gender-role stereotyp-
tions that were nontraditional for their gender. ing also were examined: (1) gender-related person-
Consistent with this finding, it has been noted by ality attributes (i.e., self-perceptions of instrumen-
several researchers that there is greater pressure for tal and expressive traits); (2) attitudes toward the
boys than for girls to adhere to traditional gen- rights and roles of women, and; (3) achievement
der-role stereotyped behavior (e.g., Massad, motivation. The purpose of including these vari-
1981). This appears to be related to the fact that ables was to examine the broader context of gender-
masculine behaviors, preferences, and interests are related attitudes and attributes among the partici-
Spring 2002 ▼ 97
The Journal of Secondary Gifted Education
pants and to examine the relationships between 5. What are the relationships (examined sepa-
these variables and career aspirations. Previous rately for girls and boys) between the sex typ-
research with gifted high school girls (Fleming & ing, education level, and prestige level of
Hollinger, 1979) and with unselected high school career aspirations and (a) gender-related per-
and college students (Spence & Helmreich, 1978) sonality attributes, (b) attitudes toward the
has shown significant positive correlations between rights and roles of women, and (c) achieve-
educational and occupational aspirations and both ment motivation?
students’ self-perceptions of instrumental traits
(stereotypically male characteristics such as Method
assertiveness, confidence, and independence) and
achievement motivation. Expressive traits (stereo- Participants
typically female characteristics Participants were 227 students (132 girls, 95
▼ such as kindness, gentleness, and
understanding) and attitudes
boys) in grades 6–8 who had been selected to par-
ticipate in a program for gifted students. They
toward the rights and roles of ranged in age from 11 to 14, with a mean age of
In particular, there is greater women have shown weaker rela- 12.8 years. All students attended one of two
tionships with career aspirations schools in a suburban district just outside a large
(e.g., Raffaele Mendez, 2000; southwestern city. At the time of data collection,
acceptance of women Spence & Helmreich, 1978). No 166 girls and 196 boys were enrolled in the gift-
previous studies, however, have ed program in the two schools where data was col-
examined these relationships for lected. Thus, of the 362 students who were eligi-
pursuing male-dominated gifted early adolescent boys and ble to participate, 227 (63%) agreed to
girls. participate, received parental permission, were
The intention of present on the day of data collection, and provid-
careers and greater choice this study was to understand the ed complete data on all of the measures. The
nature of the relationships majority of participants (n = 199) were Caucasian.
between career aspirations and Minority groups were minimally represented with
in childcare options. each of the other variables (i.e., 28 minority participants in the sample. Two were
gender-related personality attrib- African American, 7 were Hispanic, 13 were
utes, achievement motivation, Asian, and 6 identified themselves as belonging
and attitudes toward the rights to another ethnic group. Most of the students
and roles of women) specifically were from families of middle to upper socioeco-
▼ for a group of gifted early adoles-
cent students. These relation-
nomic status.
To identify students for its gifted program,
ships were examined separately for boys and girls the district from which students were recruited
in order to determine if they differed by gender. used indices of intellectual ability, achievement,
motivation, and creativity. The specific measures
Research Questions used to identify students were the SRA
Five major research questions were addressed Achievement Series (the Educational Abilities
in this study. Score was used to assess ability), a student inter-
1. Do gifted early adolescent boys and girls dif- view, and the Scales for Rating the Behavioral
fer in the sex typing, education level, and Characteristics of Students (Learning and
prestige associated with the careers to which Motivation subscales only, Renzulli & Hartman,
they aspire? 1971). Students were required to earn 14 points
2. Do gifted early adolescent boys and girls dif- or greater on a matrix involving these different
fer with regard to gender-related personality components in order to be admitted into the pro-
attributes? gram. The only exception to this process occurred
3. Do gifted early adolescent boys and girls dif- for students who earned a score of 130 or greater
fer in their attitudes toward the rights and on the Educational Abilities Scale of the SRA and
roles of women? an SRA achievement composite score at or above
4. Do gifted early adolescent boys and girls dif- the 95th percentile. These students automatically
fer in achievement motivation? qualified for the program. Most students were
98 ▼ Spring 2002
Gender-Role Stereotyping
identified for the gifted program in the early ele- occupations included in the female-dominated,
mentary school years. neutral, and male-dominated categories were
based on percentages from 1992 data.
Measures Occupations listed on the OCL-R required
Parent questionnaire. A 17-item parent ques- varying levels of education (i.e., high school diplo-
tionnaire designed specifically for this study was ma, master’s degree) and were easily recognizable
used to gather information on ethnic background, to adolescents. In addition, occupations were
whether the child had been identified for a gifted selected to approximate the percentages of persons
or special education program in another school employed in each of Holland’s six work environ-
district, current household composition, educa- ments (i.e., Realistic, Investigative, Social,
tional levels of parents, occupations of parents Conventional, Enterprising, Artistic). Students
(which were used to calculate socioeconomic sta- were given instructions to check
tus and the degree to which the mother’s occupa-
tion would be considered traditional), and the age
either “Might Choose” or
“Would Not Choose” for each
▼
and gender of siblings. Occupational status scores occupation listed. For scoring
for mothers and fathers were calculated using the purposes, occupations were . . . strong adherence
Duncan Revised Socioeconomic Index of assigned the following values: 1
Occupational Status (Stevens & Featherman, = traditional (female dominat-
1981). The child’s socioeconomic status (SES) ed), 2 = neutral (neither male to gender-role stereotypes
score was recorded as the higher of the two par- nor female dominated), and 3 =
ents’ occupational status scores. The degree to nontraditional (male dominat-
which a mother’s occupation would be considered ed). Sex Type scores were calcu- in career aspirations
traditional was calculated using census informa- lated by summing the assigned
tion regarding the percentage of female workers values for all of the items
employed in various occupations (U.S. Bureau of checked “Might Choose” and may be diminishing
the Census, 1992). Occupations were coded as dividing by the number of items
follows: (a) traditional = 70% or more female checked “Might Choose.”
workers; (b) neutral = 30–69% female workers, Higher scores were associated among gifted girls.
and; (c) nontraditional = 29% or less female with more male-dominated
workers (Brooks, Holahan, & Galligan, 1985). career aspirations, while lower
Revised Occupational Checklist (OCL-R). The scores represented more female-
career aspirations measure used in this study was a dominated career aspirations.
revision of the Occupational Checklist (OCL;
Brooks, Holahan, & Galligan, 1985). The OCL,
The authors reported two-week,
test-retest reliability by item
▼
which utilized 1980 census data (U.S. agreement to be 85% for a group of 30 middle
Department of Commerce, 1980), measured stu- school girls and 86% for a group of 25 high
dent interest in careers that were traditional, neu- school girls (Brooks, Holahan, & Galligan, 1985).
tral, and nontraditional for women. The original The Education Level score was calculated by
OCL included 60 occupations: 20 traditionally first assigning each occupation one of the follow-
female occupations (defined as occupations in ing values: 1 = occupations generally requiring a
which 70% or more of workers in 1980 were high school degree or less; 2 = occupations gener-
female), 20 neutral (occupations in which ally requiring at least a college degree, but less
30–69% of workers in 1980 were female), and than a graduate degree; 3 = occupations generally
20 traditionally male (occupations in which 29% requiring at least a graduate degree. In the same
or less of the workers in 1980 were female). The manner as Sex Type was calculated, the Education
OCL was revised by the first author (OCL-R; Level score was calculated by summing the
Raffaele Mendez, 2000) based on 1992 data (U.S. assigned values for all the items checked “Might
Bureau of the Census, 1992). All other aspects of Choose” and then dividing by the number of
the measure, including number of items, direc- items checked “Might Choose.”
tions for completing the measure, and scoring, The Prestige score was calculated in the same
were the same as described by Brooks, Holahan, manner. Rankings of prestige were assigned to
and Galligan. The major difference was that the each of the occupations using the Duncan Revised
Spring 2002 ▼ 99
The Journal of Secondary Gifted Education
review of the fear of success literature, contained mother’s employment would be considered “tradi-
the original four items plus an additional eight tional,” sibling position, or living arrangement
items designed to assess the same construct as the (e.g., with both biological parents, with mother
original scale. Using a sample of 209 early adoles- and stepfather, etc.). Significant differences did
cent girls, Raffaele Mendez (2000) reported inter- emerge between groups, however, on mother’s
item reliability of .83 for the 12-item scale, which occupational status, F(1,188) = 5.04, p < .03.
compares favorably to reliability coefficients of Girls had mothers with higher occupational sta-
around .50 for the original 4-item scale (see tuses than boys (M = 40.93 for mothers of girls vs.
Adams, Priest, & Prince, 1985; Spence & M = 32.54 for mothers of boys) based on the
Helmreich, 1978). Duncan Revised Socioeconomic Index of
Occupational Status (Stevens & Featherman,
Data Collection 1981).
Table 2
Pearson Product Moment Correlations Between
the OCL-R and the PAQ, WOFO, and AWSA (by Gender)
Measure PAQ WOFO-R AWSA
Sex Type
Girls .37 -.02 .26 .45 .26 -.10 .13
Boys .02 -.35 -.14 .04 .08 -.13 -.03
Ed Level
Girls .19 .13 .29 .40 .18 -.09 .10
Boys -.02 .09 .14 .19 .00 -.15 .15
Prestige
Girls .18 .09 .32 .40 .13 -.07 .01
Boys -.04 .05 .17 .17 -.01 -.17 .16
Note. EPU = Expanded Personal Unconcern Scale; OCL-R = Occupational Checklist–Revised; PAQ = Personal Attributes Questionnaire; AWSA = Attitudes Toward
Women Scale for Adolescents; WOFO = Work and Family Orientation Questionnaire.
This may be because the student sees those oppo- value competition, while girls are more likely to
site-sex characteristics as more salient because they value connectedness and cooperation (e.g., Berk,
are different from what would be expected. Thus, 2000), although, with an effect size of .35, it was
a boy who has a strong sense of being kind and not a particularly strong finding.
understanding may be pulled toward a career in
the helping professions, whereas many girls would Achievement Motivation
be pulled toward those careers simply because and Career Aspirations
they are female dominated and consistent with The relationships that emerged between
expectations. achievement motivation and career aspirations
Consistent with previous work conducted by differed by gender. Results showed that girls who
Spence and Helmreich (1978), attitudes toward perceived themselves as harder working and more
the rights and roles of women showed low corre- internally motivated were more
lations with sex typing of career aspirations both
for boys (r = -.02) and girls (r = .13). This indi-
likely to aspire to careers that are
male dominated, require more
▼
cates that attitudes toward the rights and roles of education, and/or are higher in
women in society have little to do with the types prestige. In contrast, boys’ career Among girls, however,
of careers to which gifted early adolescents aspire. aspirations appeared largely
It might be hypothesized that this relationship unrelated to their self-perceived
would be stronger for girls than for boys given achievement motivation. This
those who aspire to the most
that girls with less liberal attitudes might choose finding may reflect a tendency
more traditional roles. In actuality, however, this for most gifted boys to aspire to prestigious careers appear
study showed that gifted girls were very liberal in prestigious, male-dominated
their attitudes (M = 2.63 on a scale of 0 to 3) with occupations requiring high levels
little variability in responses (SD = 0.28). Thus, of education regardless of how to be those who perceive
even if a relationship existed between attitudes strong they perceive their
toward women and career aspirations among girls, achievement motivation to be. themselves as higher
it would have been difficult to detect it in this Among girls, however, those
study because of the restriction of range among who aspire to the most presti-
girls’ scores. gious careers appear to be those in achievement motivation.
who perceive themselves as high-
Achievement Motivation er in achievement motivation. It
Few differences emerged between boys and may be that among girls, high
girls in achievement motivation in this study. The
failure to find a significant difference on the
career aspirations are not neces-
sarily expected, and it is the
▼
Expanded Personal Unconcern scale (measuring hardest working girls who are most likely to aspire
lack of concern about the reactions of others to to nontraditional careers for women.
one’s success—or an absence of fear of success) is
notable in that many of those writing about gifted Conclusions
girls have posited that this population of girls is
particularly vulnerable to the perception that aca- Overall, results of this study show that gifted
demic and social success are incompatible (e.g., early adolescent boys continue to aspire to careers
Kerr, 1995; Reis, 1987). In this investigation, girls that are higher in education required and prestige
showed the same desire for hard work and meet- than their female counterparts. This appears to
ing one’s own internal standards of excellence as be due mainly to the fact that boys limit their
boys, but appeared no more likely than boys to aspirations to those careers that are male domi-
be concerned that this would negatively impact nated (and high in prestige), while girls express
their social standing. The only scale on which interest in a wider range of careers that includes
boys and girls differed was the Competitiveness both male- and female-dominated options. It is
scale, on which boys scored significantly higher notable that girls who perceive themselves as pos-
than girls, indicating a greater desire than girls to sessing higher levels of instrumental (or stereotyp-
outperform others. This finding is consistent with ically masculine) personality traits and are higher
research indicating that boys are more likely to in achievement motivation are more likely than
We are looking for papers that address the following topics or issues:
• the new core curriculum status for foreign language in the United States
in the 21st century;
• the differentiation of foreign language for gifted secondary students;
• the elements that make foreign language highly appropriate for gifted
children; and
• practical observations for teaching foreign language to gifted children.
Other significant topics welcome.