Ba Hughes S 2018
Ba Hughes S 2018
Ba Hughes S 2018
Sean Hughes
1727477
March 2018
Department of Psychology
Dublin Business School
1
Table of Contents
Acknowledgments................................................................................................................................... 3
Abstract ................................................................................................................................................... 4
1 Introduction ......................................................................................................................................... 5
1.1 Social Media Usage ....................................................................................................................... 8
1.2 Evaluating Subjective Emotional States (Mental Wellbeing) ...................................................... 10
1.3 Depression .................................................................................................................................. 10
1.4 Anxiety ........................................................................................................................................ 12
1.5 Stress ........................................................................................................................................... 14
1.6 Effects of Facebook Usage (Facebook Intensity Scale) ............................................................... 16
1.7 Rationale ..................................................................................................................................... 18
1.8 Hypothesis................................................................................................................................... 19
2 Methodology ...................................................................................................................................... 20
2.1 Participants ................................................................................................................................. 20
2.2 Inclusion Criteria ......................................................................................................................... 20
2.3 Exclusion Criteria......................................................................................................................... 20
2.4 Design and procedure ................................................................................................................. 21
2.5 Materials ..................................................................................................................................... 22
3 Results ................................................................................................................................................ 26
3.1 Introduction to results section .................................................................................................... 26
Table 1: Descriptive Statistics ....................................................................................................... 27
3.2 Descriptive Statistics ................................................................................................................... 28
Figure 1: Age range comparison with number of social media outlets used. .............................. 29
3.3 Hypothesis................................................................................................................................... 30
3.3.1 Hypothesis Model 1 - Depression ............................................................................................ 30
Table 2 Regression Coefficient Table for Depression ................................................................... 30
3.3.2 Hypothesis Model 2 – Anxiety ................................................................................................. 31
Table 3 Regression Coefficient for Anxiety ................................................................................... 31
3.3.4 Hypothesis Model 3 - Stress..................................................................................................... 32
Table 4 Regression coefficient table for Stress ............................................................................. 32
4 Discussion........................................................................................................................................... 33
4.1 Hypothesis Model 1 Depression ................................................................................................. 34
4.2 Hypothesis Model 2 Anxiety ....................................................................................................... 35
4.3 Hypothesis Model 3 Stress .......................................................................................................... 36
4.4 Strengths and Limitations ........................................................................................................... 37
4.5 Practical Implications of Findings ................................................................................................ 38
2
Acknowledgments
I would like to express my gratitude to my family and friends for their continued support. In
particular Adrian, Sinead and Grace for their ongoing friendship and support throughout the
entire course. I would also like to thank my supervisor Cathal O`Keefe for his mentorship
throughout this research project and the wider DBS team for all of their efforts. Finally I would
like to thank my Mam, Dad, Sister and wider family for their guidance and support throughout
the duration of the course.
4
Abstract
The aim of this exploratory study was to gain a deeper understanding of the relationship
between Age, Time spent using social media daily, Number of social media platforms used
daily, Facebook Intensity Scale and Mental Wellbeing (Depression, Anxiety and Stress). A
quantitative, correlational coefficient survey was implemented to gather the necessary data. A
convenience sample of 73 participants completed the survey which consisted of the Facebook
Intensity Scale and the Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scale. Analysis of the data displayed
that there was a positive, significant correlation between number social media platforms across
all three negative emotional states, Depression, Anxiety and Stress. There was no significant
correlation between Age, Time spent on social across all platforms, the Facebook Intensity
Scale and Mental Wellbeing (DASS). The practical implications of these findings may involve
the introduction of an educational campaign in order to increase awareness of the potential
negative effects of social media.
5
1 Introduction
Social media is everywhere, for many, it is one of the first and last activities of the day.
It is where time is passed, statuses posted and friendships are made. It is a place to stay in touch
with friends, communicate with distant family and even find love, but is social media all
positive? A huge amount of research tells of a darker side to social media, one where constant
social comparisons are made, a place where happiness is dependent on the amount of likes or
shares one gets on a daily basis. O`Keefe and Clarke-Pearson (2011) define social media as;
“any Web site that allows social interaction is considered to be a social media site.” (O`Keefe
and Clarke-Pearson, 2011, pp.800). Social media comes on a number of platforms some of the
best known are; Facebook, Snapchat, Twitter and Instagram. One of the commonalities these
platforms share is an approval system where friends may “Like,” “retweet”, “comment” or
“share” your input into the network this may come in the form of a status, photo or video.
Social media is now a huge part of daily life for many people, particularly in western
countries; “Multi-platform use is on the rise: 52% of online adults now use two or more social
media sites, a significant increase from 2013, when it stood at 42% of internet users.” (M.
Duggan et al, 2015 pp.2). This may be due to the introduction of popular new social media
apps such as Snapchat, Instagram and Twitter. The mass appeal of social networks on the
Internet could be a cause for concern (Kuss & Griffiths, 2011). This mass attraction to social
media is a multi-factor appeal, the low cost, high stimulation means it is readily accessible to
anyone with a screen and internet connection. More than one billion people are active on
Facebook today, a social network that grew from a college campus in 2004, this mass growth
and interest in an online service has never been seen before. Due to this very fast influx and
growth, psychology has found itself in unchartered waters in terms of social media and its
effects either positive or negative. Today, psychologists do not know what a fifty year old
6
person who has been exposed to social media from birth looks like. The template for human
behaviour and interaction has changed drastically in the past twenty years due to social media,
train journeys, waiting areas, coffee shops even restaurants are in many cases no longer
opportunities for human interaction but rather a chance to tweet, like or post something on a
social media platform. It is due to this change of human behaviour that research into how this
effects our mental wellbeing must continue to evolve and remain up to date with the latest
trends in social media. Social media in the latest form has been described as “A group of
Internet-based applications that build on the ideological and technological foundations of Web
2.0, which allows the creation and exchange of user-generated content.” (Andreas Kaplan &
Michael Haenlein, 2012, pp. 104). Facebook and similar social media sights by its very nature
is designed to connect the individual with friends and family, as such it is a place where
individual want to feel valued and part of something larger. However, the way in which
Facebook and other social media platforms impact on self-esteem is disputed among research,
for example Mehdizadeh (2010) found a negative correlation between Facebook engagement
and self-esteem, however it must be noted that this study was limited by having subjectivity in
its coding and may have been better served by employing several ‘raters’ from varying age
(2010) no significant relationship between social media and their participants` self-esteem was
found. This conflict of results has become a norm amongst scholars and popular literature since
research began in the area of social media. Further research conducted by Valkenburg, Peter &
Shouten (2006) suggests that social media platforms like Facebook may actually serve to boost
self-esteem through positive recognition and feedback of posted content by friends online.
Correa, Hinsley and De Zuniga (2010) conducted research into personality types and their
relationships within social media usage, findings suggested that extraverted men and women
were both likely to be more frequent users of social media tools, while only the men with
7
greater degrees of emotional instability were more regular users. A large part of social media
is projecting a self-image that will receive a kind of social currency in the way of friends’
positive outward image is crucial to some social media users and can develop into what is
known as Public Self Consciousness (PSC), which is defined as; ‘‘A tendency to think about
those self-aspects that are matters of public display, qualities of the self from which impressions
are formed” (Scheier & Carver, 1985, p. 687). One study conducted by Lee, Moore, Park &
Park (2012) which sought to provide insight into the phenomena of social compensatory
friending on Facebook, and its relationship with self-esteem and PSC, the results demonstrated
that there is a negative association between self-esteem and number of Facebook friends, also
stating the moderating role PSC has in social compensatory friending. Social compensatory
friending, Valkenburg & Schouten, (2005) works on the premise that those with low self-
esteem in their offline self and who have low satisfaction with their offline relationships may
seek to compensate for this by developing wider networks of online relationships, hence the
term social compensatory friending. Interestingly, the number of Facebook friends was
significant only for Facebook users who were high in public self-consciousness.
O'Keeffe & Clarke-Pearson (2011) conducted research into social media usage in
teenagers. Due to their limited capacity for self-regulation and susceptibility to peer pressure,
adolescents are at some risk as they navigate and experiment with social media. It is important
to note that while the current study is only using participants of 18 and above it does recognise
that users far younger than the age of 18 have access to and regularly use social media sites.
8
There has been arguments made that social media is in fact addictive, some say that
because of its draw and seemingly pleasurable outcomes, that it can quickly develop into an
addiction. Kuss (2011) notes that induvial prone to egocentric behaviour may be at higher risk
to becoming addicted to social media. This is due to social media acting as a vehicle to project
themselves in a positive light to a wide audience of friends and followers which gives them an
emotional high. Kuss (2011) argues that this may lead to the cultivation and facilitation of
learning behaviours that drive the development of social media addiction. Social media
addiction, like substance addictions manifests in a number of ways and in varying degrees of
intensity. These include mood modification, salience, tolerance, conflict and relapse.
Understanding these motivators are key to understanding why some people continually use
social media even why it begins to negatively impact their lives. Sriwilai, K., &
Charoensukmongkol (2016) state that people who are highly addicted to social media tended
to have lower mindfulness levels and tended to use emotion-focused coping to deal with stress.
While it is unrealistic to say that all users of social media are addicted, it is worth noting that
these addictive variables are present and can impact on the duration and regularity in which we
use social media. It is in the platform provider’s interest to suck people in and get them to keep
coming back, much like a casino, they stand to make money by just having users logged on to
their site in the form of advertising. Social networking sites have grown and developed from
desktop applications to the refined mobile apps on smartphones. Prompts in the form of
messages, comment alerts and friend requests now arrive in real time making putting the phone
away a huge challenge for many people today, in particular younger generations. Some of the
social media platforms referred to in the current research includes the following; Facebook,
Twitter, Snapchat, Instagram, LinkedIn, it is important to note that all of these platforms are
9
both image and text driven and involve sharing often daily activities to friends and the general
public.
10
Anxiety and Stress Scale was used. This is a 21 item measure which is non diagnostic in nature,
however it is designed with the intention to measure to what degree symptoms of Depression,
1.3 Depression
Today, the prevalence of depression is at an all-time high affecting 350 million people
million Americans adults. Irish prevalence, according to the HSE is one in ten for males and
one in four for women It has significant cost to mental health expenditure yearly, the total
economic burden of MDD (Major Depressive Disorder) is estimated to be $210.5 billion per
year (Greenberg et al, 2015). According to Belmaker and Agam (2008) depression is related to
normal feelings of sadness however these feelings do not remit even when the cause of the
emotions dissipates and are often disproportionate to their original cause. Depression is a major
contributing factor to suicide rates and is linked to the development of other mental disorders
restlessness and irritability, insomnia and fatigue. Depression often presents with other
negative emotional states such as anxiety and stress. Chronic depression can heavily impact on
an individual’s ability to live their daily lives which creates a downward spiral of events leading
to further negative emotional states. The rise of depression to its stratospheric heights today
have run parallel to the rise of social media. It is difficult to ignore the similarities between the
contributing factors to depression and the side effects of social media, things like – social
isolation, low self-esteem difficulty sleeping. Kross et al (2013) conducted a cross sectional
11
study examining peoples Facebook usage and how prone they are to feelings of life
dissatisfaction. They found that the more one used Facebook over a two week period the more
their life satisfaction levels declined while this isn’t strictly speaking depression – it does tell
a story of negative emotional states which leaves a short step to depression. In a recent study
comparisons which in turn elevates the risk of negative self- appraisal. While this research
seeks to uncover a clearer understanding of the potential negative impact of social media
through all ages, it is important to note that the current research will be seeking input from
adults only. Similarly, in a correlational study between the number of strangers followed on
Instagram and depressive symptoms was found in a study conducted by Lup, Trub &
Rosenthal, (2015). Instagram similarly to snapchat is a primarily image based social media
platform which utilises the widespread availability of camera phones to update friends or
followers. Findings in this study also included how positive social comparisons was associated
1.4 Anxiety
Anxiety is something all people will experience at some stage in their lives and it is an
important part of human’s survival instinct. In the early stages of humanities existence anxiety
allowed human beings to survive dangerous encounters by engaging the “fight or flight”
anxiety becomes chronic and unrelenting it can begin to negatively impact overall mental
wellbeing, often spreading to other aspects of life other than the original source of the initial
anxiety. This can have a knock on effect on the individual’s day to day activities. Anxiety
disorders take multiple forms including social phobias, generalised anxiety disorder, post-
While there are no fixed anxiety prevalence figures for Ireland at present “It is estimated
that 1 in 9 individuals will suffer a primary anxiety disorder over their lifetime” (St. Patrick’s
mental health Service). Social media can affect anxiety particularly in young people through
the social comparisons made on social networking sites, for example comparing ones weekend
spent at home studying to a friend’s spent on a beach in the Mediterranean. This phenomena
commonly known as “Compare and Despair” is a source of self-consciousness that can quickly
term which stands for “Fear Of Missing Out” this is when an individual feels they have missed
out on an event or exciting content when they have not logged in to a social media site for a
period of time. It is this FOMO that makes users continually check and monitor the social
media traffic on their own and their friend’s pages. Graham D.L (2016) states in an article that
13
social media may cause young people anxiety by offering a platform that directly quantifies
their friendships, offers social comparisons and providing real time information about social
events that the individual is not part of, these factors come together to develop an unescapable
world of constant social anxiety that can manifest for extended periods of time.
14
1.5 Stress
Stress is a reactive mechanism that has developed in humans as a survival tool to allow
us to make quick decisions regarding immediate threats. However, experiencing long term
stress can lead to chronic illnesses both physical and mental. In order to understand how social
media might affect stress it is important to understand what stress entails. Stress, similarly to
anxiety is something most people will experience at multiple stages throughout their lives,
developed through the use of social media and there are a number of theories as to why social
media use may cause stress. First is the constant highlight reel effect seen on friends Facebook
pages, this is apparent when one might look at a friend’s page or posts and only see the best
moments in his or her life – creating a pressure to do more with their life or buy a nicer car.
Another possibility for stress presenting in social media usage is pressure to portray the “ideal
self” this pressure may be conscious or unconscious be either way it can pressurise people into
getting on the latest trends with music, fashion, cars and even homes. These items can also
cause large financial strain on the individual leading to further stress. In a two part study
conducted by Duggan & Ellison (2014) interested with social comparisons made on Facebook
and the correlation of this phenomena and depression found that both studies provided strong
evidence to support the notion that social media negatively impacted its users emotional state
having been logged in for extended periods of time because they felt badly about themselves
when comparing to others` on their Facebook page. Another cause for concern around social
media and stress is how engaging in social media at bed time can cause people to stay up later
than they normally would. Due to a “blue light” emitted from laptop and smart phone screens,
this results in less sleep. Minkel et al (2012) conducted a study to examine the effects sleep
deprivation has on stress levels and found that those whom had less sleep experienced higher
15
levels of stress in a test that those with a healthy amount of sleep. This may be another
Facebook was one of the founding social networking sites has many popular features and
is held in high regard among many social media users, features such as member profiles that
contain personal information, displays friends list. Photo sharing which provides quick and
easy updates on whoever’s page one chooses. This coincided with tagging features and the
ability to comment on others friends` content makes for enjoyable experiences, Facebook one
of the most popular social networking site (SNS), with 96% of students reporting use of the
site (Smith & Caruso, 2010). Concerns for Facebooks impact on mental health of young people
in particular has been raised by researchers, one concern is the sites ability to promote the “ideal
self-image” this can include anything from altering photos to unrealistic self-promoting which
can have a lasting impact on interpersonal relationships with others. A study conducted by
Steers, Wickham, & Acitelli (2014) supported the notion that the long one spends using
Facebook the greater the likelihood that they will engage in spontaneous upward social
comparison and as a result experience negative emotional states regularly and thus increasing
their risk to depression. Within this research they state a number of contributing factors to this
experience which include; high status individuals only sharing the best parts of their lives (self-
enhancing which creates the highlight reel effect mentioned earlier, they even found a negative
effect on wellbeing between downward social comparisons and the individual, stating it may
(2015) found that Facebook use on its own does not directly lead to depression, in fact they
produced findings stating that Facebook may actually have the opposite effect, lessening the
loneliness. While this study offers an interesting alternative view to Facebook and social media
in general the study was not without limitations one of which was the non-generalizable nature
of their population sample which was drawn from one class of college students studying
17
introductory journalism. Facebook as a social media platform and the Facebook Intensity Scale
will be used as a representative service for other forms of social media the participants may be
involved in, the current research will generalise the Facebook Intensity Scale results across
other social media platforms in order to understand the individuals relationships to social media
services.
18
1.7 Rationale
While some research has examined the effects of social media on mental health in the
past, still little is known in regard to how using social media on a regular basis affects mental
health. Social media has only come into fruition on a mass scale in the past ten years and
continuous research is needed to monitor the affect it has on its users. The current research
aims to develop our understanding of the relationships with social media sites and the potential
negative emotional affect it has on its users. It seeks to improve understanding by researching
new comparisons that have not yet been compared, looking for correlations between variables
like age, total social media use, likelihood of developing depressive, anxious and stressful
symptoms along with how many social media platforms they use daily. By comparing these
variables the current research can make new approximations on how to combat the potential
negative affects social media has on the population today. Not all research has stated that the
social media experience is a bad one, in fact (2007) found that Facebook usage was found to
interact with measures of psychological well-being, possibly providing greater benefits for
users experiencing low self-esteem and low life satisfaction. (B. Ellison, Charles Steinfield &
1.8 Hypothesis
Hypothesis model 1
That Age, Hours spent on S/M per week, Number of S/M outlets used and Facebook
Hypothesis model 2
That Age, Hours spent on S/M per week, Number of S/M outlets used and Facebook
Hypothesis model 3
Finally that Age, Hours spent on S/M per week, Number of S/M outlets used and
2 Methodology
2.1 Participants
The target population for the current study was both males and females of wide age range
that use social media to varying degrees. In order to access the sample, a convenience sampling
method was used in conjunction with the snowballing effect through online questionnaires
posted on social media and sent out via email. The participants were then prompted to volunteer
to participate by checking the consent box and begin the questionnaire. The questionnaire
sought to develop an understanding of the relationship between mental health and social media
usage. No incentives or rewards were given to participants on completion of the study. The
study was given approval by the Dublin Business School Psychology Research Ethics
Committee and all ethical principles in the Code of Professional Ethics were adhered to
The collected sample consisted of seventy three participants that met the inclusion
criteria, the majority of the respondents were between the ages of 18-30 (52.1%). With the
other participants making up the 30-50+ (47.9%) age range. Interestingly 61.6% participants
stated that they use three or more social media platforms on a daily basis. 65.7% of participants
stating they use various social media for an hour or more per day. With 26% stating they spend
This study employed a questionnaire based study using quantitative, between sample
design that sought to investigate the relationship between time spent on social media daily,
amount of social media platforms used daily, Facebook intensity scale and age as independent
variables. While employing the DASS 21, using Depression, Anxiety and Stress scales as
dependant variables.
In order to gain access to the sample, an online questionnaire was developed with a link
that was then posted onto Facebook and also emailed out to various acquaintances which in
turn was distributed further allowing the snowballing method to take effect. All private group
pages on Facebook where the online survey was posted had permission prior to posting. All
eligible and willing participants were presented with a short message prior to the briefing page
explaining briefly what the nature of the study is and what it is seeking to investigate. Within
the briefing page participants were informed of their right to withdraw and also given some
information regarding support services in case any content of the questionnaire raised difficult
feelings. Participants were also given the researchers email address should they have any
questions before or after completion of the survey. All participants were administered the
2.5 Materials
The questionnaire used in this study consists of four sections, each section serving a
their rights and what the study concerns and another section to administer the Facebook
Before commencement of the survey a short briefing sheet is provided (see Appendix A),
containing information regarding the nature of the questionnaire is displayed along with contact
information of the researcher so that they may make contact if they have any questions. As well
as this, various personal support services numbers which are in place to allow the participant
to seek help should the content of the questionnaire raise any difficult feelings. At the bottom
of this page there is a “continue to survey” button which will bring them into section 2.
The first section of questions seek to ascertain key demographic information as well as
information regarding each participants` consent, age eligibility and age range. Participants
were provided the following options for age range (18–25yrs, 26–30yrs, 31–35yrs, 36-40yrs,
41-50yrs, and 50+ yrs). (See Appendix A). Following this the DASS 21 (Lovibond &
Lovibond, 1995) item questionnaire is administered, this is the condensed version which uses
seven items for each variable. The participants answer twenty one questions relating to their
overall mental wellbeing. The DASS 21, Lovibond & Lovibond (1995) consisted of three main
criteria; Depression, Anxiety and Stress. Participants were prompted to read the following
instructions before ‘Read each statement and circle a number 0, 1, 2 or 3 which indicates how
23
much the statement applies to you recently. There are no right or wrong answers. Do not spend
too much time on any statement’. The various scales include statements like; “I was unable to
become enthusiastic about anything.” Participants were then asked to give an opinion on how
they felt about each statement on a Likert scale of zero ‘Did not apply to me at all’, and five
‘almost always’. The depression scale is in place to estimate how prone individual is to
hopelessness, self-depreciation, anhedonia, and lack of interest in life. The Anxiety scale
provides insight into an individual’s situational anxiety, their subjective experience of anxious
affect and their proneness to autonomic arousal. Finally the stress scale is in place to detect
how prone an individual is to becoming nervous, over reactive, having difficulties relaxing and
being irritable or easily upset (See Appendix B for further details on section 2). The DASS
scoring system is slightly unconventional in that it is a shorted version of the larger DASS 21
questionnaire, therefore the scores must be multiplied by 2. The DASS questionnaire does not
produce a singular total score but rather multiple scores across the three scales. Comparisons
can be made when the data from the questionnaire is transferred onto a profile sheet where
severity rankings are given dependant on the individual’s responses. This questionnaire
possesses adequate construct validity, with reliability of the scales being .88 for Depression,
.82 for Anxiety, .90 for Stress, and .93 for the Total scale (Henry & Crawford, 2005)
24
Having completed section one the participant will be presented with a continue button
which will prompt them into the second part of the questionnaire which involves the Facebook
Intensity Scale. This gathers information about the overall feelings each participant has towards
Facebook, and how much time they spend using the site. The Facebook Intensity scale (Ellison
et al., 2007) was employed in the current research in order to measure the participants`
Facebook usage, emotional connectedness to the site and its integration into their daily
activities. The measure consists of eight items (See Appendix C). The first six items were rated
on a 5-point Likert scale, one item was rated on a 5-point ordinal scale and one item was rated
on a 10-point ordinal scale. Responses to the 5-point Likert scale questions ranged from
“strongly disagree” to “strongly agree”. These items included statements like “I am proud to
tell people I am on Facebook”. The 5-point ordinal scale was used to measure how much time
individuals spend actively engaging with Facebook daily. Responses to these questions were
comprised of “0-29 minutes”, “30-59 minutes”, “60-89 minutes”, “90-119 minutes” and “120
minutes or more”. The 10-point ordinal scale was used to measure the number of Facebook
friends each participant has. Responses for this question ranged from “10 or less” to “more
than 400”. After the 10-point ordinal scale has been transformed into a 5-point scale, the total
score was computed by averaging the responses of all 8 items. The total scores could range
from 1 to 5, with higher scores reflecting a greater intensity of Facebook use. The scale has
been found to have high internal consistency with the result of .852 for Cronbach`s alpha.
25
On the final page of the survey participants were presented with a short message from
the researcher, thanking them for their participation and reiterating the personal support
3 Results
A quantitative quasi experimental correlational design was used for the current study.
Hours spent on social media daily, number of social media outlets used and results from the
Facebook intensity scale were used as predictor variables and the criterion variable is the DASS
21 subscale. This would be employed using three hypothesis models, time spent on social
media daily, amount of social media platforms used daily, Facebook intensity scale total and
age compared to Depression, Anxiety and Stress scores respectively. Data from the respondents
were entered into the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) computer program version
21.0, and an alpha of .05 was set for determining statistical significance. In addition to a variety
of summary statistical tests, the researcher used a multiple regression to predict the impact of
the predictor variables on the criterion variable and a partial correlation to explore relationships
within the data. The present investigation obtained a sample size of seventy three participants
all of which were recruited via social media and email. The majority of respondents were
between the ages of 18-25 accounting for 37.8% of completed surveys. With the 50+ category
Variable M SD N a
Scale
Standard deviations of the Depression – DASS, Anxiety – DASS, Stress – DASS and the
All statistics were computed on SPSS 22 for Windows. The total number of respondents
was (N=73) of which the majority of respondents were between the ages of 18-25 accounting
for 37.8% of completed surveys. With regards to the daily social media usage categories,
25.7% stated that the spend 120 minutes or more per day on social media across all platforms.
With 35.2% of the combined responses reporting that they spend less than an hour per day
across all platforms. 16.2% of respondents reported that they use 5 or more social media
platforms per day with the majority of participants reporting they use 3 platforms daily, 25.7%.
This is displayed in figure 1 below in a comparison with age ranges. Participants were asked
to comment on, as part of the Facebook Intensity Scale, the following statement “Facebook is
part of my every day activity” to which 32.4% of participants strongly agreed and 40.5% agreed
to, with only 5.4% responding to strongly disagreeing. Participants were asked how many total
Facebook friends they have, 36.5% of participants stated they have 400 or more friends. With
12.2% of participants stating they have 100 or less friends on Facebook. Interestingly the mean
score of Stress was highest out of all the DASS scores (M=15.75) that the other subscales in
Figure 1: Age range comparison with number of social media outlets used.
30
3.3 Hypothesis
In order to test all three hypothesis models a multiple regression was the best suited, Field
(2009) states that a multiple regression analysis allows us to predict future outcomes based on
values of predictive variables. The current research used this as a means of gathering data
without threats to reliability that can often occur with other data collection forms (Suskie,
1996).
A multiple regression was used to test whether age, number of social media outlets used
daily, Time spent using social media daily (across all platforms) and the Facebook intensity
scale scores were predictors of depression scores. The results of the regression indicated that 4
predictors explained 23.4% of the variance (R2 = .23 F(4,68)=6.51, P=.000). It was found that
amount of social media platforms used daily significantly predicted depression scores (Beta =
.27, P = .035, 95% CI = .12, 3.34). Therefore the null can be rejected for this hypothesis.
A multiple regression was used to test whether age, number of social media outlets used
daily, Time spent using social media daily (across all platforms) and the Facebook intensity
scale scores were predictors of anxiety scores. The results of the regression indicated that two
predictors explained 20% of the variance (R2 = .20 F(4,68)=5.47, P=.001). It was found that
amount of social media platforms used significantly predicted anxiety scores (Beta = .31, P =
.017, 95% CI = .35, 3.47). Therefore the null can be rejected for this hypothesis.
A multiple regression was used to test whether age, number of social media outlets used
daily, Time spent using social media daily (across all platforms) and the Facebook intensity
scale scores were predictors of stress scores across the sample collected (N=73). The results of
the regression indicated that 4 predictors explained 16.1% of the variance (R2 = .16,
F(4,68)=4.46, P=.003). It was found that amount of social media platforms used daily
significantly predicted stress scores (Beta = .35, P = .009, 95% CI = .56, 3.7). Therefore the
4 Discussion
The aim of the current research is to develop the current understanding of social media
and its potential negative effects on the human emotional state, depression anxiety and stress,
thus expanding on today’s current literature. This study has been conducted using three models
of testing, comparing the variables of age, Facebook Intensity Scale scores, how many social
media outlets an individual uses on a daily basis and how much time total they spend using
various social media platforms against each subscale of the DASS21 respectively. There are a
total of three hypothesis that will be examined and discussed below in relation to their results.
This chapter will discuss these findings the limitations of the research and finally the future
Throughout the three hypothesis models used in this study one common variable was
found to be a significant correlation of symptoms of; Depression, Anxiety and Stress. Number
of social media outlets used was significant across all three subscales with the strongest
correlation being between number of social media outlets used and anxiety scores on the
DASS21 subscale.
34
This hypothesis sought to find a correlation between, Age, Hours spent on S/M per week,
Number of S/M outlets used and Facebook Intensity Scale score will significantly predict
Depression Scores. The current study found that the number of social media outlets one used
daily has a significant, positive correlation with depression scores on the DASS21 subscale.
There are a number of reasons as to why this finding may have occurred. High number of social
media outlets correlating with high depression scores was expected prior to the current research
being carried out. This hypothesis was developed due to past research being conflicting as
regards to social media potentially causing negative emotional states. This significance might
be explained with research conducted by Blease (2015) which states that Facebook offers an
which in turn elevates the risk of negative self- appraisal, which over prolonged periods of time
increases the individual’s likelihood of developing depression. While this research focuses on
Facebook only, it is clear from research displayed that these side effects of Facebook are
scalable to all social media referenced in this research. This research is in accordance with
previous research by Blease (2015) which found prolonged exposure to Facebook increased
This hypothesis sought to find a correlation between, That Age, Hours spent on S/M per
week, Number of S/M outlets used and Facebook Intensity Scale score will significantly predict
Stress Score. The current study produced a significant finding which included the number of
social media outlets used had a significant, positive correlation with anxiety scores on the
DASS21 subscale. Previous literature Graham D.L (2016) implicated social media in increased
anxiety levels due to users’ tendencies to quantify relationships, gain real-time access of social
events that the individual is not part of which can potentially result in prolonged social anxiety.
This significant finding falls in accordance with prior literature in that it supports the basis that
using more social media platforms correlates with anxiety scores. These results may be due to
excessive use of multiple platforms may contribute to greater levels of FOMO (Fear Of Missing
Out), which may be a contributor to higher levels of social anxiety. The current research back
up evidence found in other studies for example; Elhai, Levine, Dvorak & Hall, (2016)
conducted research seeking to understand problematic smartphone us and its association with
depression and anxiety, they found that variables like FOMO account for high levels of anxiety
Some previous research displayed evidence that less REM sleep resulted in a greater
susceptibility to stress. This previous research was imperative to a stress variable being
included in this study as social media screens tend to be the first and last thing users look at
before and after sleep. This hypothesis sought to find a correlation between, That Age, Hours
spent on S/M per week, Number of S/M outlets used and Facebook Intensity Scale score will
significantly predict Stress Score. The current study produced a significant finding which
included how; the number of social media outlets used daily has a significant, positive
correlation with stress scores on the DASS21 subscale, and this interesting finding may have a
number of contributing factors to the dependant variable. These findings may be due to
participants experiencing a lack of proper sleep due to social media at bed time particularly if
they have a high number of social media platforms to engage with before they go to sleep. This
act could be resulting in increased stress levels which is in line with Minkel et al`s (2012)
research into stress and sleep deprivation. An alternative explanation for these findings may be
having a higher number of social media apps on ones smartphone is likely to increase the
number of “push notifications” generated by the apps which may in turn lead to a feeling of
There are a number of strengths within this study which include an under-researched Irish
population which this study may serve as a benchmark for future research. The sample size,
while small offered a varied age range and social media usage range which strengthened the
studies validity. There are a number of limitations in regards to this present study. Firstly, the
sample size of the current research (N=73) is not a representative of the wider social media
using population. Future research may incorporate a larger representative sample in order to
attain a more generalizable sample. Scarcity of time and resources limited this study to the
researcher’s wider network of friends and colleagues, while efforts were made to reach wider
audiences limited time restricted this, meaning the sample taken is not ideal. However, future
research may expand this investigation to a wider and more diverse audience. The present study
limited to choose an answer on a Likert scale which may not accurately reflect their thoughts,
thus refining their answers overall. Future research might investigate these areas with
establishing a better layout, allowing for a greater disparity of participants responses. Other
limitations include the DASS21 scale, particularly with the Depression and Anxiety subsets,
these two variables are heavily interlinked and overlap to varying degrees which may present
issues when trying to use both variables against the same independent variables. The current
research did not account for gender disparity which may have been skewed to a particular
gender. Future research might include a gender indication to ensure a balanced gender
variation. Finally the last notable limitation of the current research is the fact that there may
be other factors that may have influenced participants’ mental health that could not have been
This research found that individuals that use a higher number of social media platforms
on a daily basis are more likely to score higher for Depression Anxiety and Stress scores in the
DASS 21. Some practical implications of this research might include the introduction of some
tools of awareness in order to offset any potential risk of social media creating negative
emotional states. As seen in previous research conducted by Lenhart, Purcell, Smith & Zickuhr
(2010) over than 70% of adolescents use SNSs, most common of which is Facebook the
younger population tend to have higher engagement rates with social media and, according to
the current and past research are at higher risk of negative emotional effect due to high usage.
It is this high risk population that would be targeted with various awareness campaigns as well
incorporated into the education system in Ireland and Europe. Other campaigns might include
the promotion of digital Sabbath which is a recent trend that involves people switching off all
screens for one day a week in order to relax and spend their time doing other activities.
39
Future research in this area should acquire a larger sample size as the total participants
of seventy three may restrict the ability generalise the findings of the study, also with a smaller
sample size some aspects of the demographics may become redundant. An increase in the
sample diversity may also be considered when conducting new research as it is imperative to
gather a wide sample in order to generalise these findings to the population. As well as this,
future research may seek a better understanding of possible correlations between such variables
as depression as used in this study and other negative emotional states that are less similar to
depression for example envy. Depending on the research, a wide range of variables could be
added to this study, for example testing narcissism or neuroticism along with life or job
satisfaction against the use of social networking sites. Further Research could also look at
dependency on social media for social approval in young adults, in an attempt to further
develop our understanding of social media and its draw to users. Another interesting direction
for future research might look at where the line can be drawn between intense social media use
and social media addiction and what variables contribute to the latter.
40
4.7 Conclusion
This study set out to examine the effects of social media use on mental wellbeing in
particular, its effects on depression anxiety and stress. This research is relevant due to the
disputed previous research on social media which has produced conflicting evidence in how
high use effects users. In some cases for instance research stated that social media can actually
increase self-esteem while others say it is detrimental to mental wellbeing due to various factors
including envy, self-consciousness and upward social comparisons. This paper found
significant evidence within a multiple regression to suggest that using a high number social
media platforms, for example Snapchat, Facebook, Instagram, LinkedIn and Twitter can affect
all three negative emotional states examined, depression anxiety and stress. While total time
spent on social media across all platforms, age and Facebook Intensity Scale did not show a
significant effect on the negative emotional states examined. This paper has served as a
benchmark finding which can later be compared with other research in relation to an Irish
context.
41
5 References
Baum, A. (1990). Stress, intrusive imagery, and chronic distress. Health psychology, 9(6), 653.
Belmaker, R. H., & Agam, G. (2008). Major depressive disorder. New England Journal of
Correa, T., Hinsley, A. W., & De Zuniga, H. G. (2010). Who interacts on the Web?: The
Elhai, J. D., Levine, J. C., Dvorak, R. D., & Hall, B. J. (2016). Fear of missing out, need for
touch, anxiety and depression are related to problematic smartphone use. Computers in
Ellison, N. B., Steinfield, C., & Lampe, C. (2007). The benefits of Facebook “friends:” Social
capital and college students’ use of online social network sites. Journal of Computer‐
Field, A. (2009). Discovering statistic using SPSS, 189 (3rd ed.). London: SAGE Publications
Ltd
Graham, D. L., 2016, Why we worry, Social Media, Loneliness, and Anxiety in Young People,
we-worry/201612/social-media-loneliness-and-anxiety-in-young-people
Greenberg, P. E., Fournier, A. A., Sisitsky, T., Pike, C. T., & Kessler, R. C. (2015). The
economic burden of adults with major depressive disorder in the United States (2005
Gomez, F. A. (2014). Guide to the Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scale. New South Wales:
Inner West Sydney Medicare Local. Retrieved October 17th 2015 from Kuss, D. J., &
Henry, J. D., & Crawford, J. R. (2005). The short‐ form version of the Depression Anxiety
Stress Scales (DASS‐ 21): Construct validity and normative data in a large non clinical
Kaplan, A. M., & Haenlein, M. (2012). Social media: back to the roots and back to the
Lee, J. E. R., Moore, D. C., Park, E. A., & Park, S. G. (2012). Who wants to be “friend-rich”?
Social compensatory friending on Facebook and the moderating role of public self-
Lenhart, A., Purcell, K., Smith, A., & Zickuhr, K. (2010). Social Media & Mobile Internet Use
among Teens and Young Adults. Millennials. Pew internet & American life project.
Lovibond, S.H & Lovibond, P.F. (1995). Manual for the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales.
Lup, K., Trub, L., & Rosenthal, L. (2015). Instagram# instasad?: exploring associations among
Maeve Duggan, Nicole B. Ellison, Cliff Lampe, Amanda Lenhar, Mary Madden (2015,
Http://Www.Pewinternet.Org/2015/01/09/Social-Media- Update-2014/
43
Minkel, J. D., Banks, S., Htaik, O., Moreta, M. C., Jones, C. W., McGlinchey, E. L., . . .
Dinges, D. F. (2012). Sleep deprivation and stressors: Evidence for elevated negative
affect in response to mild stressors when sleep deprived. Emotion, 12(5), 1015-1020.
O'Keeffe, G. S., & Clarke-Pearson, K. (2011). The impact of social media on children,
Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). The Self‐ Consciousness Scale: A Revised Version for
Use with General Populations1. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 15(8), 687-699.
Smith, S. D., Salaway, G., & Caruso, J. B. (2010). Key findings: the ECAR study of
educause.edu/Resources/ECARStudyofUndergraduateStuden/217333 [Accessed 10
October 2011].
https://www.cesphn.org.au/images/mental_health/Frequently_Used/Outcome_Tools/Dass21.pdf
Sriwilai, K., & Charoensukmongkol, P. (2016). Face it, don't Facebook it: impacts of social
Steers, M. L. N., Wickham, R. E., & Acitelli, L. K. (2014). Seeing everyone else's highlight
reels: How Facebook usage is linked to depressive symptoms. Journal of Social and
Institutional Resarch.
Tandoc, E. C., Ferrucci, P., & Duffy, M. (2015). Facebook use, envy, and depression among
146.
Valkenburg, P. M., Peter, J., &Schouten, A. P. (2006). Friend networking sites and their
Behaviour, 9, 584-590.
Valkenburg, P. M., Schouten, A. P., & Peter, J. (2005). Adolescents’ identity experiments on
6 Appendices
Appendix A
Welcome page (Online Survey)
My name is Sean Hughes, a fourth year Psychology student and I am conducting research as
part of my final year thesis. I want to find out the impact that social media has Depression
Anxiety and Stress. Filling out this survey below will allow me to develop an understanding
of the relationship between these variables and social media.
Your participation in this survey is voluntary and you are free to not participate. This
research is completely anonymous. However, it is my intention to ensure this experience is a
positive one. If you have any queries about the study beyond that provided here please do not
hesitate to contact me at *******@mydbs.ie. While the survey asks some questions that
might cause some minor negative feelings, it has been used widely in research.
If any of the questions do raise difficult feelings for you, contact information for support
services are included below. These are some help lines you contact if you are experiencing
any emotional, physical problems.
The Samaritans 42 (116123) and email: jo@samariatans.org
Teen line Ireland (1800 833 634)
Mental Health (01) 284 1166
Participation is completely voluntary and so you are not obliged to take part.
Participation is anonymous and confidential. As such - responses cannot be attributed
to any one participant. For this reason, it will not be possible to withdraw from
participation after the questionnaire has been collected.
The data taken from these online questionnaires will not be used for anything other than this
thesis and a short oral presentation. It is important that you understand that by completing and
submitting the questionnaire that you are consenting to participate in the study.
Tick the box below if you consent to participating in this study.
Please tick the box below if you are over the age of 18.
46
Appendix B
DASS-21
Please read each of the statements below and select a number (0, 1, 2 or 3) which
indicates how much the statement applied to you over the past week. There are no right
or wrong answers. Do not spend too much time on any statement.
1) I found it hard to wind down 0 1 2 3
2) I was aware of dryness of my mouth 0 1 2 3
3) I couldn't seem to experience any positive feeling at all 0 1 2 3
4) I experienced breathing difficulty (eg, excessively rapid breathing, breathlessness in the
absence of physical exertion) 0 1 2 3
5) I found it difficult to work up the initiative to do things 0 1 2 3
6) I tended to over-react to situations 0 1 2 3
7) I experienced trembling (eg, in the hands) 0 1 2 3
8) I felt that I was using a lot of nervous energy 0 1 2 3
9) I was worried about situations in which I might panic and make a fool of myself 0 1 2 3
10) I felt that I had nothing to look forward to 0 1 2 3
11) I found myself getting agitated 0 1 2 3
12) I found it difficult to relax 0 1 2 3
13) I felt down-hearted and blue 0 1 2 3
14) I was intolerant of anything that kept me from getting on with what I was doing 0 1 2 3
15) I felt I was close to panic 0 1 2 3
16) I was unable to become enthusiastic about anything 0 1 2 3
17) I felt I wasn't worth much as a person 0 1 2 3
18) I felt that I was rather touchy 0 1 2 3
19) I was aware of the action of my heart in the absence of physical exertion (eg, sense of
heart rate increase, heart missing a beat) 0 1 2 3
20) I felt scared without any good reason 0 1 2 3
21) I felt that life was meaningless 0 1 2 3
48
Appendix C
Facebook Intensity Scale
Below is a collection of statements about your feelings toward Facebook. Using the scale
provided please indicate how true each statement is of you.
1 – Strongly disagree
2 – Moderately disagree
3 – Neither agree nor disagree
4 – Moderately agree
5 – Strongly agree
Facebook is part of my everyday activity.
12345
I am proud to tell people I'm on Facebook.
12345
Facebook has become part of my daily routine.
12345
I feel out of touch when I haven't logged onto Facebook for a while.
12345
I feel I am part of the Facebook community.
12345
I would be sorry if Facebook shut down.
12345
7 251–300
8 301–350
9 351-400
10 more than 400
In the past week, on average, approximately how much time PER DAY have you spent
actively using Facebook?
1 0-29min
2 30-59 min
3 60-89 min
4 90-119 min
5 120 min or more
Appendix D
Thank you for your participation in this study, as stated before If any of the questions within
this study did raise difficult feelings for you, please contact the support services are included
below. These are some help lines you contact if you are experiencing any emotional, physical
problems.
The Samaritans (042)116123 and email: jo@samariatans.org
Teen line Ireland (1800 833 634)
Mental Health (01) 284 1166
Thank you,
Sean Hughes