Women Lawyers before the
Supreme Court of Canada
ERIN B. KAHENY University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee
JOHN J. SZMER University of North Carolina at Charlotte
TAMMY A. SARVER Benedictine University
American and Canadian scholars have found significant evidence of gender bias in the legal system, including the legal profession ~ABA, 2007;
CBA, 1993; Foot and Stager, 1990; Kay and Brockman, 2000; Lopez,
2008; Noonan et al., 2005; Rhode, 2002: 1001!. Interestingly, scholars
studying lawyer gender bias in the US have recently inquired as to
whether gender bias against women lawyers has crept into the decisional processes of the US Supreme Court ~USSCT!. Specifically, a study
conducted by Szmer and colleagues ~2010! indicates that women lawyers are not disadvantaged in finding success on case outcomes but does
suggest that conservative justices are less likely to side with parties represented by women attorneys. On the other hand, the study also concluded that the justices are more likely to side with the position advocated
by women lawyers in cases raising “women’s issues” ~Szmer et al.,
2010: 27!.
Acknowledgments: We wish to thank Frank Collucci and the anonymous reviewers
for CJPS for their comments and suggestions for improving the manuscript and Donald Songer for providing much of the data employed in the study. We also thank
Jacqueline Baldwin for providing a French translation of the abstract.
Erin B. Kaheny, Department of Political Science, University of Wisconsin, Milwaukee, PO Box 413, Milwaukee, WI 53201, phone: ~414! 229-6560, fax: ~414! 2295021, ekaheny@uwm.edu
John J. Szmer, Department of Political Science, University of North Carolina at Charlotte, 9201 University City Blvd., Charlotte, NC 28223, phone: ~704! 687-3941, fax:
~704! 687-3497, jjszmer@uncc.edu
Tammy A. Sarver, Department of Political Science, Benedictine University, 5700 College Road, Lisle, IL 60532, phone: ~630! 829-6473, fax: ~630! 829-6231, tsarver@
ben.edu
Canadian Journal of Political Science / Revue canadienne de science politique
44:1 (March/mars 2011) 83–109
doi:10.10170S000842391000106X
© 2011 Canadian Political Science Association ~l’Association canadienne de science politique!
and0et la Société québécoise de science politique
84
E.B. KAHENY, J.J. SZMER,
AND
T.A. SARVER
Given the similarities between the USSCT and the Supreme Court
of Canada ~SCC! and the similar history of gender inequities in the legal
professions of both countries, we test whether lawyer gender has the
same influence on the SCC. We examine the impact of female lawyers
on SCC decision making for several reasons. First, it is important to
assess whether the gender bias in the USSCT extends to a similarly situated common law high court. As Hausegger and Haynie ~2003: 635!
note, comparative analyses of this sort will help test the breadth of judicial decision-making theories. Second, due to the historically greater
proportion of women justices on the SCC, it becomes important to assess
whether a more gender-balanced decision-making body collectively views
women lawyers in a more favourable manner. Finally, it is crucial to
compare how women, as advocates, shape ~or fail to shape! the policies
made by both courts.
Gender Bias as Sex Discrimination in the Legal Profession
In approaching this topic, it is helpful to examine feminist scholarship
that has sought to explain the persistence of gender discrimination in
the legal profession and elsewhere. One school of thought suggests that
disparate treatment of women in the political and legal realms results
from the application of “gender schemas” ~Valian, 2005: 198!. “A
schema” is “a cognitive structure that enables an individual to make sense
of and impose structure on the many stimuli with which he or she is
bombarded in the course of daily living” ~Matland, 1994: 278, citing
Nisbett and Ross, 1980!. Specifically, a schema serves as “a mental representation of a category of objects, events, or persons and their typical
characteristics” ~Haslett et al., 1992: 57!. Thus, an individual’s “gender
schema” would involve those things deemed to be “‘the typical attributes’
of men and women” and it is through perceptions of such traits, that
gender schemas can perpetuate “gender stereotypes” ~Haslett et al., 1992:
58, citing Taylor and Crocker, 1981 and Fiske, 1982!.
As Valian ~1999! explains, individuals naturally assign different
attributes, physical and mental, to men and women. Men are viewed as
independent, strong, and rational; women are seen as communal, emotional, and nurturing. It is not hard to understand, therefore, how the
application of gender schemas might ultimately lead to the “overrating”
of men and the “underrating” of women ~Valian, 1999: 1045; Valian,
2005: 198!. Indeed, because individuals tend to form impressions about
men and women from extreme cases, an individual’s view of the sexes
can be quite polarized, super-intensifying schematic applications of gender attributes to the benefit of one and the detriment of the other ~Valian, 1999!.
Abstract. Recent work by Szmer, Sarver, and Kaheny ~2010! exploring US Supreme Court
decision making has suggested that lawyer gender might play a role in influencing judicial
voting behaviour. Specifically, while women lawyers were not revealed to have a more difficult time winning cases before the US Supreme Court, the study did suggest they face a tougher
challenge in gaining support from the more conservative justices on that bench. Here, we test
whether women lawyers face similar challenges before the SCC. Our findings do not reveal
any disadvantage for litigation teams with larger proportions of women and, in most instances,
such teams have an advantage. Specifically, in our model of civil rights and liberties votes,
litigation team gender had no bearing on individual SCC justice decisions. However, in a
pooled model of all issues combined and in separate models of criminal and economic votes,
SCC justices were more likely to side with litigation teams with larger proportions of women
lawyers.
Résumé. Une étude récente de Szmer, Sarver et Kaheny ~2010! explore la manière dont la
Cour suprême des États-Unis prend ses décisions, suggérant que le sexe des avocats pourrait
avoir une influence sur le comportement décisionnel des juges. Plus spécifiquement, bien que
les avocates n’aient pas plus de difficulté que leurs collègues masculins à gagner leurs procès à
la Cour suprême des États-Unis, l’étude suggère que leur plus grand défi est d’obtenir le soutien des juges plus traditionnels de cette cour. Dans le présent article, nous cherchons à déterminer si les avocates canadiennes font face à un défi semblable à la Cour suprême du Canada.
Les résultats de notre étude ne révèlent aucun désavantage pour les équipes d’avocats comprenant plus de femmes et dans la plupart des cas, ces équipes bénéficient même d’un avantage. Plus précisement, dans notre modèle décisionnel en matière de droits et libertés civiles, le
sexe des membres des équipes d’avocats n’avait aucune incidence sur les décisions individuelles des juges de la Cour suprême du Canada. Cependant, dans un modèle commun réunissant
tous les types de dossiers et dans des modèles séparés pour les décisions sur des dossiers criminels
et financiers, les juges de la Cour suprême du Canada étaient plus enclins à prendre parti pour
des équipes comportant une plus grande proportion d’avocates.
Gender schema theory has received empirical support in a variety
of political contexts, including studies of voting behaviour ~Sanbonmatsu, 2002! and the perceptions of women politicians ~Kahn, 1996!.
Most recently, the previously noted study of attorney effects on the USSCT
~Szmer et al., 2010: 28–29! suggested that one potential reason for its
findings is that justices apply “gender schemas” when assessing the credibility of the information presented by male and female attorneys.
Aside from this study, however, few scholars have examined whether
appellate judge perceptions of lawyer arguments are affected by the application of gender schemas. Instead, prior studies of gender and the American and Canadian judiciaries have focused either on the sex of judges
~for example, Brockman, 1993; McCormick and Job, 1993; Peresie, 2005;
Songer et al., 1994; Songer and Johnson, 2007; Wetstein and Ostberg,
2007!, or on trial attorney gender ~for example, Nelson, 2004!. In fact,
we could not identify a study to date that has assessed the potential influence of lawyer gender on judicial decision making in Canada at any level.
Building from prior scholarship on the USSCT ~Szmer et al., 2010!, we
are unique in providing an analysis of the effect of female lawyers on
SCC decision making.
86
E.B. KAHENY, J.J. SZMER,
AND
T.A. SARVER
Lawyer Capability on the USSCT and the SCC
An analysis of the effect of lawyer gender on SCC decision making must
begin with a review of scholarship on the role of lawyer capability on
the American and Canadian high courts. In the US, it is evident that
USSCT justices rely on lawyers for information ~Corley, 2008; Johnson, et al., 2006; McGuire, 1995!. Presumably, the ability to persuasively convey information to justices varies across lawyers. Moreover,
the credibility justices attach to lawyers also varies. Prior studies have
identified characteristics that reflect both lawyer credibility and persuasiveness. Most prominently, USSCT studies ~for example, McGuire, 1995!
consistently find that lawyers with more prior litigation experience are
more likely to influence the justices’ decisions. Similarly, there is evidence that former USSCT law clerks are more successful litigators
~McGuire, 2000!. Research has also shown that an array of lawyer characteristics including experience, clerkship and level of government
employment influenced Justice Blackmun’s informal assessments of the
lawyers’ performances during oral arguments and, in turn, those assessments partially explain the other justices’ votes ~Johnson et al., 2006!.
This line of research has been extended to the SCC. Flemming
~2004! models lawyer leave to appeal activity, finding that honorary
Queen’s Counsel ~QC! designation, appellate expertise and prior success on the merits in SCC litigation all help explain the degree to which
lawyers participate in leaves to appeal. Two SCC studies of the impact
of lawyer capability on decisions to grant leaves found little evidence
of the impact of lawyer experience or QC designation on the Court’s
decision ~ Flemming, 2004; Flemming and Krutz, 2002!. However, more
recently, Szmer and colleagues ~2007! demonstrate that both experience
and litigation team size affect the merits decisions of SCC justices, though
QC designation does not. Thus, it appears as if the lawyer also matters
in SCC litigation. What is less clear is whether the gender of the lawyer
matters.
As noted above, a recent USSCT study found that, although the sex
of the orally arguing lawyer or the gender composition of the larger litigation team was not related to case outcomes, conservative justices were
less likely to vote for litigants represented by women lawyers at oral arguments ~Szmer et al., 2010!. Moreover, justices were more likely to vote
in favour of women advocates ~in male versus female contests! in cases
raising women’s issues. These relationships subsisted after controlling for
an array of factors including attorney capability, the ideological direction of the attorney’s arguments and justice gender. While the study could
not identify the precise causal mechanism, the authors suggested the use
of gender schemas by political elites in a manner that could either hinder
or help women advocates.
Women Lawyers before the Supreme Court of Canada
87
Lawyer Gender Hypotheses
Women in the US and Canada have repeatedly experienced gender inequities in their efforts to fully participate in the legal profession. This might
also include women barristers litigating before the SCC. It is possible
that the justices apply gender schemas that affect how they weigh the
credibility of arguments presented by women. This theory, combined with
the finding that arguments by female attorneys are devalued by at least a
subset of the USSCT, a court similar in a number of ways to the SCC,
suggests that SCC justices might be less likely to vote in favour of parties represented by women.
However, there is at least one significant difference between the
USSCT and the SCC: the latter’s gender diversity. While only four women
have served on the USSCT, seven have served on the SCC. Presently, of
the nine members of the SCC, four are women, including Chief Justice
Beverley McLachlin. Perhaps the presence of more women on the SCC
bench indicates that gender bias may not affect judicial decision making
to the same extent there as it does in the American context.
Consequently, it is possible that this enhanced gender diversity has
mitigated any bias that might otherwise affect the evaluations of the legal
arguments of female lawyers. As Powell observes, “over time, being supervised by a woman or simply working with women seems to affect attitudes toward them” ~1993: 110!. This was seen, Powell notes, in a study
of firefighters. As men in the study gained more working experience with
females, not only did they hold their female peers in higher regard, but
they also held “less traditional ... attitudes toward women in general”
~Powell, 1993: 110, reviewing Craig and Jacobs, 1985!.
Moreover, Powell ~1993! mentions the importance of Kanter’s ~1977a;
1977b! work in understanding how the gender composition of a given
group might influence the use of gender stereotypes within that group.
Kanter ~1977a, 1977b: 966!, of course, discussed a number of key problems confronted by “token” members in groups. However, she also stressed
how the proportion of token members in a group can shape how such individuals are perceived. “It is easier,” Kanter argues, “to retain the generalization and distort the perception of the token” when group membership
is highly unbalanced ~Kanter, 1977b: 972!. This suggests that familiarity
with individuals who fall into a token group, along with the presence of
a larger proportion of tokens in a given group ~that is, greater diversity!,
might reduce negative stereotyping ~Powell, 1993: 112!.
In addition, although scholars have not directly tested whether greater
gender diversity on a court influences a judge’s perception of the arguments presented by female lawyers, some studies suggest that diversity
on the bench can contribute to differences in a judge’s voting behaviour.
Recent studies have found that male appellate judicial behaviour is affected
88
E.B. KAHENY, J.J. SZMER,
AND
T.A. SARVER
by the increased presence of female colleagues ~for example, Boyd et al.,
2010; Peresie, 2005!. Thus, depending on whether SCC voting trends follow those which describe more conservative members of the USSCT or
whether greater gender diversity has successfully mitigated negative stereotypes, we expect to see one of two possibilities in the present study:
H1: A greater proportion of women lawyers on the plaintiff/prosecution’s
team relative to the defendant’s team will have a negative or no effect on
the likelihood that a SCC justice will vote in favour of the plaintiff/
prosecution’s team.
In addition, one should weigh the likelihood that the influence of
lawyer gender will be contingent upon the gender of the justice assessing the lawyer’s argument. It is possible, for example, that women are
less likely to apply gender schemas in a negative fashion toward other
women. Therefore, if female lawyers do confront negative gender bias
when litigating before the SCC, this bias might be less evident or not
evident in the voting decisions of its female justices. Thus, we also test a
second hypothesis:
H2: A greater proportion of women lawyers on the plaintiff’s/prosecution’s
team relative to the defendant’s team will not negatively affect the likelihood that a female SCC justice will vote in favour of the plaintiff/
prosecution.
In their analysis of lawyer gender bias before the USSCT, Szmer
and colleagues ~2010! argued ~and found! a female lawyer’s prospects of
securing a favourable vote by a justice might be conditioned on the
justice’s ideology. For example, liberal justices might also tend to apply
gender schemas in ways that are not as disadvantageous toward women
and, thus, might also be more receptive to the nature of their legal arguments relative to their conservative colleagues.
This possibility leads to the generation of a third hypothesis:
H3: Conservative justices will be less likely to vote in favour of the
plaintiff/prosecution if it is represented by more women lawyers relative
to the defendant, whereas liberal justices will be inclined to offer further
support or, at the very minimum, will not be less likely to vote in favour
of the plaintiff/prosecution.
Beyond these issues, the impact of attorney gender might be conditioned upon the type of issue presented in the case by way of “issue
stereotypes” ~Dolan, 2004: 78!. Kahn ~1996!, for example, notes that individuals hold stereotypes regarding the competency of women in certain
issues, often called “women’s” or “female” issues. Presumably, to the
extent a judge shares these stereotypes, the judge will attach greater credibility to information presented by women in cases involving women’s
Women Lawyers before the Supreme Court of Canada
89
issues. Since we believe lawyer impact is a function of credibility, we
expect to find that women lawyers are more successful in cases involving
women’s issues.
H4: Representation by proportionally more women lawyers than their
opponents in cases involving women’s issues will positively affect the likelihood that a SCC justice will vote in favour of the plaintiff/prosecution.
How a justice responds to arguments presented by women lawyers
might also be moderated by panel gender diversity. Specifically, as the
proportion of women sitting with the judge on the panel increases, we
might expect the judge to be more receptive to arguments made by
women. Indeed, Carli reports “both men and women show a variety of
less sex-stereotyped behaviors when interacting with those of the opposite gender” ~1989: 573!. If the gender composition of a group can affect
differences among its participants, might a mixed-sex panel mitigate
negative gender-based perceptions of those women lawyers who argue
before it?
In the judicial scholarship, there is evidence that male jurists on panels with women adjust their behaviour. Gryski and colleagues ~1986!,
for example, find that the presence of just one female judge on a court
influences the male judges’ behaviour. More recently, Farhang and Wawro
~2004! find that the presence of one female judge on a US Courts of
Appeals panel can affect the decision making of male judges in employment discrimination cases. If judges’ voting behaviour is being influenced by the presence of a female on the bench, it is also possible that
such judges’ perceptions of female counsel may be altered in a positive
fashion. This behaviour, moreover, might not be restricted to male judges.
Since the SCC often hears cases in smaller five- and seven-judge panels,
there is significant variation in panel gender composition, thus providing
a good context to test the following hypothesis:
H5: As the proportion of female justices sitting with a justice on a panel
increases, a justice will be more likely to side with plaintiff/prosecutions
who are represented by proportionally more women lawyers than their
opponents.
Finally, it is possible that justices who are more likely to hire women
law clerks are more likely to respond favourably to arguments made by
women barristers. First, the decision to hire a woman clerk might reflect
the judge’s perception of the competency of women lawyers. However,
even if such hiring decisions are a function of other factors ~for example, political reasons!, experience over time with many highly competent female clerks could help alleviate gender stereotypes. As such, we
hypothesize:
90
E.B. KAHENY, J.J. SZMER,
AND
T.A. SARVER
H6: SCC justices with a higher proportion of former women law clerks
will be more likely to vote in favour of the plaintiff/prosecution when the
plaintiff/prosecution is represented by a larger proportion of women barristers than the defendant.
Data and Methods
The data for the analysis include all SCC appeals from 1988–2000.1 The
case information comes from the Canada Supreme Court Reports (CSCR).2
We incorporated in the model all non-reference cases from 1988–2000 in
which an ideological direction could be determined. The unit of analysis
is the justice’s vote in the case. In all, 1,393 of the 8,340 justices’ votes in
non-reference cases were excluded from the analysis because of missing
data.
Since we are concerned with the influence of lawyer gender on the
behaviour of individual justices, the dependent variable is the justice’s
decision for or against the plaintiff0prosecution. Additionally, while many
prior studies of lawyer capability model a judge’s vote for or against the
appellant ~for example, Johnson et al., 2006; McAtee and McGuire, 2007!,
we believe that is inappropriate in this context because it would not allow
us to control for the tendency of the SCC to affirm ~Songer, 2008!. This
is confounded by the slightly greater propensity for women lawyers to
represent the respondent ~on average, 15 per cent of the appellants’ lawyers were women, compared to 19 per cent of the respondents’ lawyers!.
As such, we follow the lead of Haire and colleagues ~1999!, who also
studied a court with a pronounced tendency to affirm ~the US Courts of
Appeals!. Thus, our dependent variable is dichotomous, coded ‘1’ if the
justice’s vote in the case supported the plaintiff0prosecution at trial and
‘0’ if the justice sided with the defendant.3 Because the dependent variable is dichotomous, we employed logistic regression, with standard errors
clustered by the justice to account for possible interdependent error terms
resulting from repeated votes by the same justices.
Independent variables
There are many alternative methods for measuring lawyer gender, including the gender of the orally arguing lawyers, the total number0proportion
of women lawyers representing a litigant and a dichotomous measure of
the presence or absence of at least one woman on the litigation team.
The first measure is likely the best. When more than one lawyer represents the litigant, the orally arguing lawyer clearly plays a significant
role. We do not have the same level of certainty regarding the role of the
other lawyers. Unfortunately, the SCC does not maintain records of the
lawyers who present oral arguments in prior cases nor is this information
Women Lawyers before the Supreme Court of Canada
91
in the CSCR. Court opinions, however, do list the names of the lawyers,
which we used to determine the lawyers’ genders. Thus, absent a better
alternative, we employ the proportion of women lawyers representing the
parties to assess lawyer gender. Given our interest in exploring the potential role of gender schemas in this analysis, our choice was to utilize a
measure that would be the most reasonable surrogate for the sex of the
orally arguing lawyer. While we cannot correlate the two variables, more
than half of the litigation teams in our sample were represented by only
one lawyer. As such, at least in those cases, the proportion of women
lawyers on the team will reflect whether the orally arguing lawyer was a
woman. Moreover, it is likely that this construct, more than the sex of
the orally arguing barrister alone, best captures the influence of a lawyer’s
sex on the factums.4 The actual variable, lawyer team gender, was constructed to account for the adversarial nature of lawyer influence, as well
as the nature of the dependent variable. As such, this measure is the difference in the proportion of women lawyers representing the plaintiff0
prosecution and the defendant. As discussed above, we expect a negative
or no relationship between this variable and the likelihood of a proplaintiff0prosecution vote.
Our analysis also tests for the likelihood that the impact of lawyer
gender on judicial decisions is conditional on the gender of the justices.
To test this, we include a variable that is the product of the lawyer team
gender variable and the justice gender variable, the latter of which is
coded ‘1’ if the justice is a woman and ‘0’ if the justice is a man. Consequently, we expect a positive coefficient on this multiplicative term.
We also posit that women have a more significant impact in cases
involving women’s issues. Prior studies have described women’s issues
as those that involve questions of equality and0or the private sphere—for
example, family and marriage ~Kahn, 1996; Segal, 2000!. As such, the
women’s issue variable is coded ‘1’ if the case involves one of the following types of issues: rape, equality, privacy, marriage, divorce or other
family related issues. To examine the potential conditional relationship
between lawyer gender and women’s issues, we constructed a multiplicative term, lawyer team gender * women’s issues. We expect to find a
positive coefficient on this term.
Since justices who hire more women law clerks might be less likely
to devalue arguments by women, we include the lawyer team gender *
proportion of women clerks multiplicative term. The proportion of women
clerks variable was constructed using a SCC clerk list; it is the proportion of women clerks employed by the justice in prior terms. We expect a
positive coefficient on the multiplicative term.
We also posit that the presence of women on the panel with the justice could influence his or her support for women lawyers. As such, we
include a lawyer team gender * panel gender multiplicative term. The
92
E.B. KAHENY, J.J. SZMER,
AND
T.A. SARVER
panel gender variable is the percentage of women justices sitting on the
panel with the justice casting the vote. We expect a positive coefficient
for the multiplicative term.
We also suggest the impact of lawyer gender might be conditioned
by justice ideology. As such, we incorporate a multiplicative term which
is the product of lawyer team gender and justice ideology. The ideology
measure was constructed by Ostberg and Wetstein ~2007! who, following the method employed by Segal and Cover ~1989! to estimate USSCT
justice ideology, used nomination-period editorials in a sample of nine
Canadian newspapers to estimate SCC justice preferences. Higher values of the ideology variable indicate increased liberalism and, thus, we
expect a positive coefficient for the multiplicative term.
Ideology is also an important control variable ~Ostberg and Wetstein, 1998, 2007!. Since the dependent variable is the justice’s vote for
or against the plaintiff0prosecution, the measure ~ justice/plaintiff ideological congruence! estimates the congruence of the justice’s preferences
~using the Ostberg and Wetstein scores! with the ideological direction of
the preferred outcome of the party that initiated the litigation at trial. If
the plaintiff0prosecution preferred a liberal outcome, the variable is the
justice’s Ostberg and Wetstein score ~where higher values indicate increasing liberalism!. If the plaintiff0prosecution preferred a conservative outcome, the variable is the negative of the justice’s ideology score; higher
values indicate increased congruence.
Many prior studies have used lawyer prior litigation experience to
explain the behaviour of the courts and0or judges serving on them ~see,
for example, Flemming, 2004; Haire et al., 1999; McGuire, 1995; Szmer
et al., 2007!. Furthermore, lawyers with more experience are more likely
to present quality oral arguments before the USSCT ~Johnson et al., 2006!,
and the justices are more likely to incorporate into their opinions language from the more experienced attorneys’ briefs ~Corley, 2008!. To construct the litigation experience variable, we first calculated the total
number of times each lawyer participated in a case litigated before the
SCC in the previous 10 terms. To reflect the entire litigation team ~and
because we could not identify the orally arguing lawyers!, we then took
the mean value for the lawyers representing the plaintiff0prosecution and
defendant. Because there is a diminishing marginal utility of prior litigation experience, we logarithmically transformed each mean.5 Due to the
adversarial nature of the judicial process, the actual litigation experience
variable is the difference between the values for the plaintiff0prosecution
and defendant. Positive scores should be associated with a greater likelihood of a pro-plaintiff0prosecution vote.
We included two variables which also reflect lawyer expertise via
prior clerkships with the SCC: plaintiff lawyer clerkship and defendant
lawyer clerkship.6 The SCC clerks, like their counterparts at the USSCT,
Women Lawyers before the Supreme Court of Canada
93
provide research assistance for a justice, where they develop an insider’s
understanding of the SCC’s processes. Of course, a prior clerkship likely
reflects more than expertise; it also reflects the scholastic achievements
of the clerk while in law school ~Western Law Career Services Office,
2005!. Given the former clerks’ expertise and superiour law school
achievements, we expect that lawyers with prior clerkships are more likely
to influence the justices. For the purposes of our analysis, the actual
concept of prior clerkship experience is operationalized as two separate
dummy variables, plaintiff lawyer clerkship and defendant lawyer clerkship. We assert the former will have a positive relationship with the
dependent variable while the latter will exhibit a negative relationship.
Contrary to conventional wisdom, it is possible that more lawyers
are a good thing—at least for the party they are representing. Larger litigation teams are advantageous because they can increase the time spent
on research, attack problems from additional perspectives and present
and critique mock oral arguments. In this respect, we might argue that
litigation team size is an important indicator of litigation advantage ~see
Galanter, 1974!. Indeed, prior studies ~see, for example, Szmer et al.,
2007! found significant relationships between litigation team size and
judicial decisions. The litigation team size variable is the difference in
the number of lawyers representing the plaintiff0prosecution and the defendant. We expect a positive coefficient.
All parties are not treated equally by the courts, and there is evidence that this inequity extends to the SCC ~McCormick, 1993; Szmer
et al., 2007!. As such, we include measures of party capability, versions
of which have been incorporated into several prior studies of common
law appellate courts ~for example, McCormick, 1993; Songer and Sheehan, 1992!. In the absence of direct measures ~for example, financial
resources; prior litigation experience!, these studies used the organizational characteristics of the parties to indirectly assess capability. Although
they utilized scales which order litigant categories based on assumptions
regarding experience and resources, since such scales impose a structure
that might not adequately capture litigant advantages before the Canadian high court, we include dummy variables to describe the category of
the plaintiff0prosecution and defendant types in our study: associations,7
businesses, local governments, provincial governments and the national
government. The reference ~excluded! dummy variables are individual
plaintiff and individual defendant. ~However, in our model examining only
criminal cases, where a natural person typically faces off against the government, we examine only such cases and, hence, do not include party
capability variables!. Here, it is on safe ground to posit that plaintiffs0
prosecutions who are not individuals are more likely to have greater
resources than plaintiffs0prosecutions who are individuals and thus we
expect positive and statistically significant coefficients on all of the
94
E.B. KAHENY, J.J. SZMER,
AND
T.A. SARVER
included plaintiff litigant dummy variables. On the flip side, we can safely
assume that defendants who are groups, businesses or governmental entities tend to have greater litigant resources than individual defendants.
Greater resources in the hands of defendants should translate into a lower
likelihood of plaintiff0prosecution success ~the dependent variable! and,
therefore, we expect negative coefficients on the included defendant party
capability variables.
Since SCC justices tend to vote to affirm the decision of the lower
court ~Songer, 2008!, all things being equal, a justice is more likely to
side with the plaintiff0prosecution if it is the respondent on appeal. To
account for this, we include the plaintiff appellant dummy variable ~‘1’
if the party that initiated the litigation at the trial level also initiated the
appeal, ‘0’ if they were the respondent in the case!. Given our theory, we
expect to find a negative coefficient.
Results
Because the findings may be conditional on the subject of the litigation,
we present our models across all issue areas combined ~with and without
hypothesized conditional relationships! and separately ~with hypothesized
conditional relationships, where applicable! across criminal, economic0
labour and civil rights0liberties cases. The results are presented in Table 1.
Clearly, the most interesting result across all models is the positively signed coefficient on the lawyer team gender variable; the variable is statistically significant across the two models involving all issue
areas combined ~models 1 and 2! and in the separate model of votes cast
in criminal cases ~model 3!. It is also significant, two-tailed, at the 0.10
level in the economic issues model ~model 4!. Therefore, plaintiffs who
employ a larger proportion of women on their litigation teams relative to
the proportion employed by defendants are more likely to receive a favourable vote by a Canadian justice, though this tendency is more pronounced
in the criminal case context. In the civil rights and liberties issue area,
on the other hand, the coefficient on the lawyer team gender variable
does not approach conventional levels of significance ~see model 5!. Thus,
in this area, we see neither an advantage nor a disadvantage for litigation
teams with a higher proportion of women lawyers. As noted above,
although we did not expect to find a strong, positive result with respect
to this variable, the lack of a negative relationship is not necessarily a
surprising finding and, in fact, is in line with expectations concerning
the role of gender diversity on this Court. Indeed, one possibility is that
the greater presence of female jurists on the SCC has minimized the negative application of gender schemas in interactions with female lawyers
and0or has mitigated other forms of gender bias in the assessment of
Women Lawyers before the Supreme Court of Canada
95
their legal arguments. In terms of the criminal issue area, however, we
are tapping something other than the mitigation of negative gender stereotypes, since teams with a greater proportion of women criminal lawyers relative to their opponents tend to have the upper hand.
The results also suggest that the influence of lawyer team gender is
not conditional on a justice’s gender. None of the models including the
relevant multiplicative term ~models 2–5! suggest a statistically significant relationship. Thus, women SCC justices are not more likely to support women counsel than their male counterparts. Moreover, there is little
evidence to suggest the influence of lawyer gender is conditioned on the
ideology of a SCC justice. Although the coefficient of the relevant multiplicative term is positive where included, it is statistically significant in
criminal cases ~model 3! only. In this context, the results suggest liberal
justices might be more likely than their conservative colleagues to support the position of the prosecution on appeal, if the prosecution consists
of a greater proportion of women lawyers.
Unlike previous USSCT studies, our results provide mixed support
for the notion that justices are more likely to side with women lawyers in
“women’s issue” cases. One area where this type of relationship does
seem apparent, however, is in criminal cases; parties with greater proportions of female lawyers have an even more pronounced advantage in criminal cases raising a women’s issue. On the other hand, in civil rights0
liberties cases, the coefficient on the lawyer team gender * women’s issues
multiplicative term is statistically significant and negative; thus, greater
numbers of female counsel do not translate into more success in persuading individual justices. Rather, we see the opposite tendency.8 It is important to note that these findings do not appear to be a function of the
tendency on the part of women counsel to represent the liberal position
before the SCC. Indeed, in the data used for this study, women lawyers,
overall, are more likely to represent the conservative side ~see appendix
at http:00pantherfile.uwm.edu0ekaheny0www0! and, in the women’s issues
cases we model, the difference is rather striking. On average, 21 per cent
of the lawyers on the liberal side are women, compared to 34 per cent on
the conservative side.
Moreover, we found no support for our hypothesis that a justice’s
inclination to hire female clerks positively affects the justice’s decision
to support teams with a higher proportion of female lawyers. As seen
across all models in which this conditional relationship was assessed
~models 2–5!, the coefficient on the associated multiplicative term is negative and, thus, is not in the hypothesized direction. Of course, the proportion of women sitting on a given panel in this dataset varies over time,
and this proportion, we argued above, could also influence an individual
SCC justice’s decision to support litigation teams with higher proportions of female counsel. In the Canadian context, however, there is mixed
96
TABLE 1
Logistic Regression Models of the Likelihood of a Pro-Plaintiff Vote by a SCC Justice, 1988–2000 ~Robust Standard
Errors Clustered on Judge in Parenthesis!
0.20~0.03!⫹
—
—
—
—
0.38~0.11!⫹
⫺0.08~0.06!
0.03~0.05!
⫺0.01~0.23!
⫺0.34~0.27!
—
0.12~0.07!*
0.28~0.13!*
0.11~0.04!**
0.03~0.06!
0.12~0.11!
0.63~0.13!⫹
0.03~0.02!
0.12~0.02!***
—
0.16~0.14!
⫺0.34~0.07!⫹
⫺1.31~0.21!⫹
0.38~0.13!**
0.04~0.09!
⫺1.48~0.19!***
⫺0.37~0.14!**
⫺0.49~0.11!***
0.02~0.06!
0.16~0.11!
0.64~0.13!⫹
0.03~0.02!
0.12~0.02!***
0.16~0.11!
0.16~0.14!
⫺0.35~0.07!⫹
⫺1.29~0.21!⫹
0.42~0.13!***
0.03~0.09!
⫺1.43~0.18!***
⫺0.36~0.15!**
⫺0.51~0.11!***
Model 3
CRIM
Model 4
ECON
Model 5
CIVRTS
0.59~0.34!
⫺0.28~0.25!
0.14~0.15!
—
⫺1.38~0.61!⫹
0.63~0.94!
0.34~0.39!
0.08~0.23!
⫺0.67~0.32!⫹
⫺0.56~1.85!
1.54~0.25!***
0.04~0.22!
⫺1.77~0.35!⫹
0.21~0.05!***
0.67~0.85!
⫺0.08~0.13!
⫺0.24~0.06!***
0.00~0.12!
0.91~0.19!⫹
0.05~0.02!*
0.18~0.03!***
0.20~0.19!
—
—
—
—
—
—
—
—
0.16~0.05!⫹
0.37~0.16!**
0.59~0.14!⫹
0.10~0.04!*
0.02~0.05!
⫺0.12~0.13!
0.10~0.23!
⫺0.49~0.08!⫹
⫺0.14~0.48!
0.93~0.19!***
⫺0.39~0.14!⫹
⫺1.47~0.31!***
⫺0.23~0.12!*
⫺0.08~0.20!
0.63~0.12!⫹
0.02~0.06!
0.10~0.02!***
2.23~0.92!**
⫺0.26~0.29!
⫺0.06~0.18!
0.12~0.19!
1.35~0.35!⫹
⫺0.12~0.07!
0.14~0.07!*
0.37~0.21!
0.63~0.21!**
0.74~0.16!***
⫺16.19~0.40!⫹
⫺0.18~0.20!
0.59~0.18!***
0.27~0.61!
1.71~0.53!⫹
1.15~0.46!⫹
T.A. SARVER
Lawyer Team Gender
Lawyer Team Gender * Justice Gender
Lawyer Team Gender * Justice Ideology
Lawyer Team Gender * Women’s Issue
Lawyer Team Gender * Proportion
Women Clerks
Lawyer Team Gender * Panel Gender
Justice0Plaintiff or Pros. Ideological
Congruence
Plaintiff0Prosecution Appellant
Plaintiff0Prosecution Lawyer Clerkship
Defendant Lawyer Clerkship
Litigation Team Size
Lawyer Experience
Justice Gender
Plaintiff Association
Plaintiff Business
Plaintiff Local Govt.
Plaintiff Prov’l Govt.
Plaintiff Nat’l Govt.
Defendant Association
Defendant Business
Defendant Local Govt.
Model 2
AND
Model 1
E.B. KAHENY, J.J. SZMER,
Independent Variable
⫺0.84~0.09!***
⫺0.99~0.09!***
—
—
—
—
0.65~0.09!***
6,958
64.6
0.082
⫺0.83~0.09!***
⫺0.98~0.09!***
⫺0.00~0.06!
⫺0.28~0.12!⫹
⫺0.04~0.38!
1.27~0.30!***
0.33~0.21!
6,958
65.0
0.093
—
—
⫺0.16~0.21!
⫺0.19~0.29!
⫺0.02~0.59!
1.50~0.37!***
0.31~0.32!
4,302
67.6
0.006
⫺0.97~0.18!***
⫺0.68~0.11!***
0.09~0.04!*
—
⫺0.43~0.41!
0.53~0.72!
0.49~0.27!
1,665
61.7
0.188
0.93~0.44!⫹
0.88~0.44!⫹
0.06~0.07!⫹
⫺0.37~0.22!
0.88~0.40!*
3.80~1.19!*
⫺2.58~0.56!***
664
64.5
0.269
p ⱕ 0.05 ⫽ *; p , 0.01 ⫽ **; p ⱕ 0.001 ⫽ ***; ⫹significant at p ⱕ 0.05 but sign of coefficient is opposite to that hypothesized.
~Tests are one-tailed except of those performed on the Justice Gender, Justice Ideology, Proportion of Women Clerks, and Women’s Issue variables as well as the
constant!.
Women Lawyers before the Supreme Court of Canada
Defendant Prov’l Govt.
Defendant Nat’l Govt.
Justice Ideology
Women’s Issue
Proportion Women Clerks
Panel Gender
Constant
Number of Observations
% Predicted Correctly
PRE
97
98
E.B. KAHENY, J.J. SZMER,
AND
T.A. SARVER
support for the proposition. In model 2 ~all issues combined! and model
3 ~votes cast in criminal cases!, the coefficient on the multiplicative term,
lawyer team gender * panel gender, suggests the possibility of this type
of conditional relationship. On the other hand, the multiplicative term
fails to reach significance in the civil rights model ~model 5! and, if
anything, suggests the opposite relationship in economics cases.
Next, we briefly discuss our controls, as they too reveal interesting
findings. First, we find a positive, statistically significant relationship
between the ideological congruence term and the dependent variable when
examining votes cast across all issue types as well as economic cases. In
these contexts, justices are more likely to side with the plaintiff if their
ideological preferences favour the plaintiff ’s preferred position. In criminal cases, the coefficient is positive but is not statistically significant,
suggesting that ideological considerations weigh less in this area. Indeed,
in criminal cases, the results suggest that lawyer characteristics, including a litigation team’s size, experience and gender composition play more
of a role than ideology. On some levels, this is surprising given previous
research demonstrating significant effects of ideology on SCC decisions
~Ostberg and Wetstein, 2007!. However, the role of lawyer gender was
not examined in previous models of SCC justice votes and, as noted here,
its influence is particularly pronounced in criminal cases. As its influence is less pronounced in civil rights and liberties cases, our findings
suggesting a nonsignificant influence for judicial ideology in this issue
area are reasonable considering previous work ~see, for example, Ostberg and Wetstein, 2007!.
Litigant resources also appear to influence SCC justice votes, but
the results suggest caution when assuming which litigant types tend to
be more advantaged. As seen across the models, the relative advantages
of a given litigant type are conditional on the issue area. For example,
we hypothesized that individual plaintiffs would be at a greater disadvantage when confronting governments and, thus, expected negative and
statistically significant coefficients for all defendant government variables. However, while the provincial and national governmental defendant variables performed as expected in most instances, they did not
perform as hypothesized in the civil rights0liberties model ~model 5!.
Here, the coefficients on both of these variables were positive and significant. The local government defendant variable, moreover, was positively signed in the civil rights model as well. These results suggest that
scholars should be careful in utilizing the traditional ~US! party capability scale in studies of the SCC.9 Indeed, certain litigant categories
may not be advantaged or disadvantaged in the same way in Canada,
and the extent to which party resources translate into judicial success
appears contingent on the issue area.10 The results also suggest that controls for both lawyer capability and ideological congruence could affect
Women Lawyers before the Supreme Court of Canada
99
the observed impact of party capability. Indeed, this is not surprising
given that many ~see, for example Galanter, 1974; McCormick, 1993!
posit that the party capability advantage at least in part stems from
the ability to use superiour resources to hire expert legal counsel. This
is also supported by McGuire’s ~1998! finding that the apparent advantage of the US national government in USSCT litigation disappears
after controlling for lawyer experience. Finally, Sheehan and colleagues
~1992! suggest, at least in the context of the USSCT, observed party
capability advantages are a function of the ideological congruence of
the justices with typically advocated legal positions by certain classes
of litigants.
Our results with respect to our lawyer controls are also notable. First,
contrary to our hypothesis, the data do not suggest a decisive advantage
for plaintiffs represented before the SCC by a former clerk. Although the
coefficient is positive as hypothesized, it reached significance only in
the model of votes cast in economic cases. Similarly, if this particular
experience should give defendants represented by former clerks an advantage, we should see plaintiffs more likely to lose when facing a former
clerk. In fact, that is not what happens, according to the results in Table 1.
A plaintiff ’s chances of securing a favourable vote by a SCC justice
increases in all models if they confront a former clerk among the defense
team, save in the context of civil liberties0rights disputes ~model 5!. Such
results are at odds with the notable advantages of clerks who litigate before
the USSCT ~see, for example, Johnson et al., 2006, finding an indirect
relationship between clerkships and oral argument quality; McGuire, 2000,
who finds that former clerks working near the USSCT are more successful! and, thus, warrant further investigation. Finally, like Szmer and colleagues ~2007!, our results lend some support to the notion that litigation
team size is an influential factor in predicting litigant success before the
SCC; however, in the present analysis, support for this proposition is
mixed. Across criminal and economic cases ~model 4!, the litigation team
size variable is positively signed and statistically significant. However,
no support for our hypothesis was found in the context of civil liberties0
rights cases or in the models of votes cast across all issue areas ~models
1 and 2!.11
To assist in providing more meaningful interpretations of our logistic regression coefficients, we present a graph of the predicted probabilities of a justice’s vote for the plaintiff0prosecution across different
values of lawyer team gender. Given our interest in assessing the role
of lawyer gender, the probabilities here and those presented below are
based on the results produced in model 1. As seen in Figure 1, there is
a noticeable increase in the probability of a SCC justice casting a proplaintiff0prosecution vote as the proportion of women lawyers on the
plaintiff ’s side increases relative to the defendant’s.
100
E.B. KAHENY, J.J. SZMER,
AND
T.A. SARVER
FIGURE 1
Graph of the Predicted Probabilities of a Justice’s Vote for the
Plaintiff0Prosecution Given a Change in the Difference in the
Proportion of Women Lawyers Representing the Plaintiff0Prosecution
and Defendant, with Confidence Intervals
To illustrate the substantive effects of all independent variables, we
report changes in the predicted probability that a SCC justice will vote
for the plaintiff in Table 2. Specifically, we assess the impact of individual variables as they change from their minimum to maximum values
while holding the other independent variables constant at select values
~interval level variables are set to their mean; clerkship and party capability variables are held to their modal values!.
In terms of these estimated probabilities, although litigation team
experience appears to have a particularly pronounced effect on a SCC
justice’s decision to support the plaintiff, lawyer team gender also has a
pronounced effect. The change in the probability of a justice casting a proplaintiff vote is approximately 0.10 as one moves from a situation in which
an all-male plaintiff team squares off against an all-female defense team
to a situation in which an all-female plaintiff team confronts an all-male
defense team. This positive change in the estimated probability of a proplaintiff vote is attributed to the relative gain in female representation for
the plaintiff ’s team. Importantly, these changes in the estimated probabilities that a SCC justice votes for the plaintiff indicate that the change produced when altering the lawyer team gender variable is comparable to that
produced when varying the justice/plaintiff ideological congruence measure from its minimum to maximum value. Here, one can see about a 0.12
difference in the probability of a pro-plaintiff vote when the congruence
Women Lawyers before the Supreme Court of Canada
101
TABLE 2
Change in the Predicted Probability that a Justice
Votes for the Plaintiff0Prosecution when the
Independent Variable is Set to Maximum and
Minimum Values, Holding Other Variables
Constant ~Using Model 1!
Independent Variable
Lawyer Team Gender
Justice0Plaintiff Ideological Congruence
Plaintiff0Prosecution Lawyer Clerkship
Defendant Lawyer Clerkship
Lawyer Experience
Litigation Team Size
Plaintiff0Prosecution Appellant
Plaintiff Association
Plaintiff Business
Plaintiff Local Govt.
Plaintiff0Prosecution Prov’l Govt.
Plaintiff0Prosecution Nat’l Govt.
Defendant Association
Defendant Business
Defendant Local Govt.
Defendant Prov’l Govt.
Defendant Nat’l Govt.
Change in the
Predicted Probabilities
0.0950
0.1211
0.0310
0.1560
0.1728
0.0842
0.0060
0.0327
⫺0.0819
⫺0.2570
0.0937
0.0100
⫺0.2779
⫺0.0851
⫺0.1125
⫺0.1810
⫺0.2423
Interval level variables are held at their mean values and the clerkship
dummy variables along with the party capability variables are held to
their modal categories ~‘0’ for the plaintiff0defendant clerkship variables, plaintiff national government v. individual person defendant!.
measure is varied from its minimum to maximum values. Thus, a plaintiff will certainly be advantaged when trying to persuade a given justice
if the plaintiff ’s position in a case ~liberal0conservative! is in line with
the policy preferences of the justice. This attitudinal influence, however,
is not the dominating force in this model. The impact of ideology competes with key characteristics of the litigation team, including experience, size and, yes, its gender composition. Not only does litigation team
gender influence the likelihood of a given SCC justice voting to support
the plaintiff0prosecution; its influence is on par with judicial ideology.
We also estimated changes in the predicted probabilities ~Table 3!
that specific SCC justices will vote for the plaintiff when altering the
lawyer team gender variable from its minimum to its maximum value.
The probabilities reflect situations in which the justice’s ideological preferences are consistent with the outcome preferred by the plaintiff and
other variables are held constant at the select levels noted above. As seen
102
E.B. KAHENY, J.J. SZMER,
AND
T.A. SARVER
TABLE 3
Difference in the Predicted Probabilities for
Minimum ~all-female defense team v. all-male
plaintiff0prosecution team! and Maximum
~all-female plaintiff0prosecution team v. all
male-defense team! Values of Lawyer Team
Gender, by Justice
Justice
Dickson
Beetz
Estey
McIntyre
Le Dain
Wilson
Lamer
La Forest
L’Heureux-Dubé
Sopinka
Gonthier
Cory
McLachlin
Stevenson
Iacobucci
Major
Bastarache
Binnie
Arbour
Le Bel
Change in the Predicted
Probabilities
0.1358
0.1180
0.1437
0.1335
0.1347
0.1359
0.1197
0.1609
0.0997
0.1350
0.1374
0.1155
0.0926
0.1083
0.0844
0.1314
0.0875
0.1329
0.0458
0.1489
These numbers only reflect the impact of lawyer team gender on predicted probabilities of a justice siding with the plaintiff when the justice has the same characteristics ~i.e., values of ideology, gender and
mean proportion of women clerks hired! as the justice listed. They also
reflect the impact of lawyer team gender on the predicted probabilities of a justice vote for the plaintiff0prosecution when the justice’s
ideology is consistent with the ideological direction of the outcome
preferred by the plaintiff0prosecution. Otherwise, interval level variables are held at their means and the clerkship dummy variables along
with the party capability variables are held to their modal categories
~‘0’ for the plaintiff0defendant clerkship variables, plaintiff0prosecution
national government v. individual person defendant!.
in the table, there is some variation among the individual justices in terms
of the assessed impact of lawyer gender. While all justices show an
increased probability in siding with the plaintiff’s team when this variable’s
value is altered in the manner noted above, the impact of this variable is
not uniform among the justices, nor is it ~as discussed above! more pro-
Women Lawyers before the Supreme Court of Canada
103
nounced in the votes of the female justices. Indeed, the estimated probability of the late Justice Bertha Wilson voting for the plaintiff is in line
with many of her male colleagues and, if anything, the gender composition of the litigation team had a less significant influence on the changes
in the reported probabilities of the remaining female justices. The reported
change in the probability for voting for the plaintiff, when stacking the
plaintiff ’s team with female lawyers in a contest with a defendant with
no female representation, was less than or just nearly 0.10 for Justices
L’Heureux-Dubé and McLachlin. The reported change in the probability
for Justice Arbour, moreover, was only 0.05. Meanwhile, Justice La Forest is the most sensitive to the hypothetical change in the lawyer gender
variable ~in terms of producing a greater chance of siding with the plaintiff!. As alluded to above, moreover, the results in Table 3 do not line up
neatly with a justice’s ideology, nor do we see those with notable conservative ideology scores ~assessed via scores from Ostberg and Wetstein,
2007! more likely to vote against teams with a greater proportion of
women, a result that was more clearly seen in the USSCT ~see Szmer
et al., 2010!.
Discussion
While scholars have taken note of the important role of legal counsel in
appellate litigation, few studies have analyzed the influence of lawyer
gender. Those which have ~Szmer et al., 2010! suggest that pursuing this
line of inquiry is crucial to achieving a more complete understanding of
judicial decision making and, specifically, of the role of lawyer characteristics. Unfortunately, that study of USSCT decision making was hindered by that court’s limited gender diversity. For instance, an obvious
question is whether women justices treat arguments by women lawyers
differently. With only four female justices ever serving on the USSCT,
and two of these recent appointments, the ability to generalize is limited.
Thus, to assess this question, we turned to another institutional context:
the SCC. In this paper, we asked whether parties who employ a larger
proportion of women lawyers before the SCC face disadvantages as a
result. Our results suggest otherwise. In the model of all cases as well as
those limited to criminal and economics issues, justices were more likely
to side with parties represented by a larger proportion of female lawyers.
In the civil rights0liberties model, the justices were neither more nor less
likely to support the position represented by a greater number of women
lawyers.
Interestingly, the positive relationship between litigation team gender and justice decision making holds after controlling for several factors. First, it is possible that women are more likely to represent liberal
104
E.B. KAHENY, J.J. SZMER,
AND
T.A. SARVER
parties and, therefore, any observed successes of women barristers are
really a function of ideology. However, we control for ideology. It is also
possible that women lawyers are systematically more or less capable than
their male counterparts, such that any observed relationship between lawyer gender and the justices’ decisions is really a function of lawyer expertise. However, we offer several controls for litigation team expertise.
We also found little evidence that the impact of lawyer gender is
conditional. We found no moderating relationship between justice and
lawyer gender. Moreover, unlike previous work conducted on the USSCT,
we find that Canadian liberal justices are only more likely to side with
litigation teams with higher proportions of women in criminal cases. Additionally, the results do not seem to be a function of the justices assigning
female lawyers greater credibility in women’s issue cases ~except a small
number of cases involving rape! and, in fact, in the area of civil rights0
liberties cases, our results suggest that teams with more women might
be less successful when arguing a “women’s issue.” Finally, to the extent
that possible working relationships have mitigated the application of gender stereotypes, it does not seem to be rooted in clerkship hires; justices
known to have hired greater numbers of women law clerks are not more
likely to side in favour of litigation teams with a greater proportion of
women. What might be more important, however, is the gender composition of the panel on which the SCC justice serves in a given case but,
even here, the evidence is mixed.
In summary, these results suggest that, despite the persistence of gender discrimination against Canadian female lawyers, women arguing
before the SCC, in some circumstances, fare much better than their male
counterparts when attempting to persuade the justices. The possible reasons for this finding deserve further scholarly inquiry. One possibility is
that this is a function of the composition of the SCC itself. The notable
presence of women on this elite institution might not only mitigate negative gender bias but also enhance the status of women in the eyes of
male justices. Throughout their tenure on the bench, the male justices we
examined served on the SCC with multiple women colleagues whereas,
on the USSCT, the male justices have not. Further, male-only decisional
contexts are unique in the pool of cases we analyzed; in fact, of the cases
examined here, nearly 96 per cent included at least one woman on the
panel.
We, of course, recognize that there are limitations to this study. For
one thing, the time period ~1988–2000! prevents us from studying whether
the all-male, pre-Wilson Court treated women lawyers differently. Of
course, as noted above, few women lawyers participated in SCC appeals
during that time. Furthermore, a future study could examine more recent
cases to determine the impact of a woman chief justice. Of course, we
are not sure whether that would change the findings dramatically, since
Women Lawyers before the Supreme Court of Canada
105
we generally find that the Court already looked more favourably upon
parties represented by more women lawyers under male chief justice stewardship. Additionally, we fail to test whether the SCC’s greater gender
diversity was indeed the underlying causal mechanism for our findings,
but recent work involving gender and judicial decision making as well as
feminist theory may prove fruitful in future research seeking to shed light
on whether this is the case. Perhaps the critical mass theory, which, to
date, has helped explain the decisional contexts in which we might see
marked differences in the voting behaviour of male and female judges
~for example, Boyd et al., 2010!, might also help explain the threshold
point at which gender schemas are no longer applied in a negative fashion or the point at which negative stereotypes no longer control.12
Notes
1 We chose this period because it is longer than most lawyer studies and there was
significant gender variation among the actors ~1988 represented the first full year in
which more than one woman served on the SCC; prior to that point, female lawyer
participation drops dramatically!. While we would like to examine the McLachlin
Court in greater detail, the difficulties in data collection were prohibitive. Moreover,
the case level data from Haynie et al. ~1999! and Tate et al. ~2002! was not available
beyond 2000 when we began the study. Indeed, one of the co-principal investigators
on the grant recently co-authored an article examining judge gender and also stopped
at 2000 ~Songer and Johnson, 2007!. Additionally, we would like to examine cases
prior to 1982 to see if the all-male Court treated women lawyers differently, but during that time few women participated in SCC litigation. For example, looking at the
first 15 cases per year decided between 1978 and 1981, one woman participated in
four in 1981, three in 1980, two in 1979 and no women at all participated in 1978.
2 These data are part of a larger project funded by the National Science Foundation,
“Collaborative Research: Fitting More Pieces into the Puzzle of Judicial Behaviour:
A Multi-Country Database and Program of Research,” SES-9975323; and “Collaborative Research: Extending a Multi-Country Database and Program of Research,” SES0137349, C. Neal Tate, Donald R. Songer, Stacia Haynie and Reginald S. Sheehan,
principal investigators.
3 See the online appendix ~http:00pantherfile.uwm.edu0ekaheny0www0! for an alternative operationalization of the dependent variable ~predicting the likelihood of a proappellant vote!.
4 Our gender schema theory assumes justices know the lawyer’s sex. When only one
lawyer prepares the factum and delivers the oral argument, the justices will obviously know the lawyer’s sex. It is also possible, however, that gender schemas can
operate via name recognition since there is evidence justices read the factums ~see,
for example, Hogg, 2006, discussing the materials read by a SCC justice or Greene
et al., 1998: 110, quoting a SCC justice describing review of factums when deciding to grant leaves!, as well as previous research suggesting written work might be
discounted on the basis of the author’s sex ~for example, Paludi and Strayer, 1985!.
Additionally, Szmer and others ~2010! report high correlations among orally arguing attorney sex and litigant team gender composition, and their findings with respect
to the effect of lawyer gender did not significantly differ when utilizing either
measure.
106
E.B. KAHENY, J.J. SZMER,
AND
T.A. SARVER
5 Technically, it is ln ~1 ⫹ mean number of cases!, since mean values of zero occur in
the data.
6 The SCC provided a complete list of SCC clerks.
7 Associations include business0trade associations, labour organizations, educational
associations and institutions, religious0charitable associations, political organizations and associations of indigenous peoples.
8 Note that we did not include the women’s issues multiplicative term in economic
cases because none of the economic cases in our data involved women’s issues.
9 It is also possible that any atheoretical results are a function of multicollinearity.
Indeed, the VIFs for many of the party capability controls are above three.
10 Of course, some of our results may be due to coding conventions adopted in the
voting data upon which we rely and the relatively few number of votes contained
within some of these party capability variables when reporting their influence in
the context of narrower issue areas. In our sample of civil rights cases, for example,
local government plaintiffs lost in all of the small number of cases in which they
appeared. Further, we expected to find a positive, significant coefficient on the plaintiff national government variable across all models. The data, though, do not support this proposition. One possible explanation is that cases involving “the Queen”
were designated as the national government even if the case was being handled by
the provincial government. Although both should be advantaged relative to individual parties and all other litigants for that matter, the assumption is that the national
government would tend to have a higher success rate than provincial governments.
Thus, perhaps the reason we do not find statistically significant results for the national
government before the SCC in all of these issue areas is that, in fact, in a significant number of cases, the national government was not litigating but, rather, it was
the provincial government. Inspection of a smaller sample of cases in which the
data source indicates the national government was a party to a lawsuit did give rise
to this concern. Fortunately, however, we have no reason to believe this possible
issue has any influence with respect to our variables of interest pertaining to lawyer
gender.
11 We also tested these models with a panel size variable, which might reflect case
complexity. With the exception of model 5 ~civil rights!, increases in this variable
were negatively related to a vote for the plaintiff0prosecution. Larger panels in the
civil rights context, however, increased the likelihood of a justice vote for the plaintiff. We also tested a panel size0lawyer team gender interaction in our combined issues
model. Lawyer team gender was significant ~and positive! in seven- and nine-justice
panels; in five-justice panels, it was not significant. This is not surprising, since fivejustice panels typically involve clear-cut applications of straightforward legal principles.
12 Of course, analyses that further probe the development and application of these gender stereotypes in the thought processes of relevant actors in high court litigation
might also prove fruitful.
References
American Bar Association ~ABA!, Commission on Women in the Profession. 2007. A Current Glance at Women in the Law. www.abanet.org0women0CurrentGlanceStatistics2007.
pdf ~25 April 2008!.
Boyd, Christina L., Lee Epstein and Andrew D. Martin. 2010. “Untangling the Causal
Effects of Sex on Judging.” American Journal of Political Science 54 ~2!: 389–411.
Brockman, Joan. 1993. “A Difference without a Distinction.” Canadian Journal of Law &
Society 8 ~1!: 149–64.
Women Lawyers before the Supreme Court of Canada
107
Canadian Bar Association ~CBA!. 1993. Touchstones for Change: Equality, Diversity and
Accountability: Report of the Canadian Bar Association Task Force on Gender Equality in the Legal Profession. Ottawa ON: Canadian Bar Association.
Carli, Linda L. 1989. “Gender Differences in Interaction Style and Influence.” Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology 56 ~4!: 565–76.
Corley, Pamela. 2008. “The Supreme Court and Opinion Content: The Influence of Parties Briefs.” Political Research Quarterly 61 ~3!: 468–78.
Craig, J.M. and R.R. Jacobs. 1985. “The Effect of Working with Women on Male Attitudes Toward Female Firefighters.” Basic and Applied Social Psychology 6: 61–74.
Dolan, Kathleen. 2004. Voting for Women: How the Public Evaluates Women Candidates.
Boulder CO: Westview Press.
Farhang, Sean and Gregory Wawro. 2004. “Institutional Dynamics on the US Courts of
Appeals: Minority Representation under Panel Decision Making.” Journal of Law,
Economics and Organization 20 ~2!: 299–330.
Fiske, S.T. 1982. “Schema-Triggered Affect: Applications to Social Perception.” In Affect
and Cognition: The 17 th Annual Carnegie Symposium on Cognition, ed. M.S. Clark
and S.T. Fiske. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.
Flemming, Roy B. 2004. Tournament of Appeals: Granting Judicial Review in Canada.
Vancouver BC: UBC Press.
Flemming, Roy B. and Glen S. Krutz. 2002. “Repeat Litigators and Agenda Setting on the
Supreme Court of Canada.” Canadian Journal of Political Science 35~4!: 811–33.
Foot, David K. and D.A. Stager. 1990. “Intertemporal Market Effect on Gender Earnings
Differentials: Lawyers in Canada, 1970–1980.” Applied Economics 21 ~8!: 1101–28.
Galanter, Marc. 1974. “Why the Haves Come Out Ahead: Speculations on the Limits of
Legal Change.” Law and Society Review 9~1!: 95–160.
Greene, Ian, Carl Baar, Peter McCormick, George Szablowski and Martin Thomas. 1998.
Final Appeal: Decision-making in Canadian Courts of Appeal. Toronto: James Lorimer
& Co.
Gryski, Gerard, Eleanor C. Main and William J. Dixon. 1986. “Models of State High Court
Decision Making in Sex Discrimination Cases.” Journal of Politics 48 ~1!: 143–55.
Haire, Susan Brodie, Stefanie A. Lindquist and Roger Hartley. 1999. “Attorney Expertise,
Litigant Success, and Judicial Decisionmaking in the US Courts of Appeals.” Law
and Society Review 33 ~3!: 667–85.
Haslett, Beth, Florence L. Geis and Mae R. Carter. 1992. The Organizational Woman:
Power and Paradox. Norwood NJ: Ablex Publishing.
Hausegger, Lori and Stacia Haynie. 2003. “Judicial Decisionmaking and the Use of Panels in the Canadian Supreme Court and the South African Appellate Division.” Law
and Society Review 37~3!: 635–58.
Haynie, Stacia L., Reginald Sheehan, Donald R. Songer, and C. Neal Tate. 1999. “Collaborative Research: Fitting More Pieces into the Puzzle of Judicial Behavior: a MultiCountry Data Base and Program of Research.” National Science Foundation,
SES-9975323.
Hogg, Peter W. 2006. “Judicial Interview Process: Opening Remarks to Ad Hoc Committee on Supreme Court Appointment.” February 27. http:00www.justice.gc.ca0eng0deptmin0pub0scc-csc0sp-dis.html ~30 October 2008!.
Johnson, Timothy R., Paul J. Wahlbeck and James F. Spriggs, II. 2006. “The Influence of
Oral Arguments on the US Supreme Court.” American Political Science Review 100
~1!: 99–113.
Kahn, Kim Fridkin. 1996. The Political Consequences of Being a Woman: How Stereotypes Influence the Conduct and Consequences of Political Campaigns. New York:
Columbia University Press.
Kanter, Rosabeth Moss. 1977a. Men and Women of the Corporation. New York: Basic
Books.
108
E.B. KAHENY, J.J. SZMER,
AND
T.A. SARVER
Kanter, Rosabeth Moss. 1977b. “Some Effects of Proportions on Group Life: Skewed Sex
Ratios and Responses to Token Women.” American Journal of Sociology 82 ~5!:
965–90.
Kay, Fiona M. and Joan Brockman. 2000. “Barriers to Gender Equality in the Canadian
Legal Establishment.” Feminist Legal Studies 8 ~2!: 169–98.
Lopez, Maria Pabon. 2008. “The Future of Women in the Legal Profession: Recognizing
the Challenges Ahead by Reviewing Current Trends.” Hastings Women’s Law Journal
19 ~1!: 53–103.
Matland, Richard E. 1994. “Putting Scandinavian Equality to the Test: An Experimental
Evaluation of Gender Stereotyping of Political Candidates in a Sample of Norwegian
Voters.” British Journal of Political Science 24 ~2!: 273–92.
McAtee, Andrea and Kevin T. McGuire. 2007. “Lawyers, Justices, and Issue Salience:
When and How Do Legal Arguments Affect the U.S. Supreme Court?” Law and Society Review 41: 259–78.
McCormick, Peter. 1993. “Party Capability Theory and Appellate Success in the Supreme
Court of Canada, 1949–1992.” Canadian Journal of Political Science 3: 535–40.
McCormick, Peter and Twyla Job. 1993. “Do Women Judges Make a Difference? An Analysis by Appeal Court Data.” Canadian Journal of Law and Society 8 ~1!: 135–48.
McGuire, Kevin T. 1995. “Repeat Players in the Supreme Court: The Role of Experienced
Lawyers in Litigation Success.” Journal of Politics 57 ~1!: 187–96.
McGuire, Kevin T. 1998. “Explaining Executive Success on the US Supreme Court.” Political Research Quarterly 51 ~2!: 505–26.
McGuire, Kevin T. 2000. “Lobbyists, Revolving Doors and the U.S. Supreme Court.” Journal of Law and Politics 16 ~1!: 113–37.
Nelson, Mary Stuart. 2004. “The Effect of Attorney Gender on Jury Perception and
Decision-Making.” Law and Psychology Review 28 ~1!: 177–94.
Nisbett, Richard E. and Lee Ross. 1980. Human Inference: Strategies and Shortcomings
of Social Judgment. Englewood Cliffs NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Noonan, Mary C., Mary E. Corcoran and Paul N. Courant. 2005. “Pay Differences Among
the Highly Trained: Cohort Differences in the Sex Gap in Lawyers’ Earnings.” Social
Forces 84 ~2!: 153–72.
Ostberg, C. L., and Matthew E. Wetstein. 1998. “Dimensions of Attitudes Underlying Search
and Seizure Decisions of the Supreme Court of Canada.” Canadian Journal of Political Science 31 ~4!: 767–87.
Ostberg, C. L., and Matthew E. Wetstein. 2007. Attitudinal Decision Making in the Supreme
Court of Canada. Vancouver: UBC Press.
Paludi, M.A. and L.A. Strayer. 1985. “What’s in an Author’s Name? Differential Evaluations of Performance as a Function of Author’s Name.” Sex Roles 12 ~3–4!: 353–61.
Peresie, Jennifer L. 2005. “Female Judges Matter: Gender and Collegial Decisionmaking
in the Federal Appellate Courts.” Yale Law Journal 114 ~7!: 1759–90.
Powell, Gary N. 1993. Women and Men in Management. Newbury Park CA: Sage
Publications.
Rhode, Deborah L. 2002. “Gender and the Profession: The No-Problem Problem.” Hofstra Law Review 30 ~5!: 1001–13.
Sanbonmatsu, Kira. 2002. “Gender Stereotypes and Vote Choice.” American Journal of
Political Science 46 ~1!: 20–34.
Segal, Jeffrey A. and Albert D. Cover. 1989. “Ideological Values and the Votes of US
Supreme Court Justices.” American Political Science Review 83 ~2!: 557–65.
Segal, Jennifer. 2000. “Representative Decision Making on the Federal Bench: Clinton’s
District Court Appointees.” Political Research Quarterly 53 ~1!: 137–50.
Sheehan, Reginald S., William Mishler and Donald R. Songer. 1992. “Ideology, Status,
and the Differential Success of Direct Parties before the Supreme Court.” American
Political Science Review 86 ~2!: 464–71.
Women Lawyers before the Supreme Court of Canada
109
Songer, Donald R. 2008. The Transformation of the Supreme Court of Canada: An Empirical Examination. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.
Songer, Donald R. and Reginald S. Sheehan. 1992. “Who Wins on Appeal? Underdogs
and Upperdogs on the United States Courts of Appeals.” American Journal of Political Science 36: 235–58.
Songer, Donald R., Sue Davis and Susan Haire. 1994. “A Reappraisal of Diversification
in the Federal Courts: Gender Effects in the Courts of Appeals.” Journal of Politics
56 ~2!: 425–39.
Songer, Donald R. and Susan W. Johnson. 2007. “Judicial Decision Making in the Supreme
Court of Canada: Updating the Personal Attribute Model.” Canadian Journal of Political Science 40 ~4!: 911–34.
Szmer, John J., Susan W. Johnson and Tammy A. Sarver. 2007. “Does the Lawyer Matter?
Influencing Outcomes on the Supreme Court of Canada.” Law and Society Review
41 ~2!: 259–78.
Szmer, John J., Tammy A. Sarver and Erin B. Kaheny. 2010. “Have We Come a Long Way
Baby? Female Attorneys before the United States Supreme Court.” Politics and Gender 6: 1–36.
Tate, C. Neal, Stacia L. Haynie, Reginald Sheehan, and Donald R. Songer. 2002. “Collaborative Research: Extending a Multi-Country Database and Program of Research.”
National Science Foundation, SES-0137349.
Taylor, Shelley E. and Jennifer Crocker. 1981. “Schematic Bases of Social Information
Processing.” In Social Cognition: The Ontario Symposium, vol. 1, ed. E. Tory Higgins, C. Peter Herman and Mark P. Zanna. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Valian, Virginia. 1999. “Roundtable: The Cognitive Bases of Gender Bias.” Brooklyn Law
Review 65: 1037–61.
Valian, Virginia. 2005. “Beyond Gender Schemas: Improving the Advancement of Women
in Academia.” Hypatia 20 ~3!: 198–213.
Western Law Career Services Office. 2005. Clerkship Handbook for Western Law Students. http://www.law.uwo.ca/info-current/cso/2005Clerkship.htm ~3 March 2010!.
Wetstein, Matthew W. and C.L. Ostberg. 2007. “Gender Differences in the US and Canadian Supreme Courts.” Paper presented at the 2007 Annual Meeting of the Midwest
Political Science Association, Chicago IL.