Phonological Skills and Writing of Presyllabic Children
Author(s): Cristina Silva and Margarida Alves-Martins
Source: Reading Research Quarterly, Vol. 37, No. 4 (Oct. - Nov. - Dec., 2002), pp. 466-483
Published by: International Reading Association
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/748262
Accessed: 02/03/2010 11:40
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless
you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you
may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use.
Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at
http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=ira.
Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed
page of such transmission.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.
International Reading Association is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to
Reading Research Quarterly.
http://www.jstor.org
Reading Research Quarterly
Vol. 37, No. 4
October/November/December 2002
C2002 International Reading Association
(pp. 466-483)
skills and writing
Phonological
of presyllabic
children
CristinaSilva
Alves-Martins
Margarida
Instituto
dePsicologia
Aplicada,Lisboa,Portugal
Superior
U
and the awareness of syllabic and intrasyllabicunits
(Liberman,Shankweiler, Fischer, & Carter,1974; Sim-Sim,
1997; Treiman, 1992), may develop spontaneously over
the course of the preschool years, whereas for most children proficiency in more complex abilities, such as synthesis or phonemic segmentation, which require
phonemic awareness, occurs when they learn to read
(Morais, 1994; Perfetti, Beck, Bell, & Hugues, 1987).
The argument about the meaning of the relationship between phonological awareness and reading acquisition has been replaced gradually by a finer analysis
concerning the type of minimum skills required to understand the alphabetic characteristicsof written language
on the one hand and the definition of those that result
from the process of learning to read on the other (Byrne,
1992, 1997; Goswami, 1998; Murray,1998; Stahl &
Murray,1994, 1998). For example, works by Stahl and
Murray(1994, 1998) tell us that the capacity to identify
common phonemes in different words is a skill that is
critical to children's understanding of the way in which
written code functions, while the abilities to segment and
blend phonemes essentially develop once the alphabet
has been learnt.
At the same time, the role phonological awareness
plays in learning to read was redefined when it was conjugated with letter-name familiarityas a fundamental basis
for understanding the alphabetic principle. The latter is
defined as the understanding of the nature of the relationships between sounds and the letters in the written code.
he discovery of the relationship between phonological awareness and the process of learning to
read is one of the most important contributions
of the last 30 years of research in the domain of
the acquisition of literacy. Despite the significance of this
discovery, for many years the texts on the subject focused exclusively on a debate about the meaning of the
causal relationship between phonological awareness and
learning to read (Goswami & Bryant, 1990; Morais, 1994).
This theoretical discussion, in which oral analytical abilities were seen sometimes as a cause and sometimes as a
consequence of learning to read, may well have arisen as
a result of disparities between the evaluative tasks that
were used in research in this field and the inconsistency
in the way in which the concept of phonological awareness was operationalized.
The concept of phonological awareness implies the
existence of a general capability with multiple dimensions that influence the difficulty experienced in the various tasks. The best way to look at this capability is
probably to see it as part of a continuum (Stanovich,
1992). Skills such as detecting and producing rhymes are
considered to be on a lower level of the continuum as
they require few analytical capabilities and simply an
awareness of similar phonological sequences, while the
abilities to segment and invert the phonemes in words
are placed on a higher level because they imply an analytical attitude and an explicit representation of phonetic
segments. Some skills, like the ability to detect rhymes
466
ABSTACT
Phonological skills and writing of presyllabic children
beTheobjectiveof thisstudywasto identifycausalrelationships
tweenthe developmentof phonologicalabilitiesandprogressin
knowledgeaboutwritingin preschoolchildren.Thestudylooked
whoweredividedintothreegroups
at 71 preschool-aged
children,
Thechildren
wasgovernedbypresyllabic
criteria.
andwhosewriting
in thefirstgroupweresubjected
intervention
that
to anexperimental
focusedon writingandwasintendedto leadthemto evolvein such
Thesecondgroupwastheoba wayas to producesyllabicwriting.
aimed
at
of
syllabicunits.Thethirdset of
phonological
training
ject
children
servedas a controlgroup.Allthreegroupswereat equivawitha similar
number
of letlentintellectual
levelsandwerefamiliar
ters.Thetwoexperimental
intervention
also
programs provedequiv-
alentin termsof theconceptual
evolutiontheytriggered,
to the exin bothexperimental
tentthatthechildren
groupsstartedwritingin
withsyllabiccriteria;
accordance
thewritingof the childrenin the
controlgroupcontinuedto be guidedby presyllabic
criteria.In
the
in
tests
children
the
two
phonological
experimental
groups
achievedresultsthatrevealeda similardegreeof progress(greater
thanthatattainedby the controlgroupchildren)in the initialclassification
tests.Thesecsyllabledeletionandtheinitial-phoneme
ondexperimental
adgroupdisplayedmorestatistically
significant
deletiontestthaneitherof theother
vancesin the initial-phoneme
twogroups.
Habilidades fonol6gicas y escritura en niflos pre-silabicos
lasrelaciones
causalesenElobjetivode esteestudiofueidentificar
treel desarrollo
de lashabilidades
fonol6gicas
y el progresode los
en nifiospre-silibicos.
Elestudio
sobrela escritura
conocimientos
en tres
examin6a 71nifiosen edadpre-escolar
quefuerondivididos
estabagobernada
porcriterios
pre-silibicos.
gruposy cuyaescritura
Losnifiosdel primergrupoestuvieronexpuestosa unaintervenci6nexperimental
y tuvocomoprop6siquese centr6en la escritura
to guiarloshaciala producci6nde escriturasilibica.Elsegundo
fonol6gicocentradoen las unidades
gruporecibi6entrenamiento
silibicas.Eltercergrupode nifiosfuncion6comogrupode control.
Lostresgruposteniannivelesintelectuales
y estabanfaequivalentes
miliarizados
con unacantidadsimilarde letras.Losdos programas
de intervenci6n
tambienresultaron
en
experimentales
equivalentes
thrminos
de la evoluci6nconceptual
al puntode
quepromovieron,
comenzaron
a esque los nifiosde ambosgruposexperimentales
cribirde acuerdoa criteriossildbicos;
la escritura
de los nifiosdel
Enlas
grupode controlcontinu6guiadaporcriterios
pre-silibicos.
nifios
los
de
ambos
lopruebasfonol6gicas,
gruposexperimentales
unciertogradode progreso
graronresultados
querevelaron
(mayor
queel alcanzado
porlos nifiosdel grupode control)en la omisi6n
de silabainicialy en la clasificaci6n
de fonemainicial.Elsegundo
mostr6
mis signigrupoexperimental
progresosestadisticamente
en lapruebade omisi6nde fonemainicialquelos otrosdos
ficativos
grupos.
Phonologische Fertigkeiten und das Schreiben von pra-syllabischen Kindern
ZieldieserStudiewares, kausaleZusammenhdnge
zwischender
undFortschritten
im
von phonologischen
Entwicklung
Fihigkeiten
zu identifizieren.
WissenumdasSchreiben
derVorschulkinder
Die
die in dreiGruppen
Studierichtetesich auf 71 Vorschulkinder,
durchpri-syllabische
Kriterien
eingeteiltundderenSchreibarbeiten
bestimmt
waren.DieKinderin dererstenGruppewurdeneinerexperimentellen
Beeinflussung
ausgesetzt,die sich aufsSchreiben
unddazubestimmt
daM
konzentrierte
anzuleiten,
war,sie dergestalt
sie syllabisches
Die zweiteGruppewurde
Schreiben
produzierten.
aufsyllabische
dasObjektphonologischen
Trainings,
ausgerichtet
Einheiten.Die dritte Auswahlan Kinderndiente als eine
befanden
sichaufvergleichbaren
AlledreiGruppen
Kontrollgruppe.
intellektuellen
Stufenundwarenmiteiner ihnlichenAnzahlvon
Buchstabenvertraut.Die zwei experimentellenInterventions-
467
imSinnederkonzeptuellen
erwiesensichalsebenbtirtig
programme
diesie hervorriefen,
inbeiden
insoweit,dagdieKinder
Entwicklung,
in
mit
Experimentiergruppen Ubereinstimmung syllabischen
Kriterien
zu schreibenbegannen;dasSchreiben
derKinderin der
wurde unter fortgesetzterAnleitungvon priKontrollgruppe
Indenphonologischen
Kriterien
Testsersyllabischen
weitergefdhrt.
reichtendieKinderin beidenexperimentellen
Gruppen
Ergebnisse,
die einen jeweils ihnlichenGradan Fortschritten
offenbarten
derKontrollgruppe
erreicht
(gr68eralsjener,dervon denKindern
undbei der
wurde),in deranfinglich-syllabischen
Ausmerzung
anfinglich-phonemischen
Unterteilungder Tests. Die zweite
in
zeigtemehrstatistisch
Experimentiergruppe
auffilligeFortschritte
demTestzuranfinglich-phonemischen
alsjedederbeiAusmerzung
denanderenGruppen.
AIBSTRACT
,J?~~l3~~%~~~L~f~
?1~5Ci
>~
7
2Dla,1L
sI
71 At~
)~-c~~i0
fI
tlIL?tko111-r 7,
- 7
)#
a?0
)VC --HfNitN:1
Y
-Ik5
itAU
;5 W;
2111)znItQ`
7 h3 -
kv -"*I,
?10,-l-o inLJ0R
-rtf-7
Competences phonologiques et ecriture d'enfants pre-syllabiques
l'?volution
intellectuelle
Cette
etudeavaitpourobjectif
d'identifier
lesrelations
causales
entre concerne
dansla
qu'ilsontdclench@e,
etleprogres
ont
mesure
oiulesenfants
desdeuxgroupes
led&veloppement
descompetences
des
phonologiques
experimentauxcomrelatives
l'acriture
chezdesenfants
tandisquel'ecriture
connaissances
d'agepresco- menc?aecrireavecdescriteres
syllabiques,
.
dugroupe
laire.L'etude
a portesur71enfants
entrois desenfants
a descriteres
a continue
d'age
repondre
prescolaire
divises
contr6le
.
' des
dont
Dans
les
tests
les
desdeux
et
1'6criture
enfants
r6pondait criteres
groupes
pre-syllabiques. pre-syllabiques.
phonologiques
Lesenfants
dupremier
ont
des
atteint
resultats
manifestant
desprogroupeontetesoumis. uneintervention groupes
experimentaux
surl'criturequiakt conduite
defagon'a
aceuxatteintsparles enfantsdu
(sup6rieurs
experimentale
centr@e
grescomparables
aproduire
Lesecondgroupe
a
uneecriture
lesamener
dansdestestsdemandant
lasuppression
delapresyllabique.
groupe
contr6le)
faitl'objet
d'unentrainement
centre
sur
des
unites
ou
du
initial.
Le
second
sylphonologique
phoneme
groupe
mieresyllabe
experimend'enfants
a servidegroupe
ensemble
condesprogres
tala presente
labiques.Letroisieme
statistiquement
plussignificatifs
quechaLestroisgroupes
avaient
unniveau
intellectuel
et
cun des deuxautresgroupesdansle testde suppression
du
tr61e.
equivalent
avecunnombre
delettres
semblable.
Lesdeux phoneme
initial.
etaient
familiarises
d'intervention
se sontreve1es
en ce qui
equivalents
programmes
(1OHOJIOFHeCKHe HaBbIKH H HHCbMOy leTeHi, He 3HaIOIUHXAJCeJHHqIHa CjiorH
B TOM,qTO6bIBbaIMBITI
Tb pHHHHO-CJICACTBeHHbie
IJenibHaCToiuero Hccine)oXBaHnJ
COCTO5naU
CBB3H
H flPpeACTaBj1eHHI
MeKAtypa3BHTHmeM
y AOUKOJIbHHKOB.
4OHonorHecKHx CnOCO6HOCTeI
o
0lHCbMe
Ha
3a
o6uMHM IHCJIOM
H4ccnexaoBaHHe
6a3HpoBanioc
Ha6nIO•eHHH TpeMtsrpynnaMn XOIKOJIbHHKOB
71 qenoBeK,qbe fHCbMOOCHOBdBaJIOCb
Haa;OcJIOrOBOM
iOAxoAe. eTHB nepBoi rpynne 6bumn
no;aepriyTbMi 3KcepBmeHTaJIHOMy
BemaTenbcTBy, HanpaBneHHOMyHa nIHCMO H
rpynna 6bina
npeHa3HaqeHHOMy AI nojrOTOBKHaeTeii K CHJan6HqeCKOMy
nHcbMy.BTOpaW
HaAH
o6,eKTOM 4oHonoriOmecKoro o6yenHRa, nauejiennoro
4epeHunnaiumo cHIIa6HqecKHIX
Bce TpHrpynnb iMeJIHOaHHaKOBbIi
Tpemsi rpynna aeTei 6Ema KOHTpOJIbHOH.
eanHHHL.
ypoBeH]H 3HaJIH OAJHaKOBOe KOJIHieCTBo 6yKB. 06e 3KCiiepHMeHTaJILHble
nHHTenneKTyaJhHbmI
HHTepBeHIHOHHble nporpaMMbL TaI•e 6un paBHno3HnatHbIB HiiaHe 3BOJOKHH nIOIHIATITHi,
KOTOpble
B
OHHpa3BHBaJIH. B HTore AeTH B o6eHX 3KCIiepHMeHTaJibHblX
rpynniax Hala1HInica
B TOBpeMA KaKAITH H3 KOHTpOJlbHOOi
COOTBeTCTBHH
C CHUIa6HieCKHMHKpHTepHSIMH,
rpylnnl•
npoAojDKaI
pyKOBO;CTBOBaThCS upinuHCbMe aocjIOrOBbNMH KpHTepH[MH.B KIaccH4HKaIu4HOHHbIX
TeCTaXAeTH H3 AByX 3KCiepeHMeHTaJbHbIX
rpynn He OnyCKaJHHalaJibHbiai
Cam
OF
rH
onpeaeMiH HaqajibHyIo OHemy OaHHaKOBO
ycIelHIHO(IIOKa3aB6oJiee BbICOKHe pe3yiqETaTbI, ieM
aeTH H3 KOHTpOJIbHOirrpyllnil).
BTopas rpynna noKa3ana6onee CTaTHCTHwiecKH3HatHMylo
TeCTOBBbSIBJIeHI4OHatlailbHOi (OHeMbi, leM OCTaJIbHbIe
AHHaMHKy IIpH BbIrImOJIHeHHH
rpynnb.
(OHojIorHIecKHX
468
Byrne(1998)showed thatthe awarenessof phonewith lettersdo not of themselves
mic unitsand familiarity
will
a
child
that
manageto make use of the alguarantee
in
real
contexts,but ratherthat
reading
phabeticprinciple
it is the combinationof the formertwo thatprovescrucial
to the discoveryof the latter.In turn,understandingthe
alphabeticprincipleenableschildrento gain access to decoding proceduresthatformpartof the readingactivity.
Phonemicawarenessis thus now seen as a skillthat
is necessaryto obtain,albeitof itselfinsufficientto ensure, a full conceptualunderstandingof the alphabetic
principle;childrenmay possess an adequatenotion of
the phonemicstructureof oral speech withoutthis in itself leadingto an understandingof the alphabeticnature
of writtenlanguage.This understandingnot only requires
phonemicanalyticalskills,but also meansthatchildren
must possess a concept of writingthatleads them to representationof the phonemicstructureof words.
In the 1970s,Read(1971, 1975)was alreadycalling
attentionto the creativespellingof preschool-agechildren,showing thatthey are not all at the same level in
termsof the way in which they look at writtenlanguage.
Sincethen, a largenumberof works (Ferreiro,1988;
Pontecorvo& Orsolini,1996;Read,1986;Sulzby,1989)
have shown thatan understandingof the abstractrules
thatunderliethe organisationof alphabeticsystemsis a
processthatbegins earlyon, via the informalcontacts
thatchildrengraduallymake with writtenlanguage.In
theireffortsto understandthe meaningsof graphicmarks
and to interactwith others(both peers and adults),children ask themselvesquestionsaboutthe correspondences between objectsand writingand aboutthe
relationshipsbetween the oral and the writtenformsof
language.In this way they build up unconventionalideas
aboutthe propertiesof writingand what it represents.
They constructa series of conceptualhypothesesthatcan
be more or less close to the real alphabeticsystem.
Ferreiro'swork (1988) on preschoolchildren'sinvented spellingssuggeststhatchildren'sknowledgeof
writtenlanguageevolves throughthree essentiallevels of
conceptualisation.The firstlevel can be characterisedby
the searchfor criteriathatmake it possible to differentiate
between drawingsand writtenlanguage,and by the
gradualperceptionthata sequence of lettersconstitutes
an object that stands in for the real thing. In parallel with
this differentiation process, the child also elaborates criteria that make a series of letters into something that can
transmit a message. He or she considers that there must
be a minimum number of letters for it to be possible to
read and write a message, and also that the letters must
vary. The latter criterion leads him or her not to use the
same sequence of letters to spell different words.
Phonological
skills and writing
A second level involvesrefiningthe formsof qualitaof the orderof lettersin his or her attive (diversification
(the minimumnumber
temptsat writing)and quantitative
differenof lettersneeded to makewritingunderstandable)
tiationbetweenchainsof letters,in such a way as to ensuredifferencesin how differentwordsare represented.
The ideas thatchildrenformaboutwritingreflect
the imagesthatthey build up fromthe examplesthey see
aroundthem, in which words are genericallycomposed
of variouslettersand those lettersare set out in different
sequences for differentwords. It is these merelygraphic
characteristics
thatchildrenseek to take into accountin
theirinventedspellings.At these levels childrenhave not
yet establishedany relationshipbetween oraland written
language.Genericallyspeaking,it is possible to call these
firsttwo levels presyllabic.
On a thirdlevel childrenbegin to relateoral language to writtenlanguage.This level consistsof a
numberof stages,beginningwith the searchfor correspondencesbetween lettersand the syllabicsegmentsin
words. In theirinventedspellingschildrenwritea letter
with which to representeach of the syllablesin a word,
even thoughthey establishthe letter/syllablecorrespondence on a purelyrandombasis-syllabic writingwithout
phonetization.
Subsequentlythey establishthis correspondenceusconventional
lettersto representone of the sounds in
ing
a given syllable-syllabic writingwith phonetization.This
syllabicwritingis particularly
apparentin languagessuch
as Spanishand Portuguese.The frequencywith which
this type of writingappearsin Portugueseprobablyis explainedby its structurein which thereare manypolysyllabic words and the syllabicstructurethatpredominates
is one of open syllablesof the consonant/voweltype
(Andrade& Viana,1993;Vigario& Fale, 1993).
Laterstill,childrenbegin to analyseoral languagein
a way thatgoes beyond the syllabiclevel. Thisgives rise
to inventedspellingsin which childrenrepresentall the
phonemes in some of the syllablesof a word yet continue to use single lettersto denote othersyllablesin the
same word-syllabic-alphabeticwriting.
The thirdconceptuallevel culminatesin an understandingof the alphabeticnatureof writtenlanguagereflected in inventedspellingsin which the phonetic
structureof the word is fullylearntand codified,even
though not all the applicable orthographic conventions
are respected-alphabetic writing.
This evolutionary path, which has been identified in
relation to a variety of languages-French (Besse, 1993,
1995, 1996; Chauveau & Rogovas-Chauveau, 1994;
Fijalkow, 1993), Portuguese (Alves-Martins,1994; AlvesMartins& Mendes, 1987), Italian (Pontecorvo &
Zuchermaglio, 1988, 1995), Hebrew (Tolchinsky, 1995;
469
Tolchinsky& Levin,1988),and English(Sulzby,1989)has not, however,been takeninto accountin the majority of researchinto phonologicalawareness.
This developmentalpath does not requirepassing
throughall the variousstages.Theremay even coexist
formsof writingthatimplydifferentways of lookingat
the writtencode (Besse, 1995;Sulzby,1986).Sulzby,for
example,considered"thatthereis no one identicaldevelopmentalsequence in the way in which childrenuse the
writtensystem"(p. 70). Inasmuchas certainwrittenforms
appear,disappear,and reappearduringthe courseof the
processof acquiringwriting,she arguedfor the existence
of a developmentaldialecticthatis both continuousand
discontinuous.
Thereare pointson this evolutionarypaththatare
similarto the stages in the developmentof writingput
forwardby Hendersonand Beers(1980) and Gentry
(1982).The latteridentifiedfive stages in children's
spelling.Duringthe firstor precommunicative
stage, children randomlycombinelettersand pseudoletterswithout
payingattentionto correspondencesbetween lettersand
sounds.Therewould appearto be similaritiesbetween
this initialstage and the firsttwo levels definedby
Ferreiro(1988)-that is to say the presyllabiclevels.
Duringthe second or semiphoneticstage, children
begin to phoneticallyrepresentsome of the components
in words by choosing letterswith which they are familiar.
The lettersthatare employedin this way may represent
both sounds and syllablesin the word in question.
This second stage has similaritiesto Ferreiro's
(1988) initialphases of the phonetizationof writingnamelythose thatinvolve syllabicwritingwith phonetization, in which childrenuse conventionallettersto
representone of the sounds in a syllable,and those that
writing.
correspondto syllabic-alphabetic
The thirdor phoneticstage is characterisedby a
systematiccorrespondencebetween lettersand sounds,
but withoutany respectfor orthographicconventions.It
is thus equivalentto the alphabeticwritingnoted by
Ferreiro(1988).
The next two stages formthe backdropto children'sprogressin relationto theirlanguage'sorthography-aspects thatFerreiro(1988) did not address.In this
transitionalphase childrenbegin to take accountof the
fact that the way in which they are supposed to write
may be influenced by morphemic factors. Finally they
reach the last stage of the process, which is to say that
they write correctly.
To the extent that it constitutes a form of oral language analysis, preconventional writing introduces a metalinguistic practice that undoubtedly has some important
consequences in learning the oral segments of words.
These consequences probably are enhanced by the
470
processof phonetizingwriting,in which children'sattemptsto matchthe lettersin and the sounds of words
constitutea concretesupportfor the gradualidentification of the successivephonemesthatformpartof the
words.Variousauthors(Stahl& Murray,1998;Treiman,
1998)consideredthatchildren'sinventedspellings,in
which a few conventionallettersrepresentsounds, stimulate phonemicawareness.Withinthe contextof invented
writing,the activationof lettersseems in turnto be govin the
erned by a word'sphonologicalcharacteristics,
sense thatit is easierwhen the words containphonetic
sequences thatactuallyrepresentthe namesof the letters
(Mann,1993;Quintero,1994).Set againstthis backactivitiesmay lead to a processof
ground,writing-related
or
appropriating constructingthe alphabeticprinciplevia
a complex developmentalinteractionbetween a child's
capabilityto segmentwords into phonemesand the use
of the graphicsupportunderlyingthe lettersin orderto
representthem. Consequentlychildren'spreconventional
writingmay constitutea way in which to organiseand
expand oralanalyticalcapabilitiesand promotean even
more preciseunderstandingof the relationshipsbetween
the oraland the graphicelements.This point of view can
be confirmedby Richgels'sstudy(1995),which showed
thatpreschool-agechildrenwho were classifiedas being
good at phonetizingin theirinventedwritingfound it
easierto readwords thatwere simplefroma phonetic
standpointthan did childrenwhose writtenoutputwas
not as rich.
Along similarlines, variousauthors(Adams,1998;
Mann,1993;Treiman,1998)began to view children'sinvented spellingsas an understandingof the alphabetic
principle,but the knowledgeaboutwrittencode that
childrenrevealin theirattemptsat writinghas often been
seen as justone more indicatorof phonologicalawareness (Mann,1993;Vale & Cary,1998).Consequently,few
studies(Alvarado,1998;Pontecorvo& Orsolini,1996;
Vernon,1998)have assessedthe relationshipbetween
the evolutionof knowledgeaboutwritingon the one
hand and progressin phonologicalskillson the other.
Alvarado(1998) tested childrenat differentconceptual levels in writingby givingthem the task of deleting
the initialphoneme in words presentedin both oraland
written form. As expected, children at the alphabetic level
achieved the highest success rates-around 50%in the
oral format and 95% in the written format. The children at
the syllabic-alphabetic and syllabic with conventional
sound correspondence conceptual levels attained similar
degrees of success-60% and 57%respectively-when
the words were presented to them in conjunction with
the written form. However, these levels fell significantly-to 16%and 8%---inthe exclusively oral format.
READING RESEARCH QUARTERLY
October/November/December
2002
37/4
The comparison between children whose writing is
governed by letter/syllable correspondence on a purely
random basis and those who choose conventional letters
with which to represent one of the sounds in a syllable
suggests the importance of the role of conventional letters as a factor that increases phonemic awareness. The
former group displayed success rates of only 15%and 7%
(with and without the written version) in the phonemic
test. Children at the presyllabic level achieved zero rates
of success in both versions of the tests.
SimilarlyVernon (1998) showed that children at a
less evolved conceptual level display fewer analytical responses to a task involving the segmentation of words
than do their more advanced counterparts. This study
also confirmed that children perform better when they
are asked to segment words if the words are presented to
them in a written format rather than an oral format.
These studies, which show that phonological capabilities gradually improve until alphabetic understanding
of writing is achieved, do suffer from a limitation as they
centre their analysis on a single phonological task and
consequently do not look upon phonological awareness
as a multidimensional ability (Schatschneider, Francis,
Foorman, Fletcher, & Mehta, 1999; Stahl & Murray,1994;
Stanovich, 1992). This point of view may, however, be
put into perspective if we bear in mind the fact that some
elementary forms of phonological awareness, such as
sensitivity to syllables and rhymes and the capability to
detect the initial phonemes in words, can develop more
or less spontaneously over the course of the preschool
years (Libermanet al., 1974; Treiman, 1992). More sophisticated phonological skills, such as segmentation or
phonemic deletion, may be stimulated at these ages by
training programs that are oriented toward fostering this
type of capacity (Goswami & Bryant, 1990; Lundberg,
1991; Troia, 1999).
It is also important to mention that some-albeit not
many-studies have shown that phonological training
leads to progress in children's invented writing (Manrique,
1997; Tangel & Blachman, 1992). Manrique conducted a
phonological intervention program in a preschool class
that included rhyming games, games that involved the initial and final sounds of words, and phonemic analysis
games. At the end of the year, 65% of the children not
only had evolved significantly in terms of their phonological skills, but also were capable of writing words and
texts in accordance with alphabetic criteria.
If the results obtained by Alvarado (1998) and
Vernon (1998) are combined with the latest set of data, it
is possible to see that children's phonological abilities
may have some effect on the way in which they look
upon writing and that their phonological skills may develop as they build up more knowledge about written
Phonological
skills and writing
code. It seems to us thatthis hypotheticalreciprocalability-enhancingactionhas not yet receivedmuch attention
fromresearchersand thatit is thereforeuseful to analyse
it, startingwith the initiallevels of knowledgeaboutwriting and workingwithinthe frameworkof a multidimensional concept of phonologicalabilities.
Researchquestions
We have formulatedthe followingresearchquestions:
1. Willthe conceptualhypothesesaboutwrittenlanguage of presyllabicchildrenevolve when they are subjectedto a writingtrainingprogramintendedto alterthe
way in which they representwriting?
2. Willthe conceptualhypothesesaboutwrittenlanguage of presyllabicchildrenevolve when they are subjectedto a phonologicaltrainingprogramdesignedto
work on syllabicunits?
3. Arethere any differencesbetween the two types
of trainingin termsof the effectsthey have on the conceptualhypothesesaboutwrittenlanguage?
4. Willthe phonologicalskillsof presyllabicchildren undergoevolutionwhen they are subjectedto a
writingtrainingprogramintendedto alterthe way in
which they representwriting?
5. Willthe phonologicalskillsof presyllabicchildren undergoevolutionwhen they are subjectedto a
phonologicaltrainingprogramdesignedto work on
syllabicunits?
6. Arethereany differencesbetween the two types
of trainingin termsof the effectsthey have on phonological skills?
Method
Participants
Participantswere 90 middle-class Portuguese children with an average age of 5 years and 8 months, a standard deviation of 3 months, a lower limit of 5 years and 1
month, and an upper limit of 6 years and 4 months. They
attended various kindergartensand had not received any
formal training in reading and writing. In those kindergarten classes there were no regular classroom activities
or instructionrelating to phonological awareness or invented spelling. None of the children knew how to
read-a fact that was verified by means of individual
reading tests. All children's names are pseudonyms.
Only children who displayed presyllabic invented
spellings in the pretest were selected for the study. The
children were then randomly divided up into three
groups-two experimental groups and one control
group.
471
Figure 1
Example of Samuel's presyllabic writing
C)TU
(URSO)
(RATO)
oB
Nineteen children ended up not taking part in the
entire experiment and were therefore eliminated from the
results. The final groups were thus made up as follows:
experimental group 1 (N= 21); experimental group 2
(N= 23); control group (N= 27).
Design
This was an experimental study in which children
were assessed on a pre- and posttest basis, both in terms
of the way in which they saw written language and of
their phonological skills. In between the pre- and the
posttests, experimental group 1 was subjected to a writing training program intended to lead to a restructuring
of the way in which they saw and employed written language, while experimental group 2 was subjected to a
phonological training program designed to work on syllabic units. The control group took part in exercises involving the categorisation of geometric figures in
accordance with criteriasuch as shape, size, and colour.
The experimental- and control-group programs bea
gan week after the pretest and lasted for 2 weeks. In
order to ensure that any progress made was of a lasting
nature, the posttest was carried out 1 month after the
programs were concluded.
Because a knowledge of letters can serve as an intermediary and an instrument that makes it easier to become aware of phonemic entities (Stahl & Murray,1998;
Treiman & Cassar, 1997), we checked how many letters
the children were familiarwith. We also determined the
level of their intelligence.
Procedure
Adult: Tryto write urso(bear).
The evaluation of children's conceptual levels
In order to assess the children's conceptual levels,
we asked them to spell their name and then to spell a set
of words to the best of their ability. After spelling each
472
word they were asked to read what they had written. The
verbal responses that frequently accompanied the act of
spelling were recorded.
We dictated 21 pairs of words: words of identical
size from a linguistic point of view but refer to items of
different size (e.g., urso/rato [bear/mouse]); words of a
different size from a linguistic perspective but refer to
items of a similar size (e.g., hipop6tamo/boi [hippopotamus/ox]); and singular and plural pairs of words (see
Appendix A).
These choices were designed to enable us to understand whether the reasoning behind the children's writing
was based on the items to which the words refer, or on
the linguistic characteristicsof the words themselves. If a
child used nonlinguistic or presyllabic criteriato govern
his or her writing, it might be expected that inasmuch as
bears are big animals and mice are small ones, he or she
wrote the word urso (bear) with more or bigger letters
than he or she used for the word rato (mouse) and justified his or her choice by referringto the size of the reference items. If the form of reasoning that governs the
writing was linguistic, we expected that both words
would be written using approximately the same number
of letters and that this would be justified (for example)
with reference to the size of the words concerned and especially to the number of syllables they contain.
Our classification of the children's responses was
inspired by the classification grids drawn up by AlvesMartins(1994), Besse (1995), and Ferreiro(1988). We employed the following categories: presyllabic writing,
syllabic writing without phonetization, and syllabic writing with phonetization.
Presyllabic writing. This category was used to classify spellings in which the children made no attempt to
connect oral and written language. In spelling the different words children took account of factors such as a minimum number of letters for each word and a different
combination of letters with which to discriminate between different words. The way in which they spelled
some words took account of the properties of the reference items-for example, by using more letters for words
that referred to large items. Generally speaking, the children did not verbalise at all when spelling, and they read
their words globally, as a single whole. Figure 1 shows
the writing of Samuel, a presyllabic child.
The following are some extracts from Samuel's
interview.
Samuel: (Writes five letters.)
Adult: Now readwhat you wrote to me and show me
with your finger.
Samuel: Urso(reads globally).
Adult: Whydid you writethe word ursolike that?
READINGRESEARCHQUARTERLY October/November/December
2002
37/4
Samuel: Because ursois a big animal.
Adult: Now write rato(mouse).
Samuel: (Writes2 letters.)
Adult: Now readwhat you wrote to me and show me
with your finger.
Samuel: Rato(readsglobally).
Adult: Why did you writethe word ratolike that?
Samuel: Because ratois a littleanimalit has to be written
with a few letters.
Syllabic writing withoutphonetization. This categowas
used for spellings in which the correspondence
ry
between oral and written language was based on syllabic
units. When they produced this type of spelling, the children often syllabically segmented what was said to them
orally before they spelled anything and then represented
each syllable using a random letter. They read the words
syllabically.
Now let us look at some extracts from an interview
with Isabel who wrote syllabically. Figure 2 portrays
some of her writing.
Adult: Tryto write urso.
Isabel: Ur(writesI ) so (writesA).
Adult: Now read what you wrote to me and show me
with your finger.
Isabel: Ur(points to the I) so (points to the A).
Adult: Why did you writethe word ursolike that?
Isabel: (Readsthe word out again,pointingto each of
the letters)ur/so,there are two.
Adult: Now write rato.
Isabel: Ra (writesS) to (writesF).
Adult: Now readwhat you wrote to me and show me
with your finger.
Isabel: Ra (pointsto the S) to (points to the F).
Adult: Why did you writethe word ratolike that?
Isabel: (Readsthe word out again,pointingto each of
the letters)ra/to,there are two.
Syllabic writing with phonetization. This category
was reserved for syllabic writing where the children
phonically analysed part of the spoken words by selecting a letter from their repertoire that adequately represented the sounds they had identified. They continued to
read words syllabically, and in their verbal output the
children tried to identify not only the quantity of letters
that they ought to use, but also-in some cases-the
most suitable ones.
Finally we have some extracts from an interview
with Pedro in which some of the words were written in
this way. Figure 3 illustrates his writing.
Adult: Now write urso.
Pedro: Ur...ur...u(writesU) so (writesI).
Adult: Now read me what you have justwrittenand
show me with your finger.
Pedro: Ur/so(points to the U and then to the I).
Phonological
skills and writing
Figure 2
Example of Isabel's syllabic writing without
phonetization
(URSO)
(RATO)
Figure 3
Example of Pedro's syllabic writing with
phonetization
I
(URSO)
(RATO)
Adult:
Pedro:
Now write rato.
Ra...ra... ra, it's A (writes A), ra/tu, tu it's U
(writes U).
Adult: Now read me what you have justwrittenagain
and show me with your finger.
Pedro: Ra/to(pointsfirstto the firstletterand then to
the last one).
When we analysed the protocols, we calculated the
number of words in each of the categories. On the basis
of this analysis we determined the percentile distribution
of the various types of spelling between the protocols
and classified the children by evolutionary level-for a
child to qualify for a given level, around 90% of his or
her spellings had to match the applicable criteria.
The evaluation ofchildren 'sphonological awareness
In order to evaluate the children's phonological
awareness, we gave them a battery of four phonological
subtests with differing levels of difficulty (so as to address
the heterogeneous nature of this particularcapability).
When we created this battery of tests, we sought to take
account of the size of the units (syllables and phonemes)
and the phonological properties of the initial phonemes
473
in each word. The battery included two classification and
two deletion tests (see Appendix B).
Each of the classification tests, one of which was
based on the initial syllable and the other on the initial
phoneme, was composed of 14 items preceded by 2 examples. In each item the children were presented with
four drawings representing four oral words (there were
no written words). In the case of the initial-syllableclassification test two of the words in each item began with
the same syllable, while the other two started with different ones (e.g., girafa [giraffe]/panela [cooking pot]/cenoura [carrot]/palhago[clown]), and the children had to
identify the words that began with the same syllable. In
the initial-phoneme classification test two of the words in
each item began with the same phoneme, whereas the
others started with different ones, and the children again
had to identify the words that began with the same one
(e.g., fivela [buckle]/telhado [roofll/janela[window]/
fogueira [bonfire]).
The children were given the following instructions
when they took the initial-syllable classification test:
"We'regoing to play a kind of cardgame in which we
have to find two words thatbegin with the same syllable,
fromamonga totalof fourwords. First,can you tell me
The childwas
what each of these drawingsrepresents?"
shown the firstexample.Whenhe or she found it difficult
to identifyany of the words thatcorrespondedto the
drawings,the researchernamedthe word and asked the
child to repeatit as manytimes as was necessaryuntilhe
or she was automaticallyable to identifyall fourwords.
The adultthen went on by saying,"Twoof these words in
the drawingsbegin with the same syllable.Let'splay, but
this time it doesn'tcount-it's justfor you to see how the
game works.Whichare the two words thatbegin with the
When the childgot it wrong the researcher
same syllable?"
asked him or her to pronounceall the words slowly, emphasisingthe initialsyllableand leadinghim or her to realise the similaritybetween the initialsyllablesof two of
the words.This procedurewas then repeatedfor the second example.When both exampleswere over the researcheradded, "NowthatI've told you how the game is
played, let's start.But be careful-this time you have to
find out which words begin with the same syllableon
your own."
The instructions for the initial-phoneme classification test were similar. They only differed in one respectinstead of saying that the children had to find the words
that began with the same syllable, they were told that
they had to find those that began with the same sound.
When a child got the first example item wrong, the researcher asked him or her to pronounce all the words
slowly, emphasising the initial phoneme and leading him
or her to see the similarity between the initial phonemes
474
in two of the words. This procedure was then repeated
for the second example item.
In the tests involving the deletion of an initial syllable and an initial phoneme, the children were asked to
pronounce in isolation each of the syllables or phonemes
of words that were presented in a figurative format (i.e.,
each oral word was again represented by a drawing), and
to say what remained of the word without the initial segment, the deletion of which might result in something
that was not a word in terms of the Portuguese language
(e.g., no/ta; ta, in the initial-syllable deletion test; or r/io;
io, in the initial-phoneme deletion test). The initial-syllable deletion test was made up of 14 items, half of which
were composed of two syllables each and the other half
of three syllables each, preceded by two examples; the
initial-phoneme deletion test was made up of 24 items
(also preceded by two examples), half of which were
composed of monosyllabic words and the remainder
were disyllabic.
The children were given the following instructions
for the initial-syllable deletion test:
"Doyou see these cards?Do you know what this drawing
shows?"When a child found it difficultto identifyone of
the words thatcorrespondedto the drawings,the researchernamedthe word and got the child to repeatit as
manytimes as was necessaryuntilhe or she was automatically able to identifythe word. "Thisgame is like this:
We'regoing to take away the firstsyllableof the word and
say what'sleft. Let'splay, but this time it doesn'tcountit'sjustfor you to see how the game works.Tell me what
the firstsyllableof the word nota (banknote) is."If the
child was unableto answer,he or she was asked to pronounce the word slowly along with the researcherin such
a way as to isolatethe firstsyllable."Nowlet's take the no
away fromthe word. What'sleft of the word nota if we
take away no?"In the event thatthe childwas unableto
complywith the instructionthe researchersaid, "Well,if
we take away the firstsyllableof the word nota we're left
with ta. Listencarefully:no/ta; ta."The same procedure
was then followed for the word in the second example,after which the researcheradded, "Nowthatyou've understood how the game works,let's play for real.You're
going to tell me the firstsyllablein each word and then
say what is left of the word once we have takenaway this
littlebit."
The instructions for the initial-phoneme deletion
test were similar, except that instead of saying that the
idea was to take away the first syllable, we said that we
were going to take away the first sound. In the event that
a child was unable to complete one of the example items
correctly, he or she was asked to pronounce the word
slowly along with the researcher in such a way as to isolate the initial phoneme and then say what was left of the
word without that sound.
READINGRESEARCHQUARTERLY October/November/December
2002
37/4
Whenever the researcher was in any doubt as to
whether the child had understood the term "syllable,"he
or she always used the term "littlebit" instead. In all the
tests one point was awarded for each correct answer.
The evaluation of children's knowledge of letters
In order to determine how many and which letters
the children were familiarwith, they were give a set of
cards bearing the letters of the alphabet in capitals (K, W,
and Ywere excluded, as they are not formally part of the
Portuguese alphabet), which they were asked to name.
They were then asked to write down the letters they had
recognised. The total number of possible points in this
test varied between 0 and 23.
The evaluation of children's intelligence
The level of the children's intelligence was evaluated using the coloured version of Raven's Progressive
Matricestest (1947-as revised in 1956), because it is not
very dependent on verbal aspects.
The writing training program
The writing training program was organised around
situations that led the child to think about the rules of
writing from two points of view: His or hers, and that of
a hypothetical boy or girl of the same age whose
spellings were syllabic. In each session the children were
asked to spell a set of words to the best of their ability.
After spelling each word they were asked to read out
what they had written. They were then confronted with
the same word, as spelled in accordance with syllabic criteria-one letter/one syllable correspondence-by a hypothetical child from another school. The children were
asked to say which of the two versions was better. Before
deciding, they had to try to slowly read both what they
had written and the writing of the hypothetical child and
to try to justify why they had written the word as they
had and why the other child had written it in another
way. From the second session onward, in addition to
these instructions, before they wrote down the words the
children were asked to say in advance how many letters
they were going to use.
In this way, we created the conditions that would
arouse a cognitive conflict in the children between two
approaches to writing, inasmuch as the situation itself led
them to confront the two ways of writing and to try to
expound the hypotheses underlying each of the two approaches. The objective of these spelling situations was
to lead children with presyllabic writing to evolve toward
syllabic writing.
Phonological
skills and writing
Thephonological training program
The phonological training program involved oral
and aural exercises of identification, segmentation, and
manipulation of syllabic units. It did not include any written words. In designing it we sought to take account of a
number of principles that we considered to be important
to research in this domain. They included factors such as
the use of drawings to represent sounds (Ball &
Blachman, 1991); the use of an object to denote each
sound (O'Connor,Jenkins, & Slocum, 1993); the modelling of sounds, with their reproduction by the child and
with the researcher calling attention to their articulatory
characteristics(Lie, 1991); explicit instructions for the
tasks employed; and the graduation of linguistic complexity, particularlyin terms of the size of the words and
the articulatoryproperties of the phonemes.
The following tasks were performed over the
course of the eight sessions that made up the training
program:
1. Syllabic segmentation games in which the child
first had to pronounce each syllabic unit and denote it by
beating time with a pencil, and then identify the number
of syllables that made up each word. The children were
given the oral words in a series of four. Each series was
composed of words that varied between one and four
syllables (e.g., sol [sun]/fada [fairy]/camelo [camel]/borboleta [butterfly]).Whenever a child was unable to carry out
the task, the correct response was modelled and he or
she was again asked to reproduce it.
2. Initial-syllableclassification games in which the
child had to identify two words that began with the same
syllable from among four that were presented to him or
her in figurative form (each oral word was represented
by a drawing). This task was organised into 12 series.
Each one was made up of six items of four words each.
In one of the series the initial syllables were vowels (e.g.,
anel [ring]/irmd [sister]/avd [grandfather]/6culos[spectacles]; in the others the target syllables in all six items began with the same consonant (e.g., bota [boot]/carro
[car]/rato [mouse]/raquete [racket];or circo [circus]/rosa
[rose]/mapa [map]/rede [net]).
We employed the following procedure in playing
this type of game. The children were asked to indicate
the initial syllable of each of the four words in such a
way as to identify words that possessed the same initial
syllable. When the children found it difficult to carry out
this operation, the researcher got them to repeat the
word slowly and to stop after they had said the first syllable. Once they were able to isolate the syllable they
pointed to each of the words, indicating only the first syllable and identifying the words that shared the common
initial segment.
475
Table 1
Number of children whose writing was
presyllabic and syllabic at the posttest
Experimental Group 1 (n = 21)
Experimental Group 2 (n = 23)
Control Group (n = 27)
Children with
presyllabic writing
Childrenwith
syllabic writing
0
0
26
21
23
1
3. Initial-syllabledeletion games in which the children were asked to pronounce in isolation each of the
syllables of 20 words, and to say what remained of the
word without the initial segment. It should be noted that
in order to facilitate the children's task, the deletion of
the first syllable of the words that we used always gave
rise to a new word. For 10 of the words we used a game
with a figurative support (each oral word was represented by a drawing). For the remaining 10, the words were
presented orally (without pictures).
We employed the following procedure in playing
this type of game. The children were told that there was
a word hidden within another word and that they could
find it if they took away the first "littlebit."They had to
try to find out which of three words that were presented
to them corresponded to the hidden one (e.g., for the
word fivela [buckle] they were given the words vela [candle]/vaca [cow]/dado [die];while for espinha [bone] they
were given linha [line]/pinha [pine]/pote [pot]). Even
when the child was immediately successful, the researcher modelled the operation that the child had just
carried out in order to show him or her that he or she
was right. When the child was unable to give the correct
answer, the researcher led him or her to identify the first
syllable, modelled the deletion operation, and made the
child repeat it.
The control group program
We organised a set of exercises with the control
group using material of the logical blocks type. The children were asked to classify geometric shapes in accordance with criteriasuch as identical shape, size, or
colour. The three programs involved eight approximately
15-minute sessions and were individually conducted by
us with the children over the course of 2 weeks.
Results
We will begin by presenting the data concerning the
knowledge of letters and the level of intelligence of the
children in the three groups. We carried out ANOVAwith
the group as the independent variable and either the
number of letters known or the level of intelligence as the
476
dependent variables. We obtained a figure of 1(2,68) =
.02, p = .978 for the number of letters known and 1(2,68)
= 1.80, p = 1.78 for the level of intelligence. There were
no statisticallysignificant differences between the three
groups.
In order to assess the effect of the two types of
training on the children's concepts of written language
(research questions 1, 2, and 3), we looked at the number of children in the three groups whose writing was
more highly evolved in conceptual terms at the posttest
moment. Table 1 suggests that both forms of training
contributed to an expansion of the children's knowledge
about written code, inasmuch as the posttest writing of
the children in both these groups was systematically governed by linguistic principles. When we look at this table
we do not need any statisticalprocedure to tell us that
the two types of training had a similar impact in terms of
their role as a factor in the progress of the children's concepts about written language.
Analysis of the children's individual protocols at the
posttest shows that all the children in the two experimental groups began to govern their attempts at writing in accordance with syllabic criteria.Only two children in
experimental group 1 and four in experimental group 2
began to phonetize some of the syllables in the words.
These phonetization procedures principally began to be
applied to the first syllable in a word and were used
more often in relation to vowels than to consonants. At
the same time they almost always occurred with words in
which the initial syllable was accentuated. These children
were the ones who were familiarwith the greatest number of letters.
The children in the control group continued to
obey presyllabic criteriain their spelling. Just one child in
this group spelled a few words using syllabic criteria
(more precisely, 22% of all the words she wrote).
Figure 4, which contains extracts from Vera's (a
member of experimental group 1) protocol, exemplifies
the presyllabic forms of writing that she produced in the
pretest.
Adult: Now write urso(bear).
Vera: (Writesin silence.)
Adult: Now readme what you have justwrittenand
show it to me with your finger.
Vera: (Readsgloballywhile simultaneouslypassingher
fingerover the writing.)
Adult: Now write rato(mouse).
Vera: (Againwritesin silence.)
Adult: Readit to me againand show me with your finger at the same time as you read.
Vera: (Readsas before.)
Adult: Why did you use differentlettersfor ursoand
rato?
READINGRESEARCHQUARTERLY October/November/December
2002
37/4
Figure 4
Exampleof Vera'spresyllabicwritingin
the pretest
Table 2 Meanscores and standarddeviationsfor the
initial-syllableclassificationtest
Pretest
hc T
Ei
ijr
D
(URSO)
Experimental Group 1
Experimental Group 2
Control Group
0
Vera:
(RATO)
(URSO)
(RATO)
Now let us look at extracts from Vera's posttest,
which are illustrativeof the characteristicsof the children's spellings at that point. Vera spelled all the words
in accordance with the syllabic hypothesis. Figure 5
shows some of the words she spelled.
Now write urso.
Adult:
Ur(writesO) so, so (writesA).
Vera:
Now read me what you wrote and show me
Adult:
with your finger.
Ur(points to the O) so (points to the A); it
Vera:
has to be two, don'tyou see...ur/so(readsthe
word again).
Adult:
Now write rato.
Ra (writesO) to (writesS).
Vera:
Now readwhat you wrote to me and show
Adult:
me with your finger.
Ra (points to the O) to (points to the S).
Vera:
Researcher:Why did you writethe word ratolike that?
Vera:
(Readsthe word out again,pointingto each
of the letters)ra/to,there are two of them.
In order to assess the effect of the two types of
training on the children's phonological skills (research
questions 4, 5, and 6) we used ANOVAwith repeated
measures to compare the children's performance in the
skills and writing
SD
Mean
SD
11.47
9.43
10.55
2.24
3.57
2.72
13.85
13.73
12.11
.35
.31
2.27
Table 3 Meanscores and standarddeviationsfor the
initial-phonemeclassificationtest
Pretest
Experimental Group 1
Experimental Group 2
Control Group
Posttest
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
5.95
6.73
5.33
2.61
2.41
1.50
8.28
9.86
5.60
1.90
1.96
2.10
This test was composed of 14 items.
Becausea bear is a bear-it's not the same thing
as a mouse, is it?
Phonological
Mean
This test was composed of 14 items.
Figure 5 Exampleof Vera'ssyllabicwritingin
the posttest
07
Posttest
various phonological tasks that the children were set on
the pre- and posttest. In order to deepen our understanding of the differences between the groups, we subsequently carried out a post hoc analysis using a version of
the Tukey test that applies to situations involving different-sized groups. In those cases in which we could not
assume that variances were homogeneous, we performed
the same analysis using the Games-Howell procedure.
We will now present the children's results in each
of the tests. In the initial-syllable classification test, the
descriptive statistics included in Table 2 suggest that the
results obtained by the two experimental groups on the
posttest are close to one another and slightly better than
those of the control group.
The results of the ANOVAreveal that there was
a
only statisticallysignificant evolution in the children's
results in this classification test between the two evaluation moments, F(1,68) = 68.84, p = .001. There were no
statisticallysignificant differences between the groups.
When we come to the initial-phoneme classification
an
test,
analysis of Table 3 enables us to see that the avthat
the two experimental groups obtained at the
erages
are
close to each other and are higher than those
posttest
of the control group. There had not been much difference between the average results achieved by the three
groups in the pretest. The results of the ANOVA show
that the moment F(1,68) = 46.72, p = .001; group F(2,68)
= 16.38, p = .001; and group x moment interaction
F(2,68) = 9.72, p = .001 variables produced statistically
significant effects.
The post hoc procedure revealed that both types of
training led to statisticallysignificant effects on the chil-
477
Table 4 Meanscores and standarddeviationsfor the
deletiontest
initial-syllable
Pretest
Experimental Group 1
Experimental Group 2
Control Group
Posttest
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
4.76
3.69
3.59
4.42
3.59
3.72
12.90
11.86
6.70
1.99
2.18
5.23
This test was composed of 14 items.
Table 5 Meanscores and standarddeviationsfor the
initial-phonemedeletiontest
Pretest
Mean
Experimental Group 1
Experimental Group 2
Control Group
3.14
5.52
1.44
Posttest
SD
6.49
6.49
3.64
Mean
SD
3.95
13.78
1.74
7.12
7.12
2.95
This test was composed of 24 items.
dren'sperformancein this phonologicaltask,inasmuch
as therewere statisticallysignificantdifferencesbetween
the resultsachievedby the two experimentalgroupson
the one hand and those of the controlgroup on the other, whereastherewere no differencesbetween the results
of the experimentalgroupsthemselves.
The averagesachievedin the initial-syllable
deletion
test (see Table4) clearlyindicatethatthe two experimental groupsobtainedmuch betterresultson the posttest
than did the controlgroup.The same table also shows
thatthe averagesthe threegroupsscored on the pretest
did not differgreatlyfromone another,albeitExperimentalGroup1 did slightlybetterthanthe others.
The resultsof the ANOVAcorroboratethe idea that
the children'sresultsin this test evolved significantly,
froma statisticalpoint of view, between the pretestand
posttestmomentsF(1,68) = 202.44,p = .001.They also
show thatthereare statisticallysignificantdifferencesdependingon the groupF(2,68) = 8.48,p = .001 and the
momentx group interactionF(2,68) = 14.05,p = .001.
Post hoc analysisshowed thatboth types of trainingproduced an impact,inasmuchas we recordedstatistically
significantdifferencesbetween the experimentalgroups
on the one hand and the controlgroupon the other,but
not between the two experimentalgroups.
As regardsthe initial-phonemedeletiontest, we can
see fromTable5 thatexperimentalgroup 2's resultsin
the posttestwere farsuperiorto those of the othertwo
groupsand therewas a considerableevolutionbetween
the two evaluationmoments.The averagesattainedby
478
the other two groups were practically identical on the
pretest and the posttest.
The results of the ANOVAshow that the children's
performance in the initial-phoneme deletion test evolved
significantly, from a statisticalpoint of view, between the
pretest and posttest F(1,68) = 29.95, p = .001. The group
F(2,68) = 13.52, p = .001 and moment x group interaction
F(2,68) = 20.43, p = .001 variables also produced substantial effects. Post hoc analysis showed that this difference
in evolution is due to the impact of the phonological
training, inasmuch as the averages that the members of
experimental group 2 obtained in this test differ significantly, from a statisticalpoint of view, from those obtained by experimental group 1 and the control group.
On the other hand the same analysis reveals no differences between the averages obtained by experimental
group 1 and those of the control group.
Discussion
One of the aspects we sought to assess in this study
was to what extent presyllabic children who are subjected to a writing training program evolve, not only in their
conceptualisations of writing, but also in their phonological skills.
Our data confirm that a writing training program
designed to alter the way in which presyllabic children
see and use writing such that they begin to employ syllabic criterianot only leads presyllabic children to
progress in their invented spellings, but also improves
their phonological skills. The children's performance in
phonological tasks on the posttest did indeed improve
significantly from a statistical point of view, both in terms
of their manipulation of syllabic units (as we can see
from the results they obtained in the initial-syllable deletion tests) and as regards their awareness of the presence
of common phonemic elements in different words
(shown by the results of the initial-phoneme classification
test). The progress we recorded in the latter test is particularly significant in light of the fact that children's ability
to identify common phonemes in different words is critical to an understanding of the alphabetic nature of writing (Stahl & Murray,1998). The results obtained in this
test also suggest that establishing systematic relationships
between segments of speech and written units may contribute to the conceptual emergence of phonemic entities
along lines that are similar to the way put forward by
Byrne and Fielding-Barnsley(1989), who looked upon
evolution in the performance of this task as concept formation.
The evolution we observed in the phonological
tasks, in the children who underwent the writing training
program, fits into the overall line of thought that Treiman
READINGRESEARCHQUARTERLY October/November/December
2002
37/4
(1998) and Adams (1998) suggested about the role invented writing plays in enabling children to grasp the
oral structure of words.
We also sought to investigate whether presyllabic
children who underwent a phonological training program
would evolve, not only in their phonological abilities, but
also in their conceptualisations of writing. The data we
obtained indicate that the phonological training program
led not only to statisticallysignificant progress in the children's phonological skills (as may be seen from the results they obtained in the initial-syllable deletion,
initial-phoneme classification, and initial-phoneme deletion tests), but also to changes in the characteristicsof
their invented spellings. Our study confirms the data that
Manrique (1997) and Tangel and Blachman (1992) produced that phonological training leads to progress in the
nature of children's invented spelling.
Finally, we sought to investigate whether the two
types of training would have identical effects on both
children's conceptualisations of writing and their phonological skills. When it came to the way in which the children represented writing, the data show that the two
types had equivalent effects (at the posttest all the children in both experimental groups had begun to govern
their writing in accordance with the syllabic hypothesis).
One element that warrants particularnote is the
case of the children who, besides beginning to write in
accordance with the syllabic hypothesis, also started to
employ conventional letters to represent some of the
sounds in words. As we said earlier, these children were
those members of the experimental groups who were familiar with the largest number of letters. Besides this, on
the pretest these children had achieved success rates that
were above the average scored by the other children in
the tests involving classification based on common
phonemes and the deletion of the initial phoneme. It was
probably the conjugation of their knowledge of letters
and a higher level of phonemic awareness that led to the
appearance of phonetization processes in these children.
The fact that phonetization procedures only occurred in these children confirms Byrne's (1998) view
about the need for coordination between knowledge of
letters and phonological abilities if children are to make
progress in learning the written code. However, in our
case we found that this was already true of children at a
less evolved level.
It is also important to add that in the relatively isolated examples of phonetization processes we came
across, vowels were used more often than consonants.
This can probably be explained by the fact that in
Portuguese the sound value of some vowels in syllables
corresponds to the name of the letter concerned (e.g., the
i in livro [book] or the u in urso [bear]).This match be-
Phonological
skills and writing
tween the name of a letter and its sound value occurs
much more often in vowels than it does in consonants.
This circumstance makes it easier to remember the letter
in question, given that various authors have shown that
one of the factors that influence children's ability to establish a relationship between letters and phonemes is
that the phoneme forms part of the name of the letter in
question (Mann, 1993; Treiman & Cassar, 1997; Treiman
et al., 1998).
As far as phonological skills are concerned, the data
show that there were both similarities and differences between the groups. Let us begin by emphasising the fact
that when set against the results of the control group,
neither of the two training programs seems to have had a
statisticallysignificant impact on the initial-syllable classification test. The relatively good results that the children
in the control group achieved in this test indicate that
these types of skills develop spontaneously over the
course of the preschool years and do not involve any development in the way in which children look at and use
writing, as has been shown in many other studies
(Libermanet al., 1974; Sim-Sim, 1997; Treiman, 1992).
At the same time the two programs had more or
less identical effects on the initial-syllable deletion test
and the initial-phoneme classification test, but not on the
initial-phoneme deletion test. In the latter the results obtained by the children who underwent the phonological
training program (experimental group 2) were clearly superior to those achieved by the group that took the writing training program (experimental group 1).
If we bear in mind the fact that the training only addressed syllabic units and did not include any form of activity aimed at appropriating segmental units, these
results raise a number of questions. There is not a lot of
literatureto help interpret them, given that when phonological intervention programs employ games with syllables or other suprasegmental units, most of them do so
as a prior step to the use of games that address phonemic
units (Byrne & Fielding-Barnsley, 1989; Lundberg, Frost,
& Peterson, 1988). Reviews of recent works on phonological training programs (Bus, Marinus,& Ijzendoorn,
1999; Troia, 1999), in which the characteristicsand the
impact of intervention programs conducted as part of the
last 20 years' research in this domain are analysed, do not
contain a single reference to a program that has been exclusively centred on syllabic units.
The results that the children in experimental group
2 obtained in the initial-phoneme deletion test may perhaps have been precipitated by the way in which the
deletion operation was modelled at syllable level. In light
of the fact that "the concept of phoneme identity is closely related to the ability to segment a part of a spoken
word" (Stahl & Murray,1998, p. 83), this aspect of the
479
program, which was coordinated with the children's
progress in forming the identity of phonemes, may have
contributed to the statisticallysignificant evolution in their
scores in this test. In addition, the progress we observed
in the performance of this task may also have been facilitated by the characteristicsof the test itself. The test included many items that were composed of monosyllabic
words in which the initial phoneme corresponds to the
onset of the syllable. Treiman (1992) said that in these
circumstances preschool-age children can achieve a certain amount of success in this test on the basis of the intrasyllabic components.
In summary, the results of this study confirm the
point of view put forward by various authors (Byrne, 1997,
1998; Pontecorvo & Orsolini, 1996; Stahl & Murray,1998;
Tolchinsky & Teberosky, 1998) who said that there is a
complex interactionbetween the processes involved in becoming aware of the oral units in speech and understanding the way in which written code works. This interaction,
which has primarilybeen noted when children begin to
see conventional correspondences between the sounds of
words and the letters they need to use (Adams, 1998;
Treiman, 1998), seems to be true even at earlier levels.
This conclusion is one of the most innovative aspects of the results we obtained, inasmuch as the interactive dynamic between oral learning processes and the
processes involved in understanding writing have previously been taken into consideration only when children
have begun to establish some graphic-phonetic correspondences in their preconventional writing (Treiman,
1998). Our study confirms that the skills that enable children to think about oral language and the way in which
children represent the written code begin to influence
one another at quite early stages.
This means that within the context of a language
like Portuguese, in which there are a lot of polysyllabic
words and open syllables of the consonant-vowel type,
phonological training programs that work on syllabic
units lead children to establish relationships between
speech and writing that are themselves based on syllabic
units. At the same time, inducing children to think about
syllabic components as units that codify writing favours
the development of phonological skills. It should be noted that this evolution also occurs at the level of the
awareness of phonemes-a skill that Byrne (1998) and
Stahl and Murray(1998) consider critical to grasping the
alphabetic characteristicsof writing.
Our study also reveals that there may be two ways
in which it is possible to transition from forms of presyllabic writing to syllabic writing formats: one derived from
the application to writing of the various ways to manipulate syllabic units, and one of reflection on the relation-
480
ship between the various parts of the written word and
the word as a whole.
From the pedagogical point of view our results
strengthen the stance that various authors (Adams, 1998;
Treiman, 1998) have taken about the importance of the
early stimulation of invented spelling activities to promote
both phonological awareness and the gradual learning of
the alphabetic principle. This implication becomes even
more importantif we remember that although they were
not yet establishing graphic-phonetic correspondences,
the children who underwent the syllabic writing program
experienced statisticallysignificant evolution in terms of
their awareness of phonemic entities-a capability that is
considered fundamental to an understanding of the alphabetic principle (Byrne, 1998; Stahl & Murray,1998)
At the same time the results of our study also seem
to indicate that programs designed to train syllabic unit
identification, segmentation, and manipulation capabilities may have an impact at the phoneme awareness level,
quite apart from the impact they have in terms of children's representations of written language. Given that
both these types of program are easily applied to children in this age group, it seems to us that they should be
more systematically used to help children understand the
alphabetic principle.
To conclude, we would also like to point out some
limitations of this study and a few directions for future research. One of the limitations is that we did not control
the general language ability of the children in our sample
group-a factor that can be important, given that some
studies have shown the existence of a relationship between this variable and phonological awareness (e.g.,
Webster & Plante, 1992).
Another limitation is that although there is some
similaritybetween the pre- and posttest and the training
programs, the children may simply have imitated learned
behaviours rather than reorganized their conceptual understandings. However, it seems to us that the fact that
the posttest was carried out a month after the training
program offers at least some guarantee that what took
place really was a number of conceptual reorganisations.
At the same time, the fact that the writing training program had an impact on the phonological tests and that
the phonological training program had an effect on the
children's representations of writing would appear to
confirm that the outcome did not constitute an imitation
of learned behaviours.
As far as developments for future research are concerned, it would be interesting to manipulate the number of letters with which the experimental groups are
familiar in such a way as to be able to specify the mediating function this knowledge performs in terms of the
training's effectiveness. Another pertinent aspect that re-
READINGRESEARCHQUARTERLY October/November/December
2002
37/4
quiresmore in-depthinvestigationin the futureis the
need to clarifythe capabilitiesthat,within the framework
of this type of intervention,may well have enabled some
childrento evolve to the use of phonetizationprocesses
in theirwriting.
REFERENCES
and learningaboutprint.
ADAMS,M.(1998). Beginningto read: Thinking
Cambridge,MA:MITPress.
M. (1998). Conscienciafonol6gicay escrituraen nihos
ALVARADO,
preescolares:la posibilidadde omitirel primersegmento. Lecturay Vida,3,
42-50.
M.(1994). Conceptualizag6esinfantissobre a linALVES-MARTINS,
guagem escritae aprendizagemda leitura.Discursos,8, 53-70.
ALVES-MARTINS,
M.,&MENDES,A. (1987). Evolugdodas conceptualiza96es infantissobre a escrita.AnalisePsicol6gica,5, 499-508.
ANDRADE,E., &VIANA,M.C.(1993). Sinerese, dierese e estruturasildbica. InActas do IXEncontroNacionalda AssociagdoPortuguesade Linguistica
(pp. 31-42). Coimbra,Portugal:APL.
B.A.(1991). Does phonemeawareness trainingin
BALL,E., &BLACHMAN,
make a differencein earlywordrecognitionand developmental
kindergarten
spelling?ReadingResearch Quarterly,24, 49-66.
BESSE,J.-M.(1993). De I'ecriture
productricei la psychogenese de la
langueecrite. InG. Chauveau,M.R6mond,&E. Rogovas-Chauveau(Eds.),
L'enfantapprenti-lecteur.
L'entr6edans le systbme ecrit(pp. 73-82). Collection
CRESASn- 10. InstitutNationalde RecherichePedagogique:L'Hartmattan.
BESSE,J.-M.(1995). Lecrit,1'6coleetl'illettrisme.Paris:Magnard.
BESSE,J.-M.(1996). An approachto writingin kindergarten.InC.
Pontecorvo,M.Orsolini,B. Burge,&L. Resnick(Eds.), Children'searlytext
construction(pp. 127-144). Hillsdale,NJ:Erlbaum.
V. (1999). PhonologicalawareBUS, A., MARINUS,
H., &IJZENDOORN,
ness and earlyreading:A meta-analysisof experimentaltrainingstudies.
Journalof EducationalPsychology,91, 403-414.
BYRNE,B. (1992). Studies in the acquisitonprocedurefor reading:
Rationale,hypothesesand data. InP. Gough, L. Ehri,&R. Treiman(Eds.),
Readingacquisition(pp.1-34). Hillsdale,NJ:Erlbaum.
of the alphabeticprinciple:Children'siniBYRNE,B. (1997). The learnability
tialhypotheses abouthow printrepresentsspoken language.Applied
Psycholinguistics,17, 401-426.
BYRNE,B. (1998). Thefoundationof literacy.Hove, England:Psychology
Press, Ltd.
R. (1989). Phonemicawareness and
BYRNE,B., &FIELDING-BARNSLEY,
letterknowledgein the child'sacquisitionof the alphabeticprinciple.Journalof
EducationalPsychology,81, 313-321.
E. (1994). Les cheminsde la
CHAUVEAU,
G., &ROGOVAS-CHAUVEAU.,
lecture.Paris:Magnard.
avantla la lettre.InH. Sinclair(Ed.),La
E. (1988). L'ecriture
FERREIRO,
produtiondes notationschez le jeune enfant(pp. 18-69). Paris:Presses
Universitairesde France.
J. (1993). Entrerdans l'6crit.Paris:Magnard.
FIJALKOW,
GENTRY,J.R. (1982). An analysisof developmentalspellingin GNYSAT
WRK.TheReading Teacher,36,192-200.
U. (1998). The roleof analogies in the developmentof word
GOSWAMI,
recognition.InJ.L.Metsala&L.C.Ehri(Eds.), Wordrecognitionin beginningliteracy(pp. 41-63). London:Erlbaum.
GOSWAMI,
U., &BRYANT,P. (1990). Phonologicalskillsand learningto
read Hove, England:Erlbaum.
HENDERSON,E.H.,&BEERS,J.W. (1980). Developmentaland cognitive
aspects of learningto spell:A reflectionof wordknowledge.Newark,DE:
International
ReadingAssociation.
LIBERMAN,
D., FISCHER,F.W.,&CARTER,B.
I.Y.,SHANKWEILER,
(1974). Readingand the awareness of linguisticsegments. Journalof
ChildPsychology, 18, 201-212.
Experimental
LIE,A. (1991). Effectsof a trainingprogramforstimulatingskillsin word
26, 234-250.
analysis in first-gradechildren.ReadingResearch Quarterly,
LUNDBERG, I. (1991). Phonemic awareness can be developed without
reading instruction. In S. Brady & D. Shankweiler (Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy (pp. 47-53). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Phonological
skills and writing
LUNDBERG,
I., FROST,J., &PETERSON,O.-P. (1988). Effectsof an extensive programforstimulatingphonologicalawareness in preschoolchildren.
23, 263-283.
ReadingResearch Quarterly,
MANN,V. (1993). Phonemeawareness and futurereadingability.Journal
of LearningDisabilities,26, 259-269.
A.M.B.(1997). Alfabetizaci6nemergente:DiferenciassocioMANRIQUE,
culturales.Unpublisheddoctoraldissertation,Universidadde Buenos Aires,
Buenos Aires,Argentina.
MORAlS,J. (1994).Lartde lire.Paris:EditionsOdileJacob.
B. (1998). Gainingalphabeticinsight:Is phonememanipulation
MURRAY,
skillor identityknowledgecausal? Journalof EducationalPsychology,90,
461-475.
O'CONNOR,R., JENKINS,J., &SLOCUM,T. (1993). Teachingphonological awareness to youngchildrenwithlearningdisabilities.ExceptionalChildren,
59, 532-546.
PERFETTI,
C.A.,BECK,I., BELL,L., &HUGUES,C. (1987). Phonemic
knowledgeand learningto readare reciprocal:A longitudinal
studyof firstgrade
children,Merrill-Palmer
Quarterly,33, 283-319.
M.(1996). Writingand writtenlanguage
PONTECORVO,
C., &ORSOLINI,
in children'sdevelopment.InC. Pontecorvo,M.Orsolini,B. Burge,&L. Resnick
(Eds.), Children'searlytextconstruction(pp.3-23). Hillsdale,NJ:Erlbaum.
C. (1988). Modesof differentiaPONTECORVO,
C., &ZUCHERMAGLIO,
tion inchildren'swritingconstruction.EuropeanJournalof Psychologyof
Education,3, 4, 371-398.
C. (1995). A passagem paraa alPONTECORVO,
C., &ZUCHERMAGLIO,
fabetizago: aprendizadonumconceitosocial. InY. Goodman(Ed.),Comoas
criangasconstroema leiturae a escrita(pp. 67-101). PortoAlegre,Brasil:Artes
Medicas.
G. (1994). El uso y funci6nde las letrasen el periodopre-alQUINTERO,
fab6tico,Lecturay Vida,15,28-38.
READ,C. (1971). Pre-schoolchildren'sknowledgeof Englishphonology.
HarvardEducationalReview,41, 1-34.
READ,C. (1975). Children'scategorizationof speech sounds in English.
Research Rep. No. 17. Urbana,IL:NationalCouncilof Teachersof English.
READ,C. (1986). Children'screativespelling.London:Routledge&Kegan
Paul.
D. (1995). Inventedspellingabilityand printedwordlearningin
RICHGELS,
kindergarten.ReadingResearchQuarterly,30, 96-109.
SCHATSCHNEIDER,
C., FRANCIS,D., FOORMAN,
B., FLETCHER,
J., &
of phonologicalawareness:An applicaMEHTA,P. (1999). The dimensionality
tionof item response theory.Journalof EducationalPsychology,91, 439-449.
SIM-SIM,I. (1997). Avaliagdoda linguagemoral:Umcontributopara o conhecimentodo desenvolvimentolinguisticodas criangasportuguesas.Lisboa,
Portugal:FundagdoCalousteGulbenkian.
B. (1994). Definingphonologicalawareness and its
STAHL,S., &MURRAY,
relationshipwithearlyreading.Journalof EducationalPsychology,86,
221-234.
B. (1998). Issues involvedin definingphonological
STAHL,S., &MURRAY,
awareness and its relationto earlyreading.InJ.L. Metsala&L.C.Ehri(Eds.),
Wordrecognitionin beginningliteracy(pp. 65-85). London:Erlbaum.
K. (1992). Speculationson the causes and consequences of
STANOVICH,
individualdifferencesin earlyreadingacquisiton.InP. Gough,L. Ehri,&R.
Treiman(Eds.), Readingacquisition(pp. 307-347). Hillsdale,NJ:Erlbaum.
SULZBY,E. (1986). Writingand readingas signs of oraland writtenlanguage organizationin youngchildren.InW.H.Teale & E. Sulzby(Eds.),
and reading(pp.50-87). Norwood,NJ:Ablex.
Emergentliteracy:Writing
SULZBY,E. (1989). Assessment of emergentwritingand children'slanin real time:
guage whilewriting.InL. Morrow&J. Smith(Eds.), Writing
Modellingproductionprocesses (pp. 83-109). New York:Longman.
TANGEL, D.M., & BLACHMAN,B.A. (1992). Effect of phoneme awareness
instruction on kindergarten invented spelling. Journal of Reading Behaviour, 24,
233-262.
TOLCHINSKY,L.L. (1995). Desenvolvimento da alfabetiza9&o e suas implica96es pedag6gicas: evid~ncias do sistema hebraico de escrita. In Y.
Goodman (Ed.), Como as crianpas constroem a leitura e a escrita (pp. 36-53).
Porto Alegre, Brasil: Artes Medicas.
TOLCHINSKY,L.L., & LEVIN,I. (1988). O desenvolvimento da escrita em
criangas israelenses pre-escolares. In E. Ferreiro & M. Palacio (Eds.), Os
processos de leitura e escrita (pp. 143-158). Porto Alegre, Brasil: Artes
Medicas.
481
A. (1998). The developmentof word
TOLCHINSKY,
L.L.,&TEBEROSKY,
segmentationand writingin two scripts.CognitiveDevelopment,13,1-24.
R. (1992).The roleof intrasyllabic
unitsin learningto readand
TREIMAN,
spell. InP. Gough,L. Ehri,& R. Treiman(Eds.), Readingacquisition(pp.
65-106). Hillsdale,NJ:Erlbaum.
R. (1998). Whyspelling?The benefitsof incorporating
TREIMAN,
spelling
intobeginningreadinginstruction.InJ.L.Metsala&L.C.Ehri(Eds.), Word
recognitionin beginningliteracy(pp. 289-313). London:Erlbaum.
de I'orthographe
en
TREIMAN,
R., &CASSAR,M.(1997). L'acquisition
anglais. InL. Rieben,M.Fayol,&C. Perfetti(Eds.), Des orthographeset leur
acquisition(pp.79-99). Paris:Delachauxet Niesti.
R., BRODERIK,
V., TINCOFF,R., RODRIGUEZ,
K.,MOUZAKI,
TREIMAN,
A., & FRANCIS,D. (1998). The foundationof literacy:Learningthe sounds of
letters.ChildDevelopment,69,1524-1540.
TROIA,G. (1999). Phonologicalawareness interventionresearch:A critical
reviewof the experimentalmethodology.ReadingResearchQuartely,34,
28-52.
VALE,A.P., &CARY,L. (1998). Escritainventadae detec9dofonemicaem
leitoresprincipiantes:
Preditoresdo desempenhoulteriorem leiturae
escrita.RevistaPortuguesade Psicologia,32, 29-56.
VERNON,S. (1998). Escrituray conscienciafonol6gicaen nihos hispanoparlantes.Infanciay Aprendizaje,81, 105-120.
VIGARIO,
M.,&FALE,I. (1993).A sflabado portugu6s:Umadescrigaoe
algumasconsiderag6esde ordemte6rica.InActas do IXEncontroNacionalda
AssociagAoPortuguesade Lingufstica(pp.465-478). Coimbra,Portugal:APL.
WEBSTER,P.E., &PLANTE,A.S. (1992). Effectsof phonologicalimpairmenton word,syllableand phonemesegmentationand reading.Language,
Speech, and Hearing,23,176-182.
ReceivedMarch5, 2001
FinalrevisionreceivedNovember6, 2001
AcceptedJanuary7, 2002
APPENDIX A
Words used to evaluate children's conceptual levels
Pairs of words that are of an identical size from a syllabic point of view, but which refer to items that
possess different sizes
urso/rato (bear/mouse)
vaca/mosca (cow/fly)
galo/pinto (cock/chick)
tigre/sapo (tiger/toad)
elefante/lagarto (elephant/lizard)
dinossauro/borboleta (dinosaur/butterfly)
cavalo/formiga (horse/ant)
Pairs of words that are of a different size from a syllabic perspective, but which refer to items of a similar size
raposa/cao (fox/dog)
hipop6tamo/boi (hippopotamus/ox)
leopardo/dragdo (leopard/dragon)
macaco/foca (monkey/seal)
sardinha/ra (sardine/frog)
gafanhoto/vespa (grasshopper/wasp)
abelha/pulga (bee/flea)
Singular/pluralpairs of words
ovelha/ovelhas (sheep/sheep)
zebra/zebras (zebra/zebras)
janela/janelas (window/windows)
uva/uvas (grape/grapes)
navio/ navios (ship/ships)
igreja/igrejas(church/churches)
gato/gatos (cat/cats)
482
READINGRESEARCHQUARTERLY October/November/December
2002
37/4
APPENDIX B
Battery of phonological
tests
Initial-syllable classification test
Cards with representative drawings:
Examples:
bolo/nariz/navio/moinho (cake/nose/ship/mill)
igreja/ouro/arroz/iogurte (church/gold/rice/yogurt)
Items:
uva/asa/unha/ilha (grape/wing/nail/island)
enxada/ourigo/agulha/apito
(hoe/urchin/needle/whistle)
rolo/sapo/figo/roupa (roll/toad/fig/clothes)
coelho/machado/piano/macaco
(rabbit/axe/piano/monkey)
garrafa/galinha/pijama/moeda
(bottle/hen/pyjama/coin)
tesoura/casaco/moinho/cavalo
(scissors/coat/mill/horse)
vaso/pipa/mesa/vaca (vase/barrel/table/cow)
chupa/fato/faca/bico (lolly pop/suit/knife/brook)
janela/menina/tomate/torrada
(window/girl/tomato/toast)
girafa/panela/cenoura/palhago
(giraffe/pot/carrot/clown)
bota/jarro/ninho/bola (boot/jar/nest/ball)
saco/sapo/burro/mota (bag/toad/donkey/motorcycle)
laranja/medalha/lagarto/pinheiro
(orange/medal/lizard/pine)
sino/data/dado/folha (bell/date/die/leaf)
Initial-phoneme classification test
Cardswith representative drawings:
Examples:
colher/chave/chuva/bola (spoon/key/rain/ball)
j6ia/n6/jipe/pd (jewel/knot/jeep/shovel)
Items:
alce/urso/arca/ovo (moose/bear/arch/egg)
orelha/alface/drvore/igreja (ear/lettuce/tree/church)
raposa/regador/viola/boneca (fox/watering
can/guitar/doll)
mala/peixe/chucha/mota (bag/fish/doll/motorcycle)
sumo/gola/leite/gato (juice/collar/milk/cat)
buzina/cegonha/vassoura/veado
(horn/stork/broom/deer)
serra/copo/cama/lupa (saw/glass/bed/magnifying
glass)
fivela/telhado/janela/fogueira
(buckle/roof/window/bonfire)
boca/tigre/selo/tacho (mouth/tiger/stamp/pot)
pato/p ra/milho/chuva (duck/pear/corn/rain)
tijolo/bolacha/seringa/banana
(brick/biscuit/syringe/banana)
cebola/toalha/gaveta/cigarro
(onion/towel/drawer/cigarette)
lata/luva/roda/fita (can/glove/wheel/ribbon)
Phonological
skills and writing
desenho/camisa/domin6/novelo
(drawing/shirt/domino/ball of wool)
Initial-syllable deletion test
Cardswith representative drawings:
Examples:
nota (bank note)
ameixa (plum)
Items:
av6 (grandmother)
orelha (ear)
radio (radio)
morango (strawberry)
gorila (gorilla)
vela (candle)
caneta (pen)
foca (seal)
tapete (carpet)
passaro (bird)
boca (mouth)
seta (arrow)
laio (tie)
dedal (thimble)
Initial-phoneme deletion test
Cardswith representative drawings:
Examples:
noz (nut)
b6ia (life buoy)
Items:
rio (river)
rosa (rose)
mel (honey)
mola (clothespin)
gas (gas)
galo (cock)
vale (valley)
vila (village)
cdo (dog)
capa (overcoat)
fio (thread)
fava (bean)
torre (tower)
telha (tile)
pdo (bread)
pipa (barrel)
boi (ox)
bolo (cake)
sal (salt)
sumo (juice)
lua (moon)
lula (squid)
dente (tooth)
dedo (finger)
483