Life Span Theory
Life Span Theory
Life Span Theory
qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 39
3
LIFE-SPAN
DEVELOPMENTAL THEORY
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 40
THE READINGS
Schoon et al. (2002) use the life-span developmental perspective in combination with concepts
from ecological theory (Bronfenbrenner & Ceci,
1994; Chapter 10) and developmental contextualism (Ford & Lerner, 1992) to examine the relationships between early and continuing social
risk and academic achievement in childhood/
adolescence as well as between earlier risk and
adult attainments. Developmental contextualism
views human development as the dynamic interaction between a changing individual and a
changing context. This reading uses fairly complex data analysis procedures (path analysis
and multiple regression) to explicate a complex
development trajectory over a significant portion
of the life span. (You can safely ignore the statistical details since the articles text is very clear in
stating what the analyses mean and how they
can be interpreted.) Another key feature of the
Schoon et al. reading is what we mentioned
earlier in this chapter: the link between theoretical perspective and methodological choices. In
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 41
FURTHER READING
Bronfenbrenner and Ceci (1994), Ford and Lerner
(1992).
Note
1. This chapter relies extensively on Baltes,
Lindenberger, and Staudinger (1998), and Goulet and
Baltes (1970).
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 42
Abstract
Introduction
42
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 43
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 44
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 45
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 46
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 47
ACA
ACA
ACA
f
Parental
Social
Class
c
d
SOC
c
d
e
a1
RISK
RISK
Birth
Figure 3.1
Early Childhood
RISK
a1
a1
Late Childhood
Adolescence
Adulthood
the additional incremental risk effects at subsequent time points (early childhood, late childhood,
and adolescence). The paths labeled d indicate
these time-lagged effects of earlier risk on later
academic attainment. The model furthermore
examined the independent influence of social risk
experienced during adolescence on adult social
status (path e), as well as the influence of academic adjustment during adolescence on adult
social status (path f). To establish whether
parental social class at birth had an influence on an
individuals adult social status, independent of
intervening experiences, path g was included,
which was an indicator of cohort effects not
accounted for by the risk and adjustment levels
carried forward in time. The model also considered
the effects of a changing sociohistorical context,
which is not shown in the diagram but was taken
into account in the analyses by applying the model
to two cohorts of young people growing up 12
years apart. The importance of the life-course
developmental perspective for this study lay in its
scope to integrate process and structure and to link
individual time with historical time. The longitudinal approach had a number of methodological
advantages with regard to the research questions:
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 48
Method
This study used data collected for the National
Child Development Study (NCDS) and the
British Cohort Study (BCS70), two of Britains
richest research resources for the study of
human development. The participants of the
NCDS included all persons born in Great Britain
between March 3 and March 9, 1958. In five
follow-up studies, data were collected on the
physical, psychosocial, and educational development of the cohort at ages 7, 11, 16, 23, and 33
years (Shepherd, 1995), and, most recently, at age
42 (Bynner, Ferri, Shepherd, & Smith, 2000). At
each sweep between 1958 and 1974, wide-ranging information was collected from parents,
teachers, school medical officers, and, at later
stages, from the cohort members themselves via
personal interviews. Satisfactory response rates
have been reported for each sweep, and comparison of data has shown that the achieved samples
did not markedly differ from the target samples,
or from other survey samples of the British
population (Shepherd, 1993, 1995).
The BCS70 has followed children born in
the week April 511, 1970, from birth through to
adulthood. Data sweeps took place when the
cohort members were ages 5, 10, 16, and 26 years
(Ekinsmyth, Bynner, Montgomery, & Shepherd,
1992), and most recently at age 30 (Bynner, Ferri,
Shepherd, & Smith, 2000). In the birth survey,
information was collected by means of a questionnaire that was completed by the midwife present
at birth, and supplementary information was
obtained from clinical records. In 1975 and 1980,
parents of the cohort members were interviewed
by Health Visitors, the cohort members themselves
undertook ability tests, and the school health
service gathered medical information on each
child. This was supplemented at ages 10 and 16 by
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 49
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 50
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 51
Attainments in Adulthood
Adult attainment was indicated by two
measures of social position: the RGSC and
the Cambridge Scale (CS) assessed at age 30 for
the BCS70 cohort members and at age 33 for the
NCDS cohort members. The 6-point RGSC scale,
developed by the Office of Population and
Census Surveys (OPCS; 1990), was described
above. For ease of interpretation the coding was
reversed, so that a high score indicated a high
social position. The CS was conceptualized as
an indicator of general social advantage and
lifestyle (Prandy, 1990). It is based on the analysis of friendship choices, judged to be the most
accurate indication of perceived and experienced
social distance between members of different
occupations. The scale is measured on a 100point continuum, whereby high scores indicate a
higher level of social advantage.
Statistical Analysis:
Modeling Cumulative Risk Effects
Structural equation modeling (Bollen, 1989)
was used to formulate theoretically derived
hypotheses about variable relations and to test
postulated pathways between the variables and
the assumed mediating processes involving latent
variables with multiple indicators. Latent variables represent hypothetical concepts that cannot
be observed or measured directly. Instead, a set of
observed variables are hypothesized to be imperfect indicators of the latent variable. Because the
study involved a cross-cohort comparison of data
that had been collected for different surveys,
some of the data were similar but not necessarily
identical for the two cohorts. Great care was
taken to define the latent constructs in as similar
a way as possible in the two cohorts. Table 3.1
summarizes the selection of comparable indicator variables in both cohorts.
All analyses were carried out using the
SEM program AMOS 4.01 (Arbuckle, 1999).
The AMOS program uses maximum likelihood
estimation that can be based on incomplete
data, known as the full information maximum
likelihood (FIML) approach. This approach is
preferable to estimation based on complete data
(the listwise deletion [LD] approach) because
FIML estimates tend to show less bias and
be more reliable than LD estimates, even when
the data deviate from missing at random and
are nonignorable (Arbuckle, 1996). In the LD
approach, the complete data covariance matrix is
the data source for the latent variable analysis. In
the FIML approach, estimation is based on the
many covariance matrices between observed
variables for all patterns of missing data in the
other observed variables. Thus, it is not possible
to present a single correlation matrix for the
observed variables. Instead, Appendices A and B
give the FIML estimates of the correlations
between the observed variables, the means and
SDs for the observed variables, and the FIML
estimates of means and SDs.
In line with current practice, several criteria
were used to assess the fit of the model to the
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 52
Variables
Socioeconomic risk (RISK)
Parental social class
Material conditions
Observed Indicators
Fathers social class
Mothers social class
No housing tenure, overcrowding (>1 person per
room), household amenities (shared use of
bathroom), receipt of state benefits
Ages 5 and 7: Human Figure Drawing,
Copy-a-Design, Reading and vocabulary
(NCDS and BCS70), and Arithmetic (NCDS
only) tests
Ages 10 and 11: Reading and mathematics tests
(NCDS and BCS70)
Age 16: Exam scores (NCDS and BCS70)
Registrar Generals measure of social class (RGSC)
Cambridge Scale (CS)
NOTE: NCDS = National Child Development Study; BCS70 = British Cohort Study.
Modeling Strategy
Four separate models were run for each
cohort. Model 1 was the Full Developmental
Contextual Model, which included all paths indicated in Figure 3.1. In addition to Model 1 three
other models were fitted to the data, to test
the increase of fit depending on the pathways
included, and to identify the most parsimonious
model for describing the long-term influences
of socioeconomic adversity on academic adjustment. Model 2 was the DevelopmentalContextual Model without path g (which assessed
the direct effect of parental social class on an
individuals adult social status independent of
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 53
Results
The distribution of the risk indicator variables
is shown in Table 3.2. Generally, material conditions improved for the later-born BCS70 cohort.
In comparison with cohort members born in 1958,
more families owned their home, there was less
overcrowding, and fewer households had to
share amenities. The percentage of families who
were receiving state benefits had remained stable. Also noted was upward mobility among the
parents of cohort members born in1970, whereas
Table 3.2
Distribution of the Risk Variables in the 1958 National Child Development Study (NCDS) and
the 1970 British Cohort Study (BCS70)
Cohort (%)
Risk Variable
NCDS
BCS70
22
24
23
55
66
19
19
44
40
7
23
54
51
7
27
18
39
30
3
22
Age 16 (1974/1986)
Social class (% in RGSC IV and V)
No housing tenure
Overcrowding (1+ person per room)
Shared use of amenities
Family receiving benefits
22
50
60
7
23
13
28
17
1
27
Birth (1958/1970)
Social class at birth (% in RGSC IV and V)
Age 7/5 (1964/1975)
Social class (% in RGSC IV and V)
No housing tenure
Overcrowding (1+ person per room)
Shared use of amenities
54
723.73
730.92
848.12
1,022.88
77
78
79
80
.998
.998
.998
.997
.998
.998
.996
.997
CFI
1,347
1,344
1,450
1,614
1,866
1,863
2,510
2,329
CAIC
.024
.024
.026
.029
.027
.027
.033
.032
RMSEA
90% CI
.023.026
.023.026
.024.028
.027.030
.025.028
.025.028
.032.035
.030.033
1 (M2-M1)
2 (M3-M1)
3 (M4-M1)
1 (M2-M1)
2 (M3-M1)
3 (M4-M1)
df
7.19
124.39
299.15
7.63
665.44
495.46
x2
< .01
< .001
< .001
< .01
< .001
< .001
NOTE: df = degrees of freedom; CFI = comparative fit index; CAIC = Consistent Akaike Information Criterion; RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation;
CI = confidence interval.
1,221.47
1,229.10
1,886.91
1,716.93
93
94
95
96
df
5:33 PM
Goodness of Fit
Comparative Goodness of Fit of Structural Equation Models Run for the 1958 National Child Development Cohort (NCDS) and the
1970 British Cohort Study (BCS70)
11/16/2004
NCDS
Model 1: The full model
Model 2: Excluding path g
Model 3: Excluding paths d
Model 4: Excluding correlations c
BCS70
Model 1: The full model
Model 2: Excluding path g
Model 3: Excluding paths d
Model 4: Excluding correlations c
Table 3.3
03-Chibucos.qxd
Page 54
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 55
56
.62
.34
.55
.80
.41
.48
.50
.80
.66
.80
.41
BCS70,
age 5
.85
.88
.78
.53
NCDS,
age 11
.83
.83
.76
.50
BCS70,
age 10
.69
.61
.74
.53
NCDS,
age 16
.87
.63
.76
.45
BCS70,
age 16
.95
.88
NCDS,
age 33
.65
.74
BCS70,
at 30
5:33 PM
NCDS,
age 7
The Measurement Model. Estimated Standardized Regression Weights for the National Child Development Study (NCDS) and the
1970 British Cohort Study (BCS70)
11/16/2004
Risk
RiskParental social class
RiskMaterial deprivation
Table 3.4
03-Chibucos.qxd
Page 56
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 57
.78
.32
Parental
Social
Class
ACA
RISK
SOC
.26
RISK
.97
.99
Late Childhood
Early Childhood
RISK
.05
Adulthood
Adolescence
.71
.81
.45
Parental
Social
Class
Birth
ACA
ACA
.45
ACA
RISK
Early Childhood
.64
.09
.15
.07
.91
Figure 3.3
.57
.49
.19
Birth
ACA
.21
.38
.88
Figure 3.2
ACA
SOC
.22
RISK
.93
Late Childhood
.94
RISK
Adolescence
.14
Adulthood
The British Cohort Study: Developmental-contextual model of accumulating risk effects from
birth to adulthood. ACA = academic adjustment; Risk = socioeconomic risk; SOC = social
position in adulthood. See text for further details.
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 58
Discussion
In the study of human development, laboratories are rare. The closest one can get to them is
when the opportunity arises to compare and contrast experiences in different societies or in different sociohistorical contexts. The present study
used data collected for two national birth cohort
studies born 12 years apart. On comparison of
the prevalence of risk indicators that were
assessed at roughly the same time (e.g., parental
social class or housing tenure experienced by 16year-olds born in 1958 and 5-year-olds born in
1970, assessed in 1974 and 1975, respectively),
it appears that the samples represented well the
state of affairs over the historical period covered.
The data suggest that there was a real-time
secular shift in material resources. For cohort
members born in 1970, the indicator variables
point to improved material circumstances for the
cohort as a whole. Furthermore, the parents of
cohort members born in 1970 experienced
upward social mobility while bringing up their
children. Those cohort members born in 1970
who suffered deprivation, however, appear to
have been more disadvantaged relative to other
children in the same cohort than similarly
affected children born earlier, in 1958. A strong
continuity of social risk effects was observed,
which was of similar strength for both cohorts.
The strength of the association can be explained
by the fact that in addition to indicators of
material disadvantage, indicators of SES, which
denote relative position in society, were also
used. However, despite improved material conditions, and the experience of upward social
mobility among parents of the BCS70 cohort, the
relative social position remained remarkably
stable. Furthermore, the influence of parental
social class at birth on academic adjustment was
greater for cohort members born in 1970 than for
those born in 1958, and the direct influence of
social risk experienced in adolescence on adult
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 59
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 60
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 61
Acknowledgments
This work was funded by the U.K. Economic and Social Research Council (Grant No.
R000238051). Previous versions of this article
were presented at the XXVII International
Congress of Psychology in Stockholm (July
2000), the 42nd Congress of the German
Psychological Society in Jena (September 2000),
and the London Conference of the British
Psychological Society (December 2000). Thanks
are due to the anonymous reviewers of an earlier
draft of this paper for their helpful and constructive comments.
References
Ackerman, B. P., Schoff, K., Levinson, K.,
Youngstrom, E., & Izard, C. E. (1999). The relations between cluster indexes of risk and promotion and the problem behaviours of 6- and
7-year-old children from economically disadvantaged families. Developmental Psychology, 6,
13551366.
Arbuckle, J. C. (1996). Full information estimation in the presence of incomplete data. In
G. A. Marcoulides & R. E. Schumacker (Eds.),
Advanced structural equation modelling techniques. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Arbuckle, J. C. (1999). AMOS for Windows. Analysis
of moment structures. Version 4.01 [Computer
software]. Chicago: SmallWaters.
Axinn, W., Duncan, G. J., & Thornton, A. (1997). The
effects of parents income, wealth, and attitudes on
childrens completed schooling and self-esteem.
In G. J. Duncan & J. Brooks-Gunn (Eds.),
Consequences of growing up poor (pp. 518540).
New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Baltes, P. B. (1987). Theoretical propositions of lifespan developmental psychology: On the dynamics between growth and decline. Developmental
Psychology, 23, 611626.
Banks, M., Bates, I., Breakwell, G., Bynner, J., Emler,
N., Jamieson, L., & Roberts, K. (1992). Careers
and identities. Milton Keynes, UK: Open
University Press.
Baumrind, D. (1993). The average expectable
environment is not good enough. A response to
Scarr. Child Development, 64, 12991317.
Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indices in
structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107,
238246.
Birch, H. G., & Gussow, J. D. (1970). Disadvantaged
children: Health, nutrition and school failure.
New York: Grune & Stratton.
Blau, P. M., & Duncan, O. D. (1967). The American
occupational structure. New York: Wiley.
Bolger, K. E., Patterson, C. J., & Thompson, W. W.
(1995). Psychosocial adjustment among children
experiencing persistent and intermittent family
economic hardship. Child Development, 66,
11071129.
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 62
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 63
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 64
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 65
66
.23
.16
.37
.35
.24
.16
.23
.28
.17
.17
.24
.12
.32
.30
.29
.63
.32
.18
.22
.19
.13
.58
.25
.21
.20
.35
.62
.13
.16
.10
.10
.34
.39
.72
.34
.23
.53
.56
.35
.33
.34
.18
.33
.22
.45
.58
.33
.32
.33
.17
.37
.21
.25
.15
.15
.69
.28
10
.14
.17
.10
.10
.37
.48
11
.51
.31
.30
.22
.11
12
.34
.32
.27
.14
13
.36
.16
.08
14
.16
.08
15
.30
16
1.17
3.62
.86 16.74 16.06
3.68
.84
5.13 23.40 23.83
7.02
3.77
.81
.83
1.27
.96 10.29
6.21
1.27
.89
2.48
7.10 7.07
2.07
1.24
.72
9,114 10,320 10,665 13,125 13,129 12,605 12,790 14,065 14,098 13,839 14,056 13,749 13,756
.77
.26
.18
.44
.41
.28
.18
.27
.33
.20
.19
.28
.14
17.78
18.14
9,114
.28
.24
.23
.16
.35
.34
.25
.16
.23
.27
.17
.16
.25
.13
17
3.83
3.34 33.86 16.74
1.13 3.62
.87 16.65 16.00
3.69
.85
.85 23.38 23.81
7.02 3.77
.82
1.23
1.33 19.26 18.22
.84 1.28
.97 10.35
6.25
1.28
.89
2.48 7.11
7.07
2.01 1.24
.72
16,994 16,994 16,994 16,994 16,994 16,994 16,994 16,994 16,994 16,994 16,994 16,994 16,994 16,994 16,994 16,994 16,994
.81
.26
.22
.21
.15
.32
.31
.23
.15
.21
.25
.15
.15
.23
.12
FIML estimates
M
SD
N
.21
.23
.26
.22
.49
.35
.30
.29
.56
.37
.18
.21
.13
.13
.59
.30
3.24 34.98
1.31 19.11
10,693 10,838
5:33 PM
Observed variables
M 3.83
SD 1.23
N 16,994
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
8.
9.
10.
11.
12.
13.
14.
15.
16.
17.
11/16/2004
Bivariate Correlations, Means, SDs, and Sample Sizes for the Variables in the 1958 National Child Development Dataset, Including the Full
Information Maximum Likelihood (FIML) Estimates from the AMOS Modeling
APPENDIX A
03-Chibucos.qxd
Page 66
.14
.07
.15
.18
.10
.23
.11
.19
.23
.12
.15
.31
.20
.39
.31
.18
.30
.73
.16
.31
.24
.38
.33
.18
.30
.26
.66
.13
.23
.19
.33
10
.42
.32
.08
.14
.11
11
.32
.26
.06
.11
.10
.44
.27
.15
.15
.13
.26
.36
13
.07
.14
14
.30
15
16
32.78 11.34
4.73
3.55
.65
9.76
3.28
1.97
1.27
.92
9,805 12,177 12,355 11,929 12,056
.11
.21
.21
.30
12
3.25
1.26
6,590
.21
.52
.10
.19
.15
.26
.56
.24
.24
.24
3.77
1.20
14,229
1.40
.69
6,457
.10
.20
.10
.19
.16
.12
.20
.30
.29
.10
.18
2.97
1.20
8,002
21.94
16.65
5,603
.14
.29
.15
.27
.20
.17
.28
.41
.41
.15
.25
.53
3.78
1.20
14,229
49.44
16.12
5,183
.11
.09
.16
Observed variables
M
SD
N
FIML estimates
M
SD
N
.30
.06
.13
.33
.08
.22
.55
.60
.13
.27
.29
.16
.27
.28
.07
.11
.27
.20
.13
.11
.19
.28
.44
.21
.13
.30
.20
.22
3. Own CS at age 30
.18
5:33 PM
11/16/2004
Bivariate Correlations, Means, SDs, and Sample Sizes for the Variables in the 1970 British Birth Cohort Dataset, Including the Full Information
Maximum Likelihood (FIML) Estimates from the AMOS Modeling
APPENDIX B
03-Chibucos.qxd
Page 67
67
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 68
Abstract
Using a life course theoretical framework, this
study examined longitudinal effects of continuity
and transitions in marital status on multiple
dimensions of psychological well-being. Data
came from National Survey of Families and
Households 19871993 respondents ages 19 to 65
(N = 6,948). Differences between men and women
as well as between young and midlife adults were
investigated. Multivariate analyses revealed a
complex pattern of effects depending on the contrast and the outcome examined. Although marriage continued to promote well-being for both
men and women, in some casesfor example,
autonomy, personal growththe single fared
better than the married. The effects of continuity
in single status were not very different for women
in contrast to men. The transition to divorce or
widowhood was associated with somewhat more
negative effects for women. Midlife adults evidenced more psychological resilience than young
adults did in facing the challenges of a marital
transition or remaining single over time.
The social institution of marriage and its
influence on adult well-being remains an enduring interest of family researchers. Historically,
marriage has been quite consistently associated
68
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 69
Theoretical and
Empirical Background
Continuity and Change in Life
Course and Life Span Development
The life course and life span developmental
perspectives suggest that adult development
is characterized by a complex interplay of continuity and change no less than child development
(Baltes, 1987; Elder, 1992; Featherman, 1983).
Family life transitions as well as family life continuity are important components of the process
that helps constitute adult development and
adult well-being (Bengtson & Allen, 1993;
Elder, 1991). Social context and how it changes
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 70
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 71
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 72
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 73
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 74
Methods
Data
The data for these analyses came from the
first and second waves of the National Survey
of Families and Households (NSFH), which
includes information from personal interviews
conducted in 19871988 (Time 1) and in
19921993 (Time 2; 5 years later), with a nationally representative sample of 13,008 noninstitutionalized American adults, 19 years old and
older. This survey included a main sample of
9,643 respondents, with an additional over sample
of 3,374 African Americans, Mexican Americans,
Puerto Ricans, single parents, stepparents,
cohabitors, and recently married persons. The
response rate at Time 1 (19871988) was about
75%. The response rate at Time 2 was about 82%
of first wave respondents. This yielded national
population coverage at a rate of about 62% for
data from both waves. Sampling weights correcting for selection probabilities and nonresponse
allow this sample to match the composition of
the U.S. population on age, sex, and race (see
Sweet, Bumpass, & Call, 1988, for more design
details). The analytic sample for this study consisted of NSFH primary respondents ages 19 to
34 or 40 to 60 in 19871988, who also responded
in 19921993, and who had complete and consistent marital status information for the period
between the two waves of the survey (N = 6,948;
138 cases2% of Time 2 respondents were
excluded due to incomplete information).
Respondents ages 35 to 39 in 19871988 were
excluded from these longitudinal analyses so that
we could make a clear differentiation in the age
group contrast analyses between persons experiencing marital status continuity and change prior
to age 40 and after age 40. (The group ages 35
to 39 between 19871988 and 19921993
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 75
Measures
Outcome measures included a 12-item modified version of the Center for Epidemiological
Studies-Depression (CES-D) index (Radloff,
1977) (alpha = .93), a 3-item measure of hostility/
irritability (alpha = .85), a standard 1-item
measure of global happiness, a 3-item version
of Rosenbergs (1965) self-esteem index
(alpha = .65), a 5-item personal mastery index
consisting of 4 items from the Pearlin Mastery
Scale (Pearlin, Lieberman, Menaghan, & Mullan,
1981) along with a single item of control-mastery
also used in Wave 1 of the NSFH (alpha = .66),
and 3-item versions of Ryffs (1989; Ryff &
Keyes, 1995) six psychological well-being
scales: Autonomy (alpha = .45), Personal Growth
(alpha = .54), Positive Relations With Others
(alpha = .53), Purpose in Life (alpha = .37), SelfAcceptance (alpha = .54), and Environmental
Mastery (alpha = .56). The relatively lower internal consistency of items used for these scales
reflects an a priori decision by Ryff to create short
scales that represent the multi-factorial structure of
the original scales (which consisted of 20 items)
rather than to maximize internal consistency.
These dramatically shortened scales have been
found to correlate from .70 to .89 with the original
highly reliable scales (Ryff & Keyes, 1995).
For three measuresthe CES-D, global happiness, and self-esteemTime 1 assessment of
the measures were available and were controlled
in the respective analyses. For the Personal
Mastery Scale, responses to one item measuring
personal mastery that was included at Time 1 of
the NSFH was included as a Time 1 control (the
correlation of this one item at Time 2 with the
other four items of the scale at Time 2 is .57).
The hostility index and the six Ryff measures
were not included at Time 1, so the CES-D
assessment from Time 1 was entered as a control
for group selection on well-being in all analyses
76
51.0
7.2
1.8
17.0
5.8
1.6
9.2
3.3
1.7
1.3
100.0
3,219
867
184
1,019
430
92
515
386
121
115
6,948
Weighted Percentage
4,166
77
74
242
82
264
240
1,822
647
166
552
Unweighted n
100.0
1.3
1.9
3.3
2.7
7.6
5.9
50.4
9.5
3.1
14.3
Weighted Percentage
Women
2,782
38
47
144
10
251
190
1,397
220
18
467
Unweighted n
100.0
1.3
1.5
3.4
0.4
10.9
5.6
51.6
4.8
0.4
19.9
Weighted Percentage
Men
5:33 PM
NOTE: Percentage columns do not always total 100.0 due to rounding errors.
Continuity
Married
Separated/Divorced
Widowed
Widowed
Change
MarriedSeparated/
Divorced
MarriedWidowed
Never married
First married
Separated/Divorced/
WidowedRemarried
MarriedSeparated/
Divorced/Widowed
Remarried
UnmarriedMarried
Separated/Divorced/
Widowed
Valid cases
Unweighted n
Total Sample
Weighted Percentage Distribution (unweighted n) of 5-Year Marital Status Continuity and Change, National Survey of Families and
Households 19871993, Primary Respondents, Ages 1964 (N = 6,948)
11/16/2004
Marital Status
Table 3.5
03-Chibucos.qxd
Page 76
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 77
Results
Table 3.6 reports the results of models that estimated the effects of multiple marital status contrasts and Gender x Marital Status interactions on
well-being. Because there was at least one significant Gender x Marital Status interaction effect in
each of the combined gender models estimated (at
least at the trend level), it was deemed appropriate to examine separate models for men and
women to confirm the gender differences in marital status effects. In addition, for these models,
to answer the third research questionDoes age
status (that is young adult vs. midlife adult) make
a difference in the effects of marital status continuity and change on well-being?we included
Age 40+ x Marital Status interaction variables
for each marital status contrast where there were
enough cases to examine contrasts across age
groups. Tables 3.7 and 3.8 provide estimates for
models that examined both genders separately
for each well-being outcome and also added
Age x Marital Status interactions.
We did not create age interactions (a) for continuity in widowhood status, because there were
so few continuously widowed men and women
under age 40; (b) for the transition from never
78
Change
MarriedSeparated/
Divorced
MarriedWidowed
Never married
First married
Separated/Divorced/
Widowed Remarried
MarriedSeparated/
Divorced/Widowed
Remarried
UnmarriedMarried
Separated/Divorced/
Widowed
Female Married
Separated/Divorced
2.23***
1.58***
.25*
.23
.28
.43*
.59
.13
.27**
.16**
.49*
.01
.11**
.22
.11**
.01
.25
.00
.01
.20
.03
.02
.01
.21
.54
.21*
2.38*
.03
.21
.25***
.16
0.51***
.23
.29***
.02
.26**
.50***
.24
.40***
.19
.08***
.04*
.37
1.73**
.23
.06
2.108
.06
.76**
.38
.33
1.00***
.27
.25
.47
.39***
.46***
.44***
.58***
.45
.43
.30
.51**
.81
.51**
.17
.88
.27
.46
.62
.31
.69***
.18
.69***
.25
.04
.27
.30
.08
.79
.39
.47*** .57***
-.01
.02
.12
.29**
Personal
Autonomy Growth
.02
.24
.96***
.95
.22
.31
.27***
.51***
.31
.80***
2.49***
.80***
.28
.21
1.98*** 1.87***
.16
.52**
.16
.52**
.83*8
1.91***
.918
.56***
.04
.56***
.91***
.56
3.6*
.97***
.19
.32*
.29
.38***
.19*
.72**
1.01*
.07
.64***
.38
.63***
.18
.42
.17
.47*
.37
.08
.27
.04
.07
Purpose SelfEnvironmental
in Life Acceptance Mastery
1.04*** 3.9
.02
.00
-1.12***
.92
.69***
.16
1.04***
.03
Positive
Relations
5:33 PM
1.06
.08
.48
.83
.55*
.29
.15
.62*
.19***
.13
.06
.00
Personal
Self-Esteem Mastery
11/16/2004
.53
.06
.05
.35**
.14***
.08*
Global
Depression Hostility Happiness
Unstandardized Regression Coefficients for the Effects of Marital Status Continuity and Change on Psychological Well-Being
Female
Age 40
Marital Status
Continuity
Married
Separated/Divorced
Widowed
Never Married
Female Separated/
Divorced
Female Widowed
Female Never married
Predictors
Table 3.6
03-Chibucos.qxd
Page 78
.91***
2.04***
.10
.35***
1.47***
.20
4.30***
.09
.23***
.34
2.26***
.18
.37***
.83***
12.74***
.14
1.76**
.32
.19
.44***
.53
.77**
.05
.05
1.33
.84
.24
.25
-.18
.48
14.46*** 12.86***
.03
.10
.27*** .22***
.84
.80
.06
.00
1.10
Personal
Autonomy Growth
.23***
13.37***
.12
.52***
1.00
.57
.10
1.92**
.05
.07
.44
.28
.40
2.49***
13.57***
.09
.50***
1.58**
.50
.02
.34
.16
Purpose SelfEnvironmental
in Life Acceptance Mastery
12.76*** 10.92***
.09
.13
.41***
1.22
.24
.25
.05
.61
Positive
Relations
NOTE: All models also included controls for race/ethnicity, employment status, years of education, household income, and presence of a child age 18 in household.
SOURCE: National Survey of Families and Households (19871993), primary respondents (N = 6,948).
Time 1 Well-Being
Depression
Global happiness
Self-esteem
Personal mastery
Constant
R2
.38
.51**
-.55
.72
.24
.20
.03
.23
.13
.42*
Personal
Self-Esteem Mastery
5:33 PM
.30
.11
2.91**
.48
.19
Global
Depression Hostility Happiness
11/16/2004
Female Married
Widowed
Female Never
marriedFirst married
Female Separated/
Divorced/Widowed
Remarried
Female Married
Separated/Divorced/
WidowedRemarried
Female Unmarried
MarriedSeparated/
Divorced/Widowed
Predictors
03-Chibucos.qxd
Page 79
79
80
Change
MarriedSeparated/Divorced
MarriedWidowed
Never marriedFirst married
Separated/Divorced/
WidowedRemarried
MarriedSeparated/Divorced/
WidowedRemarried
Never marriedMarried
Separated/Divorced/Widowed
Age 40
MarriedSeparated/Divorced
Age 40
MarriedWidowed
.10
.24
.07
.42*
.20
.22
.16
.54**
.24
.63*** .26*
.71*
.21
.14*
.16
.20
.01
.55**
.57***
.62
.28***
.04
.21*
.03
.12
Marital Status
Continuity
Married (omitted)
Separated/Divorced
Widowed
Never married
.08
Men
.14
1.75**
1.52**
.12
2.19***
.29
.35
.03
.05
.72
1.08**
.39
.51
.14
Women
Men
2.68***
2.68***
.08
.04
2.40*
.37
.98
1.05
1.41
1.59**
.76
1.02***
.29
Hostility
1.04*
.12
.12
.27
.54***
.23
.32**
.05
.26
.02
.29*
.18
.14
.06
Women
.55*
.55
.01
.45**
.62
.18
.16
.04
.03
.56*
.25
.44***
.06
Men
Global Happiness
.00
.20*
.20*
.07
.16***
.19
.19***
.03
.01
.07
.04
.05
.07
.01
Women
.32**
.32**
.15
.05
.22
.07
.09
.00
.10
.09
.20
.02
.06
Men
Self-Esteem
2.39*
.02
.02
.02
1.52***
2.73**
.69**
.50
1.16**
.40
.69*
.46
.15
.95***
Women
.81**
.81**
.21
.63
2.24*
.01
.19
.57
.63
1.48**
.35
.25
.14
Men
Personal Mastery
5:33 PM
Age 40
Separated/Divorced
Age 40
Never married
.03
Women
Depression
Unstandardized Regression Coefficients for the Effects of Marital Status Continuity and Change on Psychological
Well-Being by Gender
11/16/2004
Age 40 (Time 1)
Predictors
Table 3.7
03-Chibucos.qxd
Page 80
.11
.20
.10
2.56***
.76***
.36
Men
.10
4.30***
.24***
.37
Women
.21***
.03
Men
.18
2.03***
.39***
.14
Women
.17
2.39***
.34***
.00
Men
Self-Esteem
.15
.83***
12.35***
.32
Women
.13
83***
.28
Men
Personal Mastery
NOTE: All models also include controls for race/ethnicity, employment status, years of education, household income, and presence of a child 18 in household.
p .10. *p .05. **p .01 ***p .001 (two-tailed test).
.10
4.18***
SOURCE: National Survey of Families and Households (19871993), primary respondents (N = 6,948).
.17
1.40***
1.51*** 1.59***
.77
Women
1.05***
.18
Men
.31***
.39***
.44*
Age 40
Separated/Divorced/
WidowedRemarried
Time 1 Well-Being
Depression
Global happiness
Self-esteem
Personal mastery
Constant
12.69***
R2
Women
Global Happiness
5:33 PM
Predictors
Hostility
11/16/2004
Depression
03-Chibucos.qxd
Page 81
81
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 82
.20
Men
03
.21
.47***
.48
.70
.14
.42
.49
.53
13.90***14.41***
.09
.04
.54*** .30***
.07
.42
1.12*
.55
.08
.03
.59**
.42
.30
.51** .55**
.18
.26
.28*
Women
.84 .09
.43
.07
.18
.19
.46
.62*
.33
.22
.08
Men
Men
.74
.41
.65***
1.15*
.51
.39
1.18
.17
.25
.65*
.57*
.12
.46
.60
.58
.32
.13
.11
.75
.25
.53*** .69***
.08
.43
.25
.25
.56
.56*** .55***
Women
Personal
Growth
14.47***12.78***13.25***
.03
.12
.10
.18
.53
.58
.52
.36
.42
.73
.33
.33
.84
.33
.05
.17
.16
Men
Autonomy
NOTE: All models also include controls for race or ethnicity, employment status, years of education, household income, and presence of a child 18 in household.
p .10. *p .05. **p .01. ***p .001 (two-tailed test).
SOURCE: National Survey of Families and Households (19871993), primary respondents (N = 6,948).
.52***
.15
1.13*
.41
.66
.16
.63
.52
.46
Constant
R2
.01
.80
.35
2.23**
.43
2.19***
.20
.98*
.86***
.01
1.07***
.36***
.06
.34
.30
.29
.09
Women
.28
Men
Environmental
Mastery
Time 1 Well-Being
Depression
.77
1.80
.56
2.22***
.71
1.62***
.03
.23
.27
.07
1.74*** 1.40***
1.87*
.02
.54**
.34
Women
Women
SelfAcceptance
5:33 PM
Change
MarriedSeparated/Divorced
MarriedWidowed
Never marriedFirst married
Separated/Divorced/
WidowedRemarried
MarriedSeparated/Divorced/
WidowedRemarried
UnmarriedMarriedSeparated/
Divorced/Widowed
Age 40 Married
Separated/Divorced
Age 40
MarriedWidowed
Age 40 Separated/
Divorced/WidowedRemarried
.09
Age 40 (Time 1)
Marital Status
Continuity
Married (omitted)
Separated/Divorced
Widowed
Never married
Age 40 Separated/Divorced
Age 40 Never married
Men
Purpose
in Life
11/16/2004
1.86*** 1.73***
.62* .61*
.68*
1.08
.85*** 1.02
.74*** .87***
.01 .31
1.12***
.71
.10 .60
1.06*
.82
.72 .02
Women
Positive
Relations
Unstandardized Regression Coefficients for the Effects of Marital Status Continuity and Change on Psychological Well-Being (Ryff
scales) by Gender
Predictors
Table 3.8
03-Chibucos.qxd
Page 83
83
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 84
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 85
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 86
Note
Support for this research was provided by the John
D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation Research
Network on Successful Midlife Development and a
FIRST Grant Award to the first author from the
National Institute on Aging (AG12731). The National
Survey of Families and Households (NSFH) was
funded by CPR-NICHHD Grant No. HD21009. The
NSFH was designed and carried out at the Center
for Demography and Ecology at the University of
WisconsinMadison under the direction of Larry
Bumpass and James Sweet. We gratefully acknowledge
Larry Bumpass helpful input to this study. Address
correspondence to Nadine F. Marks, Child and Family
Studies, University of WisconsinMadison, 1430
Linden Drive, Madison, WI 537061575; e-mail:
marks@sc.wisc.edu
References
Anderson, C. M., & Stewart, S. (1994). Flying solo:
Single women in midlife. New York: Norton.
Aneshensel, C. S., Rutter, C. M., & Lachenbruch, P. A.
(1991). Social structure, stress, and mental health:
Competing conceptual and analytic models.
American Sociological Review, 56, 166178.
Ball, J. F. (1977). Widows grief: The impact of age
and mode of death. Omega, 7, 307333.
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 87
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 88
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 89
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 90
APPENDIX A
Descriptive Statistics for Analysis Variables
Total Sample
Mean (SD)
N = 6,948
Women
Mean (SD)
n = 4,166
Demographic Characteristics
Female
Age 40 (Time l)
Black
Employed
Years of education
Household income (in thousands)
Missing on income data
Child 18 in household
.52
.43
.11
.73
13.03 (2.78)
33.37 (41.52)
.32
.42
.44
.12
.64
12.81 (2.64)
32.39 (39.58)
.30
.45
.42
.10
.82
13.27 (2.90)
39.17 (44.52)
.33
.39
Psychological Well-Being
Depression
Depression (Time 2)
Global happiness
Global happiness (Time 2)
Self-esteem
Self-esteem (Time 2)
Personal mastery (1 item)
Personal mastery (Time 2)
Hostility
Autonomy
Environmental mastery
Personal growth
Positive relations
Purpose in life
Self-acceptance
2.18 (1.16)
2.12 (1.13)
5.41 (1.32)
5.36 (1.31)
4.12 (.59)
4.10 (.63)
3.60 (.96)
18.16 (3.4)
3.22 (4.21)
14.48 (2.50)
13.79 (2.74)
15.13 (2.45)
13.70 (3.11)
13.74 (2.87)
13.84 (2.74)
2.32 (1.11)
2.28 (1.11)
5.39 (1.36)
5.33 (1.35)
4.12 (.60)
4.04 (.67)
3.56 (.98)
17.87 (3.48)
3.42 (4.41)
14.36 (2.56)
13.64 (2.81)
15.16 (2.48)
14.02 (3.11)
13.68 (2.89)
13.78 (2.78)
2.04 (1.19)
1.95 (1.12)
5.44 (1.28)
5.39 (1.26)
4.12 (.58)
4.16 (.59)
3.65 (.94)
18.46 (3.28)
3.01 (3.97)
14.61 (2.43)
13.94 (2.65)
15.10 (2.42)
13.35 (3.07)
13.82 (2.84)
13.90 (2.70)
.07
.02
.17
.05
.02
.05
.02
.07
.10
.03
.14
.06
.02
.05
.004
.20
.03
.02
Marital Status
Continuity
Separated/Divorced
Widowed
Never married
Age 40 Separated/Divorced
Age 40 Never married
Female Separated/Divorced
Female Widowed
Female Never married
Men
Mean (SD)
n = 2,782
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 91
Change
MarriedSeparated/Divorced
MarriedWidowed
Never marriedFirst married
Separated/Divorced/
WidowedRemarried
MarriedSeparated/Divorced/
WidowedRemarried
Never marriedMarriedSeparated/
Divorced/Widowed
Age 40 MarriedSeparated/Divorced
Age 40 MarriedWidowed
Age 40 Separated/Divorced/
WidowedRemarried
Female MarriedSeparated/Divorced
Female MarriedWidowed
Female Never marriedFirst married
Female Separated/Divorced/
WidowedRemarried
Female MarriedSeparated/
Divorced/WidowedRemarried
Female UnmarriedMarried
Separated/Divorced/Widowed
Total Sample
Mean (SD)
N = 6,948
Women
Mean (SD)
n = 4,166
Men
Mean (SD)
n = 2,782
.06
.02
.09
.06
.03
.08
.06
.004
.11
.03
.02
.03
.02
.03
.02
.01
.01
.01
.02
.01
.01
.02
.02
.01
.02
.02
.03
.01
.04
.02
.01
.01
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 92
APPENDIX B
Index Items
I. Ryff Psychological Well-Being Scales (rated on a 6-point scale: strongly disagree to strongly agree)
Autonomy
I tend to be influenced by people with strong opinions.a
I have confidence in my opinions, even if they are different from the way most other people think.
I judge myself by what I think is important, not by the values of what others think is important.
Positive Relations With Others
Maintaining close relationships has been difficult and frustrating for me.a
I have not experienced many warm and trusting relationships with others.a
People would describe me as a giving person, willing to share my time with others.
Purpose in Life
I live life one day at a time and dont really think about the future.a
Some people wander aimlessly through life, but I am not one of them.
I sometimes feel as if Ive done all there is to do in life.a
Self-Acceptance
I like most parts of my personality.
When I look at the story of my life, I am pleased how things have turned out.
In many ways, I feel disappointed about my achievements in life.a
Environmental Mastery
The demands of everyday life often get me down.a
In general, I feel I am in charge of the situation in which I live.
I am quite good at managing the many responsibilities of my daily life.
Personal Growth
I gave up trying to make big improvements or changes in my life a long time ago.a
I think it is important to have new experiences that challenge how you think about yourself and the world.
For me, life has been a continuous process of learning, changing, and growth.
II. Self-Esteem Scale
Please indicate how much you agree or disagree with the following statements:
On the whole I am satisfied with myself.
I am able to do things as well as other people.
I feel that Im a person of worth, at least on an equal plane with others.
III. Personal Mastery Scale
Please indicate how much you agree or disagree with the following statements:
I can do just about anything I really set my mind to.
Sometimes I feel that Im being pushed around in life.a
There is really no way I can solve some of the problems I have.a
I have little control over things that happen to me.a
I have always felt pretty sure my life would work out the way I wanted it to.
a. Item reverse coded.
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 93
03-Chibucos.qxd
11/16/2004
5:33 PM
Page 94