Best Et Al. 2008
Best Et Al. 2008
Best Et Al. 2008
Introduction
Children use texts from the onset of their elementary schooling.
Therefore, understanding texts and learning from them are of
paramount importance to academic success. A critical period in
the development of such reading skills appears to occur during the
third, fourth, and fifth grades. In fact, reading difficulties emerging during this period have been referred to as the fourth-grade
slump (Meichenbaum & Biemiller, 1998; Sweet & Snow, 2003). Our
study explored third graders reading comprehension, competencies enabling it, and the influence of text genre to understand
better this critical period and to inform effective interventions for
third graders who struggle with reading.
137
138
R. M. Best et al.
Text Genre
It is likely that changes in reading requirements contribute to the
fourth-grade slump. Prior to the third grade, most reading activities focus on decoding accuracy, decoding automaticity, and oral
reading fluency, whereas afterward, children are increasingly required to read to understand. Changes in reading activities result
in children being exposed to increasingly difficult texts during the
third grade. During this period, children move beyond narrative
texts to expository texts, such as science or social studies texts,
to learn about subject domains (Bowen, 1999; Snow, 2002). We
know that elementary-school children encounter greater difficulty
comprehending expository text than narrative text (Olson, 1985;
Spiro & Taylor, 1980; Tun, 1989). However, we have a fairly limited
understanding of the competencies that support third graders
comprehension of expository texts in comparison to narrative
texts (e.g., Cote, Goldman, & Saul, 1998; Williams, Hall, & Lauer,
2004). Therefore, we compared competencies involved in third
graders comprehension of expository and narrative texts with a
view toward pinpointing the difficulty associated with expository
text comprehension.
Competencies Enabling Reading Comprehension
Reading comprehension can be defined as the ability to obtain
meaning from written text for some purpose (Vellutino, 2003,
p. 51). To comprehend successfully, readers must identify a series
of letters as a word, access the meaning of the word from their lexicon or mental dictionary, and integrate individual word meanings
into a coherent sentence-level representation. As such, reading
comprehension requires the efficient coordination and integration of a number of underlying processes, which include reading
decoding skills and the utilization of world knowledge (Kintsch,
1988; 1998; Perfetti, 1985; Snow, 2002; Vellutino, Scanlon, &
Tanzman, 1994).
READING DECODING SKILLS
Reading decoding represents the ability to apply lettersound
correspondence rules when reading words and non-words. Many
children in the third and fourth grades may still exhibit slow
Differential Competencies
139
140
R. M. Best et al.
Differential Competencies
141
142
R. M. Best et al.
On two screening measures, children in this sample demonstrated reading comprehension skills and vocabulary knowledge
that were average for their age. Across children, values from the
WoodcockJohnson III (WJ III) Tests of Achievement (ACH) Passage Comprehension test (Woodcock et al., 2001) were M = 99.7
and SD = 9.2, and values from the WJ III ACH Picture Vocabulary
test were M = 102.3 and SD = 8.5. These results support the assertion that the participating children displayed an age-appropriate
range of reading abilities and competencies.
Materials
TEXTS
The texts were drawn from a pool of 60 narrative texts and
67 expository texts obtained from basal readers and science textbooks. For these 127 texts, conventional measures of text dimensions associated with comprehension, including number of words,
number of sentences, FleschKincaid grade level, and Flesch Reading ease, were derived from Coh-Metrix, Version 1.4 (Graesser,
McNamara, & Louwerse, & Cai, 2004). Preliminary selection criteria for inclusion were FleschKincaid grade level (to be between 2.97 and 4.75) and word length (within the range of 304
to 471 words). From the text pool, we selected the narrative text
Orlando, which was obtained from Addison-Wesleys Phonics TakeHome Reader, Grade 2 (1998). We selected the expository text Needs
of Plants, which was obtained from McGrawHills Science, Grade
2 (2000). These texts were representative of the text pool and
closely equated in terms of conventional measures of text dimensions (see Table 1). In reviewing Table 1, readers should note that
it is not feasible to match texts from different genres on all text
dimensions.
TABLE 1 Characteristics of the Narrative and Expository Texts
Measure
Number of words
Number of sentences
FleschKincaid grade level
Flesch Reading ease
Narrative Text
(Orlando)
Expository Text
(Needs of Plants)
445
61
3.6
85.81
462
45
4.4
90.40
Differential Competencies
143
COMPREHENSION MEASURES
Comprehension was assessed using a combination of recall
tasks and multiple-choice questions. Multiple measures have the
benefit of providing more thorough evaluation of the breadth and
depth of comprehension.
We collected free recall and cued recall data to assess childrens comprehension of the texts. The free recall task required
children to report what they remembered about the passage they
had just read. Each child was provided the following directive: Tell
me everything you can remember about what you have just read.
Give me as many details as possible, like you were trying to tell
a friend about you just read. All responses were recorded on an
audiotape and later transcribed.
Cued recall, which assessed major themes in the text, was
used to evoke richer content from the children (Zinar, 1990).
The cued recall task required children to respond to three directives. The directives were designed to assess comprehension
of three major sections of the texts, and they essentially covered
the entire text. For the Orlando text, children were directed to
(a) Tell me everything that Salvador did after his mama told
him they would have to sell Orlando, (b) Tell me everything
that Salvadors mama said at first about what they needed to do
with Orlando, and (c) Tell me everything that happened to
Salvador and what he did after the storm began. For the plant
text, children were directed to (a) Tell me everything about
the parts of a plant and how those parts give the plant what it
needs, (b) Tell me everything that makes leaves important to
plants and how those parts give the plant what it needs, and
(c) Tell me everything that plants do in response to changes
in their environment. All responses were audiotaped and later
transcribed.
Twelve multiple-choice questions for each text measured
textbase and situation model levels of understanding (Givon, 1993;
Kintsch, 1998; Louwerse, 2002; McNamara et al., 1996). Six questions were classified as text-based, and six were classified as bridging inference questions (see Appendix). For the narrative text,
the odd-numbered questions were text-based questions, and the
even-numbered questions were bridging inference questions. The
order was reversed for the expository text. Each question and three
possible answers were presented orally and visually, and children
144
R. M. Best et al.
Differential Competencies
145
146
R. M. Best et al.
was extrapolated from but not directly specified in the texts). Using previous inference research as a guide (e.g., Kintsch, 1993),
inferences were classified as text-based, elaborative, global, or irrelevant. However, we did not further analyze inference data because
too few inferences were produced (inferences comprised only 3%
of cued recall question answers and 1% of free recall question
answers).
All the free recall and cued recall was coded by a trained graduate student. Half the data was second coded by the first author.
Interrater reliability was evaluated for all dimensions of coding
(see below). Simple agreement and kappa analyses indicated that
agreement reached 90% or above on all dimensions, which indicates a high level of agreement. Disagreements were resolved by
discussion between raters.
PROPOSITION ANALYSIS
The proposition-based analysis was conducted from the recall
transcriptions. The recall analyses assessed the number of propositions recalled. To account for the completeness of information recalled, a value of 1.0 was assigned to recall that contained the main
proposition and more detailed information cited in the subproposition. A value of 0.5 was assigned to recall data that contained the
main proposition but that did contain the detailed information in
the subproposition. A value of 0 was assigned when the proposition
was not recalled. For example, a value of 0.5 was assigned for the
sentence Plants need sunlight, water, and air to live when a child
stated that plants need water to live. A value of 1 was assigned when
the child stated that plants need water, sunlight, and air. Because
childrens propositional recall sometimes contained erroneous information, recall that contained such information received a score
of 0.5. For example, a child may have erroneously stated, Plants
do not need sunlight. Once recall scores were totaled for a text,
they were transformed into a proportion because there were an
unequal number of propositions contained in the two texts.
Free recall and cued recall propositions were summed into
separate scores to focus on different dimensions of comprehension
across tasks. Thus, it was possible for children to recall the same
information in the free recall task and in the cued recall task.
For the free recall analysis, proportion scores were calculated by
dividing the free recall score by the number of propositions that
147
Differential Competencies
could potentially be recalled for each text. For the cued recall
analysis, the cued recall score was divided by the number of directly
relevant propositions that could be potentially recalled.
To determine which sentences contained directly relevant information, raters identified propositions that directly related to
the cued recall directives from three categories: directly related
sentences, indirectly related sentences, and irrelevant sentences.
Note that the same proposition was sometimes classified as directly
relevant for more than one cued recall question; in such cases, the
same proposition was counted for each of the relevant questions.
Kappa analyses showed that there was a high level of agreement
between raters across both texts (weighted kappa = .85 expository
text and .85 for the narrative text). Disagreements were resolved
by discussion. There were 32 main propositions directly relevant
to the expository directives and 35 propositions directly relevant
to the narrative directives.
Results
Table 2 presents the descriptive statistics from the free recall,
cued recall, and multiple-choice questions. For the multiple-choice
questions, data were analyzed for all questions for each text, regardless of type (e.g., text-based or bridging inference). Internal consistency reliability (coefficient alpha) was notably higher for the
scores stemming from summing across all questions, and preliminary analysis did not indicate differences in childrens responses
for the different question types. Preliminary analyses indicated
that the proportion scores from the recall tasks were low, which
indicates that children recalled a limited amount of information
relating to the texts.
Narrative Text
M (SD)
Expository Text
M (SD)
Multiple-choice questions
Free recall propositions
Cued recall propositions
.72 (.17)
.10 (.07)
.15 (.10)
.49 (.15)
.04 (.03)
.07 (.03)
148
R. M. Best et al.
Genre
The first set of analyses investigated of the role of genre in childrens comprehension of the narrative and expository texts. A review of the descriptive statistics included in Table 3 revealed that
across the comprehension measures, childrens scores were notably higher for the narrative text than for the expository text.
Within-subjects ANOVAs confirmed that children comprehended
the narrative text better than expository text across the multiplechoice questions, F(1,60) = 54.87, MSE = 0.029, p < 0.001, the free
recall task, F(1,55) = 40.62, MSE = 0.002, p < 0.001, and the cued
recall task, F(1,54) = 30.48, MSE = 0.006, p < 0.001. These results
demonstrate that that childrens comprehension, across different
methods of assessment, was notably affected by text genre.
We hypothesized that the childrens performance on the comprehension tasks would be affected by their competencies as well
as genre. To investigate the importance of the observed effects of
TABLE 3 Correlations Between Comprehension of the Narrative and
Expository Texts, World Knowledge, and Decoding Skills
Reading Competency Measure
Comprehension Measure
by Genre
Narrative multiple-choice
questions
Narrative free recall
propositions recalled
Narrative cued recall
propositions recalled
Expository multiple-choice
questions
Expository free recall
propositions recalled
Expository cued recall
propositions recalled
World
Knowledge
(M = 97.4,
SD = 11.2)
U
Decoding
Skills
(M = 102.1,
SD = 9.7)
U
t Value for
Differences
Between
Corrected
Correlations
.36
.46 .47
.64
4.85
.28
.36
.46
.63
6.49
.41
.52 .48
.65
4.01
.37
.47
.14
.21
3.67
.47 .58
.33
.48
2.76
.44 .55
.14
.21
3.09
Differential Competencies
149
150
R. M. Best et al.
had high decoding skills and vice versa. Thus, there are shared
influences between world knowledge and decoding skills in text
comprehension, particularly for narrative texts.
Multiple Regression
A series of hierarchical (also called sequential) multiple regression analyses were used to identify the relative importance and
incremental value of world knowledge and decoding skills to comprehension of the narrative and expository texts. Building on the
correlation analyses, scores from the multiple-choice questions,
the free recall propositions recalled, and the cued recall propositions recalled for each text were used as dependent variables.
Independent variables were world knowledge and decoding skills.
Table 4 presents a summary of the regression analyses for the narrative text, and Table 5 presents a summary of the regression analyses
for the expository text.
For the narrative text, when the decoding skills variable was
entered first in the equation as an independent variable, it was
TABLE 4 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analyses for Comprehension of
Narrative Text
Regression and
Order of Entry
SE B
R2
"R 2
F(df 1,df2)
.47
.09
.37
.41
.22
.23
.14
.27
.00
.09
16.82 (1,59)
.28 (1,58)
9.33 (1,59)
6.67 (1,59)
<.001
.598
.003
.012
.46
.07
.28
.50
.21
.21
.08
.21
.00
.13
15.43 (1,58)
.16 (1,57)
5.03 (1,58)
9.59 (1,57)
<.001
.692
.029
.003
.48
.16
.41
.38
.23
.25
.17
.25
.01
.08
16.96 (1,56)
.95 (1,55)
11.53 (1,56)
5.46 (1,55)
<.001
.333
.001
.023
151
Differential Competencies
SE B
R2
"R 2
F(df1,df2)
.37
.21
.15
.51
.14
.16
.02
.16
.02
.14
9.49 (1,59)
1.53 (1,58)
1.34 (1, 59)
9.57 (1, 58)
.003
.221
.251
.003
.41
.12
.34
.33
.17
.17
.11
.17
.01
.06
10.71 (1,54)
.50 (1,53)
6.94 (1, 54)
3.81 (1, 53)
.002
.483
.011
.056
.47
.26
.13
.61
.19
.23
.02
.23
.04
.21
12.65 (1,54)
2.74 (1,53)
.95 (1, 54)
14.61 (1, 53)
.001
.104
.335
<.001
152
R. M. Best et al.
Differential Competencies
153
used for memories of the text base and for inference making (Perfetti, 1985; Seigneuric, Ehrlich, Oakhill, & Yuill, 2000). Although
our analyses indicated notable relations between world knowledge
and comprehension of narrative texts, most children have welldeveloped schemata about the settings, actions, and events described by narrative texts (Cote et al., 1998; Nelson, 1996), so individual differences in world knowledge may be less important for
accurate comprehension than those associated with the accurate
and automatic decoding of words.
Too little research has examined the comprehension of expository texts with children in the middle of elementary school (cf.
Wilson & Rupley, 1997). However, previous research has shown that
there are large effects of knowledge on reading comprehension
for adults and older school students (e.g., Afflerbach, 1986; McNamara & Kintsch, 1996). Indeed, the effects of knowledge often
override effects of reading abilities (Chiesi et al., 1979). This study
provides evidence that world knowledge is an important consideration when comprehension problems are observed during the
elementary school yearsparticularly for expository texts. For example, successful comprehension of the expository text, a science
text focusing on the needs of plants, was shown to be influenced
by the level of readers prior knowledge. The relations between
comprehension of this text and world knowledge, across a number of measures of comprehension, were significant and moderate
in magnitude. Decoding skills demonstrated much lower and inconsistent relations with comprehension of our expository text. In
fact, when allowed to compete in the prediction of comprehension of expository texts, the influence of world knowledge largely
dwarfed that of decoding skills. We can conclude that children
with less prior knowledge will struggle to form a coherent situation model when reading expository texts because they are not
able to generate the necessary inferences.
Our results are indicative of one problem that children may
face when they reach the third and fourth gradeshow to deal
with low cohesion science texts that demand knowledge that these
readers may not yet possess. This situation, we expect, could spiral
such that the child fails to understand the learning material and
thus goes deeper into a knowledge debt (Stanovich, 1986). By
the fourth grade, the situation would be aggravated and readily
apparent. Certainly, other factors will come into play during this
154
R. M. Best et al.
Differential Competencies
155
156
R. M. Best et al.
elementary grade levels use short sentences and include only words
that are relatively familiar, these texts often lack the cohesive devises that young readers need to comprehend them well (Graesser,
McNamara, & Louwerse, 2003). For example, text writers may
assume a prior knowledge on the part of the reader and consequently leave out information that scaffolds understanding (Beck,
McKeown, Sinatra, & Loxterman, 1991). This omission of information can be considered a cohesion gap that is often detrimental
to comprehension for low-knowledge readers. In addition, thirdand fourth-grade texts often contain very few connectives, such as
although, because, consequently, and so on. Connectives are essential
for readers with less knowledge to know what relationships exist
between successive ideas.
We assume that text quality can be improved and that this quality can be tailored to reader competencies. For instance, comprehension for low-knowledge readers may be facilitated by more cohesive texts, which contain fewer cohesion gaps (Britton, Gulgoz,
& Glynn, 1993; McNamara et al., 1996; McNamara & Kintsch, 1996;
Vidal-Abarca, Martinez, & Gilabert, 2000). The general approach
to increasing text cohesion is to add surface-level indicators of
relations between ideas in the text, such as explicit linguistic elements (e.g., words, features, cues, signals, and constituents), that
guide the reader to the meaning of the text. Thus, signaling causal
connections, with terms such as because and consequently, help the
reader interpret and remember relationships between concepts.
Accordingly, one way to help readers comprehend texts is to modify
cohesion. This modification can be done in numerous ways, such
as adding low-level information (e.g., identifying anaphoric referents, synonymous terms, connective ties, and headers) and supplying background information that was previously left unstated in
the text.
However, one roadblock to enhancing the cohesion of texts
has been the lack of automated tools measuring cohesion. Currently, readers, writers, editors, educators, researchers, and policymakers can only estimate the appropriateness of a text using common readability measures, such as Flesch Reading Ease and the
FleschKincaid Grade Level. These measures are based on superficial factors, such as the number of words in the sentences and
the number of letters or syllables per word (Beck et al., 1991; Britton et al., 1993). A new automated tool, Coh-Metrix (version 1.4;
Differential Competencies
157
158
R. M. Best et al.
Differential Competencies
159
References
Addison-Wesley. (1998). Phonics take-home reader, grade 2. Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Author.
Afflerbach, P. (1986). The influence of prior knowledge on expert readers importance assignment process. In J. A. Niles & R. V. Lalik (Eds.), National reading
conference yearbook, 35. Solving problems in literacy: Learners, teachers and researchers
(pp.3040). Rochester, NY: National Reading Conference.
Artelt, C., Schiefele, U., & Schneider, W. (2001). Predictors of reading literacy.
European Journal of Psychology of Education, 16, 363383.
Baker, L. (1985). Differences in the standards used by college students to evaluate
their comprehension of expository prose. Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 297
313.
Baker, L. (1996). Social influences on metacognitive development in reading. In
C. Cornoldi & J. Oakhill (Eds.), Reading comprehension difficulties (pp. 331351).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Baker, S., Gersten, R., & Grossen, B. (2002). Interventions for students with reading comprehension problems. In M. R. Shinn, H. M. Walker, & G. Stoner
(Eds.), Interventions for academic and behavior problems II: Prevention and remedial approaches (pp. 731754). Bethesda, MD: National Association of School
Psychologists.
Beck, I. L., & McKeown, M. G. (1992). Young students social studies learning:
Going for depth. In W. H. Slater (Eds.). Elementary school literacy: Critical issues
(pp. 133156). Norwood MA: Christopher Gordon.
Beck, I. L., McKeown, M. G., Sinatra, G. M., & Loxterman, J. A. (1991). Revising
social studies text from a text processing perspective: Evidence of improved
comprehensibility. Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 251276.
160
R. M. Best et al.
Best, R. M., Ozuru, Y., & McNamara, D. S. (2004, June). Self-explaining science
texts: Strategies, knowledge and reading skill. Paper presented at the International
Conference for the Learning Sciences, Los Angeles, CA.
Bowen, B. A. (1999). Four puzzles in adult literacy: Reflections on the national
adult literacy survey. Journal of Adolescent and Adult Literacy, 42, 314323.
Brand-Gruwel, S., Aarnoutse, C. A. J., & Van den Bos, K. P. (1998). Improving
text comprehension strategies in reading and listening settings. Learning and
Instruction, 8, 6381.
Bridge, C. A., & Tierney, R. J. (1981). The inferential operations of children across
texts with narrative and expository tendencies. Journal of Reading Behavior, 8,
201214.
Britton, B. K., Gulgoz, S., & Glynn, S. (1993). Impact of good and poor writing
on learners: Research and theory. In B. K. Britton, A. Woodward, & M. R.
Binkley (Eds.), Learning from textbooks: Theory and practice (pp. 146). Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Cain, K. (1996). Story knowledge and comprehension skill. In C. Cornaldi &
J. Oakhill (Eds.), Reading comprehension difficulties: Processes and intervention
(pp. 167192).Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P. (2004). Childrens reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component
skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 3142.
Carroll, J. B. (2000). The analysis of reading instruction: Perspectives from psychology and linguistics. Scientific Studies of Reading, 4, 317.
Carver, R. P., & David, A. H. (2001). Investigating reading achievement using a
causal model. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5, 107140.
Chi, M., Feltovich, P., & Glaser, R. (1981). Categorization and representation of
physics problems by experts and novices. Cognitive Science, 5, 121152.
Chiesi, H. I., Spilich, G. J., & Voss, J. F. (1979). Acquisition of domain-related
information in relation to high and low domain knowledge. Journal of Verbal
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 275290.
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Cote, N., Goldman, S. R., & Saul, E. U. (1998). Students making sense of informational text: Relations between processing and representation. Discourse
Processes, 25, 153.
Curtis, M. E. (1980). Development of components of reading skill. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 90, 294311.
Ellis, E. S. (1993). Integrative strategy instruction: A potential model for teaching content-area subjects to learning disabled adolescents. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 26, 358383.
Englert, C. S., & Mariage, T. V. (1991). Making students partners in the comprehension process: Organizing the reading POSSE. Learning Disability Quarterly,
14, 123138.
Get a Fact Sheet for your community. (2006, July). Retrieved November 8, 2006 from
http://factfinder.census.gov/home/saff/main.html? lang=en
Givon,
T. (1993). English grammar. A function-based approach (2 vols.). Amsterdam:
John Benjamins.
Differential Competencies
161
162
R. M. Best et al.
Differential Competencies
163
Roth, F. P., Speece, D. L., Cooper, D. H., & De La Paz, S. (1996). Unresolved mysteries: How do metalinguistic and narrative skills connect with early reading?
Journal of Special Education, 30, 257277.
Rupley, H. R., & Willson, V. L. (1996). Content, domain, and world knowledge:
Relationship to comprehension of narrative and expository text. Reading and
Writing, 8, 419432.
Seigneuric, A., Ehrlich, M.-F., Oakhill, J. V., & Yuill, N. M. (2000). Working memory resources and childrens reading comprehension. Reading and Writing, 13,
81103.
Snow, C. (2002). Reading for understanding: Toward an R&D program in reading
comprehension. Santa Monica, CA: RAND.
Spiro, R. J., & Taylor, B. M. (1980). On investigating childrens transition from narrative to expository discourse: the multi-dimensional nature of psychological classification
(Tech. Rep. No. 195). Urbana-Champaign, University of Illinois, Center for
the Study of Reading.
Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21,
360407.
Stein, N. L., & Trabasso, T. (1981). Whats in a story: An approach to comprehension and instruction. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of instruction
(vol. 2, pp. 213267). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Storch, S. A., & Whitehurst, G. J. (2002). Oral language and code-related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal structural model. Developmental
Psychology, 38, 934947.
Sweet, A. P., & Snow, C. E. (Eds.). (2003). Rethinking reading comprehension. New
York: Guilford.
Taschow, H. G. (1969). Reading improvement in mathematics. Reading Improvement, 6, 6267.
Tun, P. A. (1989). Age differences in processing expository and narrative text.
Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 44, 915.
Vellutino, F. R. (2003). Individual differences as sources of variability in reading comprehension in elementary school children. In A. P. Sweet & C.
E. Snow (Eds.), Rethinking reading comprehension (pp. 5181). New York:
Guilford.
Vellutino, F. R., Scanlon, D. M., & Tanzman, M. S. (1994). Components of reading
ability: Issues and problems in operationalizing word identification, phonological coding, and orthographic coding. In G. R. Lyon (Ed.), Frames of reference
for the assessment of learning disabilities: New views on measurement issues (pp. 279
329). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
Vidal-Abarca, E., Martinez, G., & Gilabert, R. (2000). Two procedures to improve
instructional text: Effects on memory and learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 107116.
Williams, J. P., Hall, K. M., & Lauer, K. D. (2004). Teaching expository text structure to young at-risk learners: Building the basics of comprehension instruction, Exceptionality, 12, 129144.
Wilson, V. L., & Rupley, W. H. (1997). A structural equation model for reading
comprehension based on background, phonemic, and strategy knowledge.
Scientific Studies of Reading, 1, 4563.
164
R. M. Best et al.
Appendix
Multiple-choice questions
Questions
Orlando questions
Q1. What was Orlando?
Q2. Why did Mama tell Salvador that they had to sell
Orlando?
Q3. What was Salvador doing when his Mama told him that
they had to sell Orlando?
Q4. Why did Salvador run from his house?
Q5. How did Salvador follow Orlando home?
Q6. Why did Salvadors mother decide to keep Orlando?
Q7. What did Salvador use to make his fort?
Q8. Why was Salvadors Mama worried about Salvador?
Q9. Why did the tree catch on fire?
Q10. What area was between Salvadors house and the forest
where his fort was?
Q11. What happened when Salvador thought of his Mama in
the fort?
Q12. What caused Salvadors Mama to stand by the door on
the porch?
Plant questions
Q1. How do humans and animals help to meet the needs of
plants?
Q2. What gas do plants use to make food?
Q3. How does carbon dioxide enter plants?
Q4. What do roots do for plants?
Q5. Why would a plant grow toward a sunlit window?
Q6. Why do plants need sunlight?
Q7. What do both plants and animals need to live?
Q8. Why are plants important to humans?
Q9. Why do plants need chlorophyll to trap energy from
sunlight?
Q10. Which statement best describes simple plants?
Q11. Why would a big leaf make more food for a plant than
a small leaf?
Q12. What do our bodies use when we eat plants?
Question Type
Text-based
Inference
Text-based
Inference
Text-based
Inference
Text-based
Inference
Text-based
Inference
Text-based
Inference
Inference
Text-based
Inference
Text-based
Inference
Text-based
Inference
Text-based
Inference
Text-based
Inference
Text-based
Note. Questions eliminated from the analysis as children performed at ceiling level
(above 90% correct).