2000 Procrastination in Job-Seeking
2000 Procrastination in Job-Seeking
2000 Procrastination in Job-Seeking
Procrastination in Job-Seeking:
An Analysis of Motivational Processes and
Feelings of Hopelessness
Caroline Senécal
Frédéric Guay
Université Laval
The purpose of this study was to propose and test a model of procrasti-
nation in job-seeking activities. This model posits that non self-deter-
mined job-seeking motivation (i.e., performing job-seeking activities
because of controls and pressure) is positively related to procrastination
in job-seeking activities. In addition, decisional procrastination is ex-
pected to be positively related to procrastination in job-seeking activi-
ties. In turn, procrastination in job-seeking is hypothesized to positively
predict change in hopelessness toward job-seeking. Participants were
345 university students who were about to graduate. Results from
regression analyses revealed that all hypothesized links were supported.
Discussion centers on the role of motivation in procrastination toward
job-seeking.
vated behaviors are those that are engaged in for their own sake, in other
words, for the pleasure and satisfaction derived from performing them
(Deci, 1971).
On the other hand, extrinsic motivation pertains to a wide variety of
behaviors where the goals of action extend beyond those inherent in the
activity itself. Different types of extrinsic motivations have been pro-
posed by Self-determination Theory which can also be ordered along the
self-determination continuum. From lower to higher levels of self-
determination, these are external, introjected, and identified regulations.
External regulation occurs when behavior is externally regulated by
rewards or in order to avoid negative consequences. That is, regardless
of whether the goal of behavior is to obtain rewards or to avoid sanctions,
the individual experiences an obligation to behave in a specific way.
Introjected regulation occurs when one performs the activity by internal
pressure such as guilt and self-approval. In contrast, identified regula-
tion occurs when a behavior is valued and perceived as being chosen by
oneself. Yet, the motivation is still extrinsic because the activity is not
performed for itself but as a means to an end.
Besides intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, Deci and Ryan (1985)
have proposed a third motivational concept namely, amotivation, to fully
understand human behavior. When amotivated, individuals experience a
lack of contingency between their behaviors and outcomes. Their behav-
iors are neither intrinsically nor extrinsically motivated. Amotivated
behaviors are the least self-determined because there is no sense of
purpose and no expectations of reward or possibility of changing the
course of events. Amotivation can thus be seen as similar to learned
helplessness (Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978) where the indi-
vidual experiences feelings of incompetence and expectancies of
uncontrollability.
According to Self-Determination Theory, these five types of moti-
vation are differentially related to various types of outcomes. These
motivations differ in their inherent levels of self-determination and self-
determination has been hypothesized to be associated with enhanced
psychological functioning (Deci, 1980; Deci & Ryan, 1985; Ryan, Deci
& Grolnick, 1995). One would expect intrinsic motivation to be mostly
associated with positive outcomes (e.g., satisfaction) followed by identi-
fied regulation. In contrast, the most negative outcomes (e.g., depressive
states) will stem from amotivation followed by introjected and external
regulations.
These findings have been obtained with several outcomes in various
life contexts (see Deci & Ryan, 1985; Vallerand, 1997 for a review). For
instance, research has shown that self-determined types of motivation
270 PROCRASTINATION: CURRENT ISSUES AND NEW DIRECTIONS
METHOD
Participants
University students were recruited on the basis of three characteris-
tics. First, they had to be involved in searching for a job. Second, they
had to be in their last semester before obtaining their bachelor degree.
Finally, they should not have the intention to pursue graduate studies
immediately after their bachelor degree. Participants were thus 345
university students (63% females; 37% males) and their mean age was
23 years. Among students who participated at Time-1, 112 participated
in the second phase of the study that was conducted 6 months later. The
mean age of this subsample was 23 years (24% males; 76% females).
Although the size of this sample was smaller than the original one, no
significant differences exist on initial measures (i.e., decisional procras-
tination, self-determined job-seeking motivation, hopelessness in job-
seeking) between students who did participate to the second phase of the
study versus those who did not participate.
272 PROCRASTINATION: CURRENT ISSUES AND NEW DIRECTIONS
Measures
Self-determined job-seeking motivation. Students completed the
Job-Seeking Motivation Inventory (JSMI; Senécal, 1998a) which as-
sesses students’ motivation toward job-seeking. The JSMI is composed
19 items that are divided into five subscales assessing the motivational
constructs of Self-determination Theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985, 1991).
One 4-item subscale assesses intrinsic motivation (e.g., “Because this
activity is full of interesting challenges”). Three subscales assess types
of extrinsic motivation: identified regulation (5 items; e.g., “Because I
choose this activity in order to attain my career goals”), introjected
regulation (4 items; e.g., “Because I feel obliged to find a job”), and
external regulation (3 items; e.g., “Because I want to have some money
rapidly’). One subscale assesses amotivation (3 items; e.g., ‘I don’t
know, I don’t see the relevance for myself’). Each item on the inventory
represents a possible reason for being involved in job-seeking. Items are
scored on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = not at all in agreement to 7 =
completely in agreement). Previous research has provided support for
the construct validity and the reliability of the scale (Senécal, 1998a). In
the present study, Cronbach alphas for the four subscales ranged from
.65 to .78.
The self-determined job-seeking motivation variable (i.e., perform-
ing job-seeking activities out of choice and pleasure) was computed to
reduce the number of variables involve in the regression analyses. This
variable was thus obtained by integrating the information from the
different motivational subscales. This was done by ascribing each subscale
a different weight and then summing the products. Consequently, intrin-
sic motivation and identified regulation subscales were assigned respec-
tively the score of +2 and +1. Amotivation and the two other types of
extrinsic motivation (introjected and external regulation) were attributed
respectively the weights of -2 and -1. The self-determined job-seeking
motivation score was thus computed using the following formula: ((2 ×
intrinsic motivation + identified regulation) - ((introjected regulation +
external regulation/2) + 2 × amotivation)). Possible range for this com-
posite measure was from -18 to + 18. Numerous studies have shown the
usefulness of this composite index (e.g., Vallerand, Fortier, & Guay,
1997; Guay & Vallerand, 1997).
Decisional procrastination. Students completed the French transla-
tion of the Mann’s Decisional Procrastination Scale (Mann, 1982). This
scale consists of 5 items rated on 5-point Likert-type scale (e.g., “I delay
making decisions until its too late’). In the present study, Cronbach alpha
for this scale was .86.
Senécal & Guay. PROCRASTINATION IN JOB-SEEKING 273
RESULTS
Preliminary Analyses
Table 1 presents correlations between the model variables. As
expected, a negative relation (r = -.25, p < .01) was found between Time-
1 self-determined job-seeking motivation and Time-2 procrastination in
job-seeking. Furthermore, procrastination in job-seeking (Time 2) was
positively related (r = .29, p < .01) to feelings of hopelessness in job-
seeking (Time 2). In addition, decisional procrastination at Time 1 was
positively associated (r = .30, p < 01) to Time-2 procrastination in job-
seeking. Finally, Time-1 hopelessness was strongly and positively re-
lated (r = .58, p < 001) to Time-2 hopelessness.
A Test of the Model
Baron’s and Kenny’s (1986) procedure was used to verify the
proposed model. According to this procedure, mediation is established
only if four conditions can be met. The first two conditions require a
demonstration in two separate regression equations that the independent
variables are related to both the dependent variable (first condition) and
the mediator (second condition). The third condition requires that the
mediator has an effect on the dependent variable after the effects of
predictors on the dependent variables are taken into account. The fourth
condition involves a comparison between results obtained under condi-
tion 1 and 3. In this case, empirical support for mediation is provided if
the effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable is
reduced when the effect of the mediator on the dependent variable is
accounted for. In the present study, the key mediator was Time-2
procrastination in job-seeking whereas the dependent variable was Time-
2 hopelessness in job-seeking. Finally, the independent variables were:
Senécal & Guay. PROCRASTINATION IN JOB-SEEKING 275
Trait of
decisional
procrastination
(Time 1) ns
.27
-.19
.29
Self-determined Procrastination Hopelessness
job-seeking -.21 in job-seeking .21
in job-seeking
motivation (Time 2) (Time 2)
(Time 1)
-.43
ns
.50
Hopelessness
in job-seeking
(Time 1)
nation (β = .01, ns), and Time-1 hopelessness (b = .57, p < .001). The
inclusion of Time-1 hopelessness in this equation enabled us to create
the change residual score in hopelessness (see Williams, & Deci, 1996
for a similar procedure). In a second step, Time-2 hopelessness was
regressed onto Time-1 self-determined job-seeking motivation (β = -.18,
p < .05). Results revealed that self-determined job-seeking motivation
had an unique contribution in predicting change in hopelessness toward
job-seeking (first condition).
The second equation assessed if sex, decisional procrastination,
Time-1 hopelessness, and self-determined job-seeking motivation are
related to Time-2 procrastination in job-seeking (i.e., the mediator). In a
first step, Time-2 procrastination in job-seeking was regressed onto sex
(β = .03, ns), decisional procrastination (β = .28, p < .01), and Time-1
hopelessness (β = .06, ns). Results thus revealed that decisional procras-
tination is positively related to procrastination in job-seeking. In a
second step, procrastination in job-seeking was regressed onto self-
determined job-seeking motivation (β = -.21, p < .05). Thus, self-
determined job-seeking motivation had unique negative contribution in
predicting Time-2 procrastination in job-seeking (second condition).
The third equation was then used to test whether Time-2 procrasti-
nation in job-seeking mediated the relationship between self-determined
Senécal & Guay. PROCRASTINATION IN JOB-SEEKING 277
DISCUSSION
The purpose of this study was to propose and test a model that
integrates antecedents and consequence of procrastination toward job-
seeking. We proposed that non self-determined job-seeking motivation
and decisional procrastination are positively related to the mediator of
procrastination in job-seeking. In turn, high levels of procrastination
toward job-seeking are expected to lead individuals to experience hope-
lessness toward job-seeking activities.
Results from regression analyses provide support for the media-
tional model. First, motivation had a unique positive contribution in
predicting procrastination toward job-seeking that was over and beyond
decisional procrastination. Second, high levels of procrastination were
associated with an increase in hopelessness toward job-seeking over a
six-month period. That is, the more individuals procrastinated in the job-
seeking process the greater their increase in hopelessness. Implications
of these results are discussed below.
As mentioned at the outset, most of the research on procrastination
has been conducted in the academic domain. Unfortunately, less re-
search has examined procrastination in the work area (Ferrari, 1992) and
more specifically related to job-seeking. Nevertheless, for students who
have received their diploma (BA or BS), having a job is an important step
to their social and professional integration. Therefore, it is important to
understand why some people delay their search for a job. The present
findings indicate that the more students have self-determined reasons to
search for a job, the less they procrastinate in job-seeking. Thus, the
more they have pleasure and satisfaction when they are engaged in job-
seeking and/or the more they perceive this activity as a personal choice,
the less they postpone job-seeking activities. On the contrary, the more
they feel external or internal pressure to perform this activity and/or the
more they perceive no relationship between their job-seeking activities
and the consequences of it, the more students delay job-seeking.
278 PROCRASTINATION: CURRENT ISSUES AND NEW DIRECTIONS
REFERENCES
Abramson, L.Y., Metalsky, G.I., & Alloy, L.B. (1989). Hopelessness depression:
A theory-based subtype of depression. Psychological Review, 96, 358–372.
Abramson, L.Y., Seligman, M.E.P., & Teasdale, J.D. (1978). Learned helpless-
ness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-
ogy, 87, 49–74.
Alloy, L.B., & Clements, C.M. (1992). Illusion of control: Invulnerability to
negative affect and depressive symptoms after laboratory and natural
stressors. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 234–245.
Audet, M. (1995). Qu’advient-il de nos dîplômés? Québec: Les Éditions du
Québec.
Baron, R.M., & Kenny, D.A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinc-
tion in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical
considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–
1182.
Beck, A.T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects.
Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Beck, A.T., Weissman, A., Lester, D., & Trexler, L. (1974). The measurement of
pessimism: The hopelessness scale. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 42, 861–865.
Beswick, G., Rothblum, E.D., & Mann, L. (1988). Psychological antecedents of
student procrastination. Australian Psychologist, 23, 207–217.
Blankstein, K.R., & Flett, G.L. (1990). Cognitive components of test anxiety: A
comparison of assessment and scoring methods. Journal of Social Behavior
and Personality, 5, 187–202.
Blankstein, K.R., Flett, G.L., Watson, M.S., & Koledin, S. (1990). Test anxiety,
self-evaluative worry, and sleep disturbance in college students. Anxiety
Research, 3, 193–204.
Deci, E.L. (1980). The psychology of self-determination. Lexington, MA: DC
Health.
Deci, E.L. (1971). Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrinsic motiva-
tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18, 105–115.
Deci, E.L. & Ryan, R.M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in
human behavior. New York: Plenum.
Deci, E.L. & Ryan, R.M. (1987). The support of autonomy and the control of
behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1024–1037.
Deci, E.L. & Ryan, R.M. (1991). A motivational approach to self: Integration in
personality. In R. Dienstbier (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation:
Vol. 38. Perspectives on motivation (pp. 237–288). Lincoln: University of
Nebraska Press.
Ferrari, J.R. (1991). Self-handicapping by procrastinators: Protecting self-es-
teem, social-esteem, or both? Journal of Research in Personality, 25, 245–
261.
Senécal & Guay. PROCRASTINATION IN JOB-SEEKING 281