HHS Public Access: Interoception and Mental Health: A Roadmap
HHS Public Access: Interoception and Mental Health: A Roadmap
HHS Public Access: Interoception and Mental Health: A Roadmap
Author manuscript
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018
Author Manuscript
July 20.
Published in final edited form as:
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2018 June ; 3(6): 501–513. doi:10.1016/j.bpsc.
2017.12.004.
Interoception and Mental Health: A Roadmap
Sahib S. Khalsa, Ralph Adolphs, Oliver G. Cameron, Hugo D. Critchley, Paul W. Davenport,
Justin S. Feinstein, Jamie D. Feusner, Sarah N. Garfinkel, Richard D. Lane, Wolf E. Mehling,
Alicia E. Meuret, Charles B. Nemeroff, Stephen Oppenheimer, Frederike H. Petzschner,
Olga Pollatos, Jamie L. Rhudy, Lawrence P. Schramm, W. Kyle Simmons, Murray B. Stein,
Klaas E. Stephan, Omer Van den Bergh, Ilse Van Diest, Andreas von Leupoldt, Martin P.
Paulus, and the Interoception Summit 2016 participants
Author Manuscript
Laureate Institute for Brain Research (SSK, JSF, WKS, MPP) and Oxley College of Health
Sciences (SSK, JSF, WKS) and Department of Psychology (JLR), University of Tulsa, Tulsa,
Oklahoma; California Institute of Technology (RA), Pasadena, Department of Psychiatry and
Biobehavioral Sciences (JDF), Semel Institute for Neuroscience and Human Behavior, University
of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, Department of Family and Community Medicine (WEM),
University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, and Department of Psychiatry (MBS),
University of California, San Diego, San Diego, California; Department of Psychiatry (OGC),
University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan; Department of Physiology (PWD), University of
Florida, Gainesville, and Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences (CBN), University of
Miami, Miami, Florida; Department of Psychiatry (RDL), University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona;
Department of Psychology (AEM), Southern Methodist University, Dallas, Texas; Department of
Neurology (SO), Department of Biomedical Engineering (LPS), and Department of Neuroscience
Author Manuscript
(LPS), Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland; Sackler Centre for Consciousness Science
(HDC, SNG), University of Sussex, Brighton, United Kingdom; Translational Neuromodeling Unit
(FHP, KES), Institute for Biomedical Engineering, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland;
Department of Clinical and Health Psychology (OP), Institute of Psychology and Education, Ulm
University, Ulm, Germany; Department of Health Psychology (OVdB, IVD, AvL), University of
Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
Abstract
Interoception refers to the process by which the nervous system senses, interprets, and integrates
signals originating from within the body, providing a moment-by-moment mapping of the body’s
Author Manuscript
internal landscape across conscious and unconscious levels. Interoceptive signaling has been
Author Manuscript
considered a component process of reflexes, urges, feelings, drives, adaptive responses, and
cognitive and emotional experiences, highlighting its contributions to the maintenance of
homeostatic functioning, body regulation, and survival. Dysfunction of interoception is
increasingly recognized as an important component of different mental health conditions,
including anxiety disorders, mood disorders, eating disorders, addictive disorders, and somatic
symptom disorders. However, a number of conceptual and methodological challenges have made it
difficult for interoceptive constructs to be broadly applied in mental health research and treatment
settings. In November 2016, the Laureate Institute for Brain Research organized the first
Interoception Summit, a gathering of interoception experts from around the world, with the goal of
accelerating progress in understanding the role of interoception in mental health. The discussions
at the meeting were organized around four themes: interoceptive assessment, interoceptive
integration, interoceptive psychopathology, and the generation of a roadmap that could serve as a
Author Manuscript
guide for future endeavors. This review article presents an overview of the emerging consensus
generated by the meeting.
Keywords
Biomarker; Computational psychiatry; Interoception; Mental health; Research Domain Criteria;
Treatment
Interoception refers collectively to the processing of internal bodily stimuli by the nervous
system. Parcellation of the nervous system’s processing of sensory signals into
interoception, proprioception, and exteroception began more than 100 years ago (1),
although it was predated by interest in linking body–brain interactions with conscious
experience (2,3). Scientific interest in interoception has fluctuated (Figure 1A). During the
Author Manuscript
ASSESSMENT
Author Manuscript
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 July 20.
Khalsa et al. Page 3
within distinct research areas or scientific disciplines [see (24,25) for noteworthy
Author Manuscript
exceptions].
Features of Interoception
Interoception is not a simple process but rather has several facets (26). The act of sensing,
interpreting, and integrating information about the state of inner body systems can be related
to different elements such as interoceptive attention, detection, discrimination, accuracy,
insight, sensibility, and self-report (Table 2). However, most interoceptive processes occur
outside the realm of conscious awareness. Consciously experienced elements are measured
clinically via subjective report, and there are few observable interoceptive signs (e.g., heart
rate, respiration rate, pupillary dilation, flushing, perspiration, piloerection, nociceptive
reflexes) (Table 3). Experimental approaches can quantify different body systems and
features of interoceptive processing. Nevertheless, these measures are only partially
Author Manuscript
overlapping and likely reflect somewhat distinct neural processes (27). Access to the full
range of interoceptive signals often involves invasive approaches, which tend to elicit
physiological perturbations and index more objectively measurable features (28). However,
many insights have been gained by the application of noninvasive approaches within
neuroscience and psychological assessment contexts (29) (see “Eavesdropping on Brain–
Body Communication” section below).
Interoceptive awareness is an umbrella term that was first used to describe a self-report
subscale (30), but it has subsequently been used to encompass any (or all) of the different
interoception features accessible to conscious self-report. Researchers from different fields
developed definitions that only partially overlapped, reflecting the need for
operationalization in neuroscience (31,32) and clinical practice (33,34). Here we develop a
more coherent nomenclature for its various components (Table 2), mirroring developments
in other fields, especially pain (35). One key aspect is the importance of distinguishing
sensation (i.e., the raw signals conveyed by bodily sensors) from perception (36,37). We
return to this theme below.
Multilevel Investigations
While interoception research to date has typically focused on single organ systems, an
Author Manuscript
expanded approach that assesses multiple interoceptive organ systems and/or elements is
needed. Examples include targeting numerous interoceptive features simultaneously and
employing different tasks that converge on the same feature (e.g., combining top-down
1Visceroception has classically referred to the perception of bodily signals arising specifically from visceral organs, such as the heart,
lungs, stomach, intestines, and bladder, along with other internal organs in the trunk of the body (19). It did not include organs such as
the skin and skeletal muscle, in contrast to contemporary definitions of interoception that typically encompasses signals from both the
viscera and all other tissues that relay a signal to the central nervous system about the current state of the body, including the skin and
skeletal/smooth muscle fibers, via lamina I spinothalamic afferents (41,138,139).
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 July 20.
Khalsa et al. Page 4
Sensing Perturbations
The inner and outer worlds of the body constantly fluctuate. The nervous system monitors
these environmental changes and responds adaptively in order to maintain a homeostatic
balance and promote survival. Because psychiatric disorders often promote or reflect the
development of chronic homeostatic and allostatic disturbances (38), there is a need for
methods capable of eliciting homeostatic perturbations in controlled settings, especially
those assessing subjective and behavioral responses to valence and arousal deviations.
However, interoception is not simply about afferent processing. The brain’s constant
monitoring of the body occurs in service of optimizing homeostatic regulation. This efferent
limb is understudied (39), and paradigms that can effectively measure visceromotor outputs
Author Manuscript
INTEGRATION
Interoception and Domain Specificity Within the Brain
There are fundamentally differing ways to interpret the evolution of brain and body
signaling in humans. The processing of interoceptive input could be domain specific, with
modular processing occurring in specialized, encapsulated neural circuits [e.g., cardiac,
respiratory, urinary, genital, chemical, hormonal; see (40) for a review of domain specificity]
or functionally coupled (e.g., cardiorespiratory, genitourinary, chemohormonal) and
Author Manuscript
integrated within a single neural circuit. Understanding the adaptive origins and functions of
interoceptive domain specidicity (if present) could tell us how the implementation and
deployment of interoceptive signals by the nervous system contributes to disordered mental
health. Because interoceptive signaling involves afferent and efferent inputs across multiple
hierarchies within the autonomic and central nervous systems, identifying where and how
information processing dysfunctions negatively affect mental health represents a challenging
problem.
Relay pathways involve primarily spinal, vagal, and glossopharyngeal afferents, with
multiple levels of processing and integration in autonomic ganglia and spinal cord
(10,19,22,41). Several brainstem (nucleus of the solitary tract, parabrachial nucleus, and
periaqueductal gray), subcortical (thalamus, hypothalamus, hippocampus, and amygdala),
and cortical regions (insula and somatosensory cortices) represent key afferent processing
regions (22,42,43). A complementary set of regions involved in visceromotor actions
represents key efferent processing regions, including the anterior insula, anterior cingulate,
subgenual cingulate, orbitofrontal, ventromedial prefrontal, supplementary motor, and
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 July 20.
Khalsa et al. Page 5
premotor areas (44–46). It is noteworthy that these neural regions coincide closely with
Author Manuscript
other sensory processing systems, especially the nociceptive and affective systems. The
degree to which these represent distinct or overlapping systems is currently unclear.
spinal cord, and peripheral organs (49). However, it is also possible that multisystem
assessments may reduce specificity for certain disorders and therefore may be unnecessary.
For example, some patients with panic disorder may experience dyspnea but not
palpitations. Localizing and then targeting the dysfunctional interoceptive domain would
become more useful than broad multisystem interventions.
and odor aversion (59)]. The principal utility of animal models is the hypothesis testing of
mechanistic processes at the biological level independent of appraisal and cognition. These
include examining effects of peripheral or central nervous system lesions on physiology/
behavior, or mapping of peripheral/central interactions via stimulation of selective neurons/
circuits using optogenetic methods (60,61), and targeted gene expression manipulation to
test genetic hypotheses (62). Animal models are advantageous in that they allow for
identification of neural mechanisms that may be distinct from higher cognitive processes
(e.g., nonmammalian [reptiles/birds] vs. mammalian [mice/rats/monkeys/apes/chimpanzees],
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 July 20.
Khalsa et al. Page 6
nonhuman primates offers intriguing opportunities. Investigations in this area have been
centered primarily on neural encoding of baroreceptor afferent stimulation (9) and
neuroanatomical circuit tracing (63). Fewer studies have examined relationships between
mechanistic manipulation of interoceptive experiences and neural representation in these
animals [see (64,65) for exceptions].
processes have most typically involved selective regulation of attentional focus (29,71) and,
less commonly, expectancy manipulations such as placebo/sham delivery (72). Functional
magnetic resonance imaging (73), positron emission tomography (74), and
electroencephalography (75,76) have provided the primary means of assessing neural
circuitry. However, a host of novel tools are capable of inhibiting, stimulating, or modulating
the activity of interoceptive brain networks. Noninvasive methods include the application of
transcranial magnetic stimulation (77), transcranial direct and alternating current stimulation
(78), low-intensity focused ultrasound (79), temporally interfering electric fields (80),
transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation (81), presentation of information during different
phases of visceral rhythms (e.g., cardiac systole vs. diastole) (82), and assessment of
corticocardiac signaling (83). An important point is that many of the critical brain structures
are difficult to modulate noninvasively because they are located deep within the brain or near
Author Manuscript
the midline. Invasive measures do not share this limitation, and while their implementation is
driven by clinical concerns, they can provide important insights. These include implanted
vagus nerve stimulation (84), direct brain stimulation (85), and intracranial electrode
recordings (86,87). Beyond these perturbation tools, the use of experimental methods to
modulate expectancies, such as placebo and sham interventions, is key. These methods will
help to determine how sensitive psychiatric and other clinical patients’ afferent/efferent
feedback loops are to processes requiring integrations of environmental context with body–
brain signals (illustrated in the next section). Finally, neurofeedback (e.g., functional
magnetic resonance imaging, electroencephalogram) represents an exciting opportunity to
participate in the brain–body conversation by simultaneously measuring and modulating
brain regions during treatment [for a noninteroceptive example, see (88)]. Equipped with
these tools, the future looks promising, but to advance progress they need to be paired with
Author Manuscript
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 July 20.
Khalsa et al. Page 7
a predictive mapping (from hidden bodily states to interosensations) with prior information
Author Manuscript
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 July 20.
Khalsa et al. Page 8
PSYCHOPATHOLOGY
Author Manuscript
Interoceptive Psychopathology
Several conceptual and heuristic models have linked dysfunctions of interoception to mental
health conditions. Specifically, mood and anxiety disorders have been linked to failures to
appropriately anticipate changes in interoceptive states (97). Eating disorders show
behavioral and neural abnormalities in interoceptive processing, particularly in the context of
caloric anticipation (72,98–100), although it remains unclear whether this is due to altered
afferent signaling, altered central sensory processing, abnormal temperament, and/or
metacognition. Drug addiction, another condition marked by interoceptive disturbances, has
an overlapping neural circuitry and abnormal responses to interoceptive cues (101–104).
Interoceptive dysfunction also likely plays a role in conditions such as posttraumatic stress
disorder and somatic symptom disorders (33). Other disorders also have interoceptive
Author Manuscript
symptom overlap; however, the specific feature involved may differ according to the
disorder or affected individual [e.g., chronic pain (105,106), Tourette’s syndrome and other
tic disorders, borderline personality disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder, autism
spectrum disorder (107), functional developmental disorders (108)]. Table 3 lists diagnostic
symptoms and clinical signs indicative of interoceptive dysfunction in several psychiatric
disorders. Conditions that have a psychiatric component include fibromyalgia, chronic
fatigue syndrome, irritable bowel syndrome, and functional disorders within medicine (e.g.,
noncardiac chest pain, functional dysphagia) as well as certain medical disorders (e.g.,
gastroesophageal reflux, asthma).
identifying the potential role played by various interoceptive processes because several of
these might not be readily identified at the symptom report level relied on by clinicians and,
accordingly, might not have entered into the diagnostic specifications for DSM. This would
allow for identification of mechanistic dysfunctions across units of analyses and might
bridge the biological gap in current diagnostic classification frameworks by directly probing
the links between physiological and psychological dysfunctions. Interoceptive investigations
in mental health populations might reveal evidence of 1) attentional bias (e.g.,
hypervigilance), 2) distorted physiological sensitivity (e.g., blunted or heightened magnitude
estimation in response to a perturbation), 3) cognitive bias (e.g., catastrophizing in response
to an anticipated stimulus), 4) abnormal sensibility (e.g., tendency to label one’s experiences
in a particular way), and 5) impaired insight (e.g., poor confidence–accuracy correspondence
on a task).
Author Manuscript
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 July 20.
Khalsa et al. Page 9
The most common application of interoceptive evaluation in current clinical practice occurs
during interoceptive exposure psychotherapy for panic disorder (118). During this
procedure, patients self-induce varieties of interoceptive symptoms via low-arousal
manipulations (e.g., hyperventilation, performing jumping jacks, spinning in a chair,
breathing through a straw) while the clinician monitors their subjective distress level.
Unfortunately these manipulations often fail to adequately reproduce the fear response,
Author Manuscript
possibly because the patient retains full control over the stimulation (the patient can quit at
any time) and the perturbation remains predictable with minimal uncertainty, raising the
question of whether modulating both physiological homeostasis and the perception of
controllability might further improve the ecological validity and efficacy of interoceptive
exposures (119). A test to verify successful interoceptive exposure therapy for panic disorder
involves completion of a standardized behavioral avoidance paradigm (120). In this setting,
the degree of tolerance to being enclosed in a small dark chamber for 10 minutes might
provide behavioral evidence verifying tolerance to triggers of interoceptive dysregulation.
There is also experimental evidence that pharmacological interoceptive exposure therapy can
reduce anxiety disorder symptom severity either as monotherapy (7,121–123) or as an
augmentative approach (124). However, there are few studies of these procedures to date, the
impact of such interventions on longer term outcomes (e.g., 6 months or beyond) are
Author Manuscript
unknown, and none of these approaches has translated into clinical practice.
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 July 20.
Khalsa et al. Page 10
therapy with exposure and response prevention to reverse or attenuate conditioned fears or
Author Manuscript
ROADMAP
The Road Ahead
Beyond the issues outlined previously, progress in determining the relevance of
interoception for mental health relies on emphasizing the features that distinguish it from
other sensory modalities. Interoception seemingly involves a high degree of connectivity
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 July 20.
Khalsa et al. Page 11
within the brain (135). It appears to be tightly linked to the self and survival through
Author Manuscript
homeostatic maintenance of the body, and by helping us to represent how things are going in
the present with respect to the experienced past and the anticipated future. These
computations may depend on what has occurred to shape the body’s internal landscape, and
it is in this regard that learning, and malleability of representations over time, could play
important roles.
The conceptual framework for investigating interoception may overlap with other processes,
including emotion (136) and pain (137), because each is integral for maintaining bodily
homeostasis. An important endeavor may involve the identification of which neural systems
for interoception, emotion, cognition, and pain are overlapping, interdigitating, or even
possibly identical. Additional effort is needed to define the neurophysiological
nomenclature, core criteria, common features, developmental aspects, modulating factors,
functional consequences, and putative pathophysiologic mechanisms of interoception in
Author Manuscript
The current work offers some conceptual distinctions and some mutually agreed-on
terminology, with many others still needed. Several low-hanging fruits, as well as promising
emerging technologies and tools, have been mentioned. Further empirical work will be
critical to delineate how interoception can be mapped to mental health measures, models,
and approaches, and benchmarks for success/failure need to be established. Models of
interoceptive processing that improve on the traditional stimulus, sensorimotor processing,
and response function concepts have been described, but these models remain theoretical
and await further testing. Therefore, the current document is best viewed as a work in
progress.
Author Manuscript
Supplementary Material
Refer to Web version on PubMed Central for supplementary material.
Acknowledgments
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS AND DISCLOSURES
We express our sincere appreciation to the William K. Warren Foundation for supporting the Interoception Summit
2016 and to all of the Laureate Institute for Brain Research staff members for their assistance with facilitating the
meeting.
AEM reports the following disclosures: research/grant: National Institutes of Health (NIH) Grant No.
1U01EB021952-01; scientific advisory board: Anxiety and Depression Association of America. AvL reports the
following disclosures: research/grants: Research Fund KU Leuven, Belgium (Grant Nos. STRT/13/002 and DBOF/
14/021), an infrastructure grant from the Herculesstichting, Belgium (Grant No. AKUL/13/07), “Asthenes” long-
Author Manuscript
term structural funding Methusalem grant (Grant No. METH/15/011) by the Flemish Government, Belgium. CBN
reports the following disclosures: research/grant: NIH, Stanley Medical Research Institute; consulting (last 3 years):
Xhale, Takeda, Taisho Pharmaceutical, Inc., Prismic Pharmaceuticals, Bracket (Clintara), Total Pain Solutions,
Gerson Lehrman Group Healthcare & Biomedical Council, Fortress Biotech, Sunovion Pharmaceuticals, Inc.,
Sumitomo Dainippon Pharma, Janssen Research & Development, LLC, Magstim, Inc., Navitor Pharmaceuticals,
Inc., TC MSO, Inc.; stockholder: Xhale, Celgene, Seattle Genetics, Abbvie, OPKO Health, Inc., Bracket
Intermediate Holding Corporation, Network Life Sciences, Inc., Antares; scientific advisory boards: American
Foundation for Suicide Prevention (AFSP), Brain and Behavior Research Foundation (formerly National Alliance
for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression [NARSAD]), Xhale, Anxiety and Depression Association of
America (ADAA), Skyland Trail, Bracket (Clintara), RiverMend Health, LLC, Laureate Institute for Brain
Research, Inc.; board of directors: AFSP, Gratitude America, ADAA; income sources or equity of $10,000 or more:
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 July 20.
Khalsa et al. Page 12
American Psychiatric Publishing, Xhale, Bracket (Clintara), CME Outfitters, Takeda; patents: method and devices
for transdermal delivery of lithium (Patent No. US 6,375,990B1), method of assessing antidepressant drug therapy
Author Manuscript
via transport inhibition of monoamine neurotransmitters by ex vivo assay (Patent No. US 7,148,027B2). HDC
reports the following disclosures: research/grants: European Research Council Horizon 2020 Proof of Concept
Grant “HeartRater: Tools for the systematic evaluation of interoceptive ability,” Medical Research Council (UK)
MRC Confidence in Concept Grant “Identifying neural, cognitive, and phenomenological markers of auditory
verbal hallucinations in borderline personality,” MQ (Mental Health) PsyImpact “Aligning Dimensions of
Interoceptive Experience (ADIE) to prevent development of anxiety disorders in autism,” Dr. Mortimer and Theresa
Sackler Foundation Sackler Centre for Consciousness Science, University of Sussex, BIAL Foundation Bursary
“Microneurography as a tool for consciousness science”; scientific advisory boards: Emteq, Ltd., unpaid governor
on board of charity “Reflecting nature in art & science”; board of directors: Codirector of Sackler Centre for
Consciousness Science, University of Sussex. KES reports the following disclosures: research/grants: Deutsche
Forschungsgemeinschaft, Transregional Collaborative Research Centre, “Ingestive Behaviour: Homeostasis and
Reward,” René and Susanne Braginsky Foundation. LPS reports the following disclosures: scientific advisory
board: Laureate Institute for Brain Research, Inc. JDF reports the following disclosures: research/grants: NIH Grant
Nos. R01MH105662, R21MH110865, and R01HD087712; scientific advisory boards: International Obsessive-
Compulsive Disorder Foundation Clinical and Scientific Advisory Board. JLR reports the following disclosures:
research/grants: NIH Grant No. R01MD007807 and Oklahoma Center for the Advancement of Science and
Technology Grant No. HR15-079. JSF reports the following disclosures: research/grants: NIH/National Institute of
General Medical Sciences (NIGMS) Grant No. P20GM121312, Brain and Behavior Research Foundation (formerly
Author Manuscript
NARSAD) Young Investigator Award. MBS reports the following disclosures: scientific advisory board: Laureate
Institute for Brain Research, Inc.; editorial board: Depression and Anxiety, Biological Psychiatry. MPP reports the
following disclosures: research/grants: the William K. Warren Foundation and NIH Grant No. R01DA016663, NIH/
NIGMS Grant No. P20DA027834, and NIH Grant Nos. R01DA027797, R01DA018307, U01DA041089, and
1R01MH101453; consulting (last 3 years): has received royalties for an article about methamphetamine use
disorder from UpToDate. OVdB reports the following disclosures: scientific advisory boards: Research Training
Group 2271 of the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft on “Expectation maintenance vs. change in the context of
expectation violations: Connecting different approaches,” University of Marburg. RDL reports the following
disclosures: research/grant: NIH. SSK reports the following disclosures: research/grants: NIH/National Institute of
Mental Health Grant No. K23MH112949, NIH/NIGMS Grant No. P20GM121312, William K. Warren Foundation,
Brain and Behavior Foundation (formerly NARSAD) Young Investigator Award. WEM reports the following
disclosures: research/grants: NCIRE (Veterans Health Research Institute, San Francisco, California), Alzheimer’s
Association, Mental Insight Foundation, the Pepper Foundation, University of California, San Francisco, Resource
Allocation Program. WKS reports the following disclosures: research/grants: NIH Grant No. P20GM121312, Brain
and Behavior Foundation (formerly NARSAD) Young Investigator Award. The remaining authors report no
biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.
Author Manuscript
References
1Sherrington CS. The Integrative Action of the Nervous System New Haven, CT: Yale University
Press; 1906
2Cameron OG. Visceral Sensory Neuroscience: Interoception New York: Oxford University Press;
2002
3Adam G. Interoception and Behaviour Budapest, Hungary: Akadémiai Kiadó; 1967
4Gorman JM, Fyer AF, Gliklich J, King D, Klein DF. Effect of sodium lactate on patients with panic
disorder and mitral valve prolapse. Am J Psychiatry. 1981; 138:247–249. [PubMed: 7457651]
5Pohl R, Yeragani VK, Balon R, Rainey JM, Lycaki H, Ortiz A, et al. Isoproterenol-induced panic
attacks. Biol Psychiatry. 1988; 24:891–902. [PubMed: 3069135]
6Woods SW, Charney DS, Loke J, Goodman WK, Redmond DE, Heninger GR. Carbon dioxide
sensitivity in panic anxiety: Ventilatory and anxiogenic response to carbon dioxide in healthy
subjects and patients with panic anxiety before and after alprazolam treatment. Arch Gen
Psychiatry. 1986; 43:900–909. [PubMed: 3092780]
Author Manuscript
7van den Hout MA, van der Molen GM, Griez E, Lousberg H, Nansen A. Reduction of CO2-induced
anxiety in patients with panic attacks after repeated CO2 exposure. Am J Psychiatry. 1987;
144:788–791. [PubMed: 3109265]
8Margraf J, Ehlers A, Roth WT. Sodium lactate infusions and panic attacks: A review and critique.
Psychosom Med. 1986; 48:23–51. [PubMed: 3511491]
9Oppenheimer S, Cechetto D. The insular cortex and the regulation of cardiac function. Compr
Physiol. 2016; 6:1081–1133. [PubMed: 27065176]
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 July 20.
Khalsa et al. Page 13
10Shivkumar K, Ajijola OA, Anand I, Armour JA, Chen PS, Esler M, et al. Clinical neurocardiology
defining the value of neuroscience-based cardiovascular therapeutics. J Physiol. 2016; 594:3911–
Author Manuscript
16Nattie E, Li A. Central chemoreceptors: Locations and functions. Compr Physiol. 2012; 2:221–254.
[PubMed: 23728974]
17Stevenson RJ, Mahmut M, Rooney K. Individual differences in the interoceptive states of hunger,
fullness and thirst. Appetite. 2015; 95:44–57. [PubMed: 26119812]
18Fealey RD. Interoception and autonomic nervous system reflexes thermoregulation. Handb Clin
Neurol. 2013; 117:79–88. [PubMed: 24095117]
19Janig W. Neurobiology of visceral afferent neurons: Neuroanatomy, functions, organ regulations and
sensations. Biol Psychol. 1996; 42:29–51. [PubMed: 8770369]
20Capuron L, Miller AH. Immune system to brain signaling: Neuropsychopharmacological
implications. Pharmacol Ther. 2011; 130:226–238. [PubMed: 21334376]
21Irwin MR, Cole SW. Reciprocal regulation of the neural and innate immune systems. Nat Rev
Immunol. 2011; 11:625–632. [PubMed: 21818124]
22Critchley HD, Harrison NA. Visceral influences on brain and behavior. Neuron. 2013; 77:624–638.
[PubMed: 23439117]
Author Manuscript
23Dworkin BR. Interoception. In: Cacioppo JT, Tassinary LG, , Berntson GG, editorsHandbook of
Psychophysiology 2. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2000 10391506
24Tsakiris M, Critchley H. Interoception beyond homeostasis: Affect, cognition and mental health.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2016; 371:20160002. [PubMed: 28080961]
25Lane RD, Wager TD. Introduction to a special issue of NeuroImage on brain–body medicine. Neuro
Image. 2009; 47:781–784. [PubMed: 19523521]
26Vaitl D. Interoception. Biol Psychol. 1996; 42:1–27.
27Baranauskas M, Grabauskaite A, Griskova-Bulanova I. Brain responses and self-reported indices of
interoception: Heartbeat evoked potentials are inversely associated with worrying about body
sensations. Physiol Behav. 2017; 180:1–7. [PubMed: 28778551]
28Khalsa SS, Rudrauf D, Sandesara C, Olshansky B, Tranel D. Bolus isoproterenol infusions provide a
reliable method for assessing interoceptive awareness. Int J Psychophysiol. 2009; 72:34–45.
[PubMed: 18854201]
29Critchley HD, Wiens S, Rotshtein P, Ohman A, Dolan RJ. Neural systems supporting interoceptive
Author Manuscript
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 July 20.
Khalsa et al. Page 14
33Khalsa SS, Lapidus RC. Can interoception improve the pragmatic search for biomarkers in
psychiatry? Front Psychiatry. 2016; 7:121. [PubMed: 27504098]
Author Manuscript
27528671]
47von Leupoldt A, Sommer T, Kegat S, Baumann HJ, Klose H, Dahme B, et al. Dyspnea and pain
share emotion-related brain network. Neuro Image. 2009; 48:200–206. [PubMed: 19527787]
48De Cort K, Schroijen M, Hurlemann R, Claassen S, Hoogenhout J, Van den Bergh O, et al.
Modeling the development of panic disorder with interoceptive conditioning. Eur
Neuropsychopharmacol. 2017; 27:59–69. [PubMed: 27887860]
49Khan HS, Stroman PW. Inter-individual differences in pain processing investigated by functional
magnetic resonance imaging of the brainstem and spinal cord. Neuroscience. 2015; 307:231–241.
[PubMed: 26335379]
50Savitz J, Harrison NA. Interoception and inflammation in psychiatric disorders. Biol Psychiatry
Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2018; 3:514–524. [PubMed: 29884282]
51Gray MA, Taggart P, Sutton PM, Groves D, Holdright DR, Bradbury D, et al. A cortical potential
reflecting cardiac function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007; 104:6818–6823. [PubMed:
17420478]
Author Manuscript
52Aziz Q, Thompson DG, Ng VW, Hamdy S, Sarkar S, Brammer MJ, et al. Cortical processing of
human somatic and visceral sensation. J Neurosci. 2000; 20:2657–2663. [PubMed: 10729346]
53Wang GJ, Tomasi D, Backus W, Wang R, Telang F, Geliebter A, et al. Gastric distention activates
satiety circuitry in the human brain. Neuro Image. 2008; 39:1824–1831. [PubMed: 18155924]
54Wittmann M. The inner experience of time. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2009; 364:1955–
1967. [PubMed: 19487197]
55Ádám G. Visceral Perception: Understanding Internal Cognition New York: Plenum; 1998
56Dworkin BR. Learning and Physiological Regulation Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1993
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 July 20.
Khalsa et al. Page 15
57DeVille DC, Kerr KL, Avery JA, Burrows K, Bodurka J, Feinstein JS, et al. The neural bases of
interoceptive encoding and recall in healthy adults and adults with depression. Biol Psychiatry
Author Manuscript
63Mesulam MM, Mufson EJ. Insula of the old world monkey: I. Architectonics in the insulo-orbito-
temporal component of the para-limbic brain. J Comp Neurol. 1982; 212:1–22. [PubMed:
7174905]
64Mitz AR, Chacko RV, Putnam PT, Rudebeck PH, Murray EA. Using pupil size and heart rate to
infer affective states during behavioral neurophysiology and neuropsychology experiments. J
Neurosci Methods. 2017; 279:1–12. [PubMed: 28089759]
65Rudebeck PH, Putnam PT, Daniels TE, Yang T, Mitz AR, Rhodes SE, et al. A role for primate
subgenual cingulate cortex in sustaining autonomic arousal. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2014;
111:5391–5396. [PubMed: 24706828]
66Damasio AR. The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness
New York: Harcourt Brace; 1999
67Hannestad J, Subramanyam K, Dellagioia N, Planeta-Wilson B, Weinzimmer D, Pittman B, et al.
Glucose metabolism in the insula and cingulate is affected by systemic inflammation in humans. J
Nucl Med. 2012; 53:601–607. [PubMed: 22414635]
Author Manuscript
68Harrison NA, Brydon L, Walker C, Gray MA, Steptoe A, Critchley HD. Inflammation causes mood
changes through alterations in subgenual cingulate activity and mesolimbic connectivity. Biol
Psychiatry. 2009; 66:407–414. [PubMed: 19423079]
69Harrison NA, Brydon L, Walker C, Gray MA, Steptoe A, Dolan RJ, et al. Neural origins of human
sickness in interoceptive responses to inflammation. Biol Psychiatry. 2009; 66:415–422. [PubMed:
19409533]
70Schulz A, Strelzyk F, Ferreira de Sa DS, Naumann E, Vogele C, Schachinger H. Cortisol rapidly
affects amplitudes of heartbeat-evoked brain potentials—Implications for the contribution of stress
to an altered perception of physical sensations? Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013; 38:2686–2693.
[PubMed: 23850227]
71Avery JA, Drevets WC, Moseman SE, Bodurka J, Barcalow JC, Simmons WK. Major depressive
disorder is associated with abnormal interoceptive activity and functional connectivity in the
insula. Biol Psychiatry. 2014; 76:258–266. [PubMed: 24387823]
72Khalsa SS, Craske MG, Li W, Vangala S, Strober M, Feusner JD. Altered interoceptive awareness in
anorexia nervosa: Effects of meal anticipation, consumption and bodily arousal. Int J Eat Disord.
Author Manuscript
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 July 20.
Khalsa et al. Page 16
76von Leupoldt A, Keil A, Chan PY, Bradley MM, Lang PJ, Davenport PW. Cortical sources of the
respiratory-related evoked potential. Respir Physiol Neurobiol. 2010; 170:198–201. [PubMed:
Author Manuscript
20036344]
77Pollatos O, Herbert BM, Mai S, Kammer T. Changes in interoceptive processes following brain
stimulation. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2016; 371:20160016. [PubMed: 28080973]
78Fox KC, Christoff K. Transcranial direct current stimulation to lateral prefrontal cortex could
increase meta-awareness of mind wandering. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2015; 112:E2414.
[PubMed: 25858955]
79Monti MM, Schnakers C, Korb AS, Bystritsky A, Vespa PM. Non-invasive, ultrasonic thalamic
stimulation in disorders of consciousness after severe brain injury: A first-in-man report. Brain
Stimul. 2016; 9:940–941. [PubMed: 27567470]
80Grossman N, Bono D, Dedic N, Kodandaramaiah SB, Rudenko A, Suk HJ, et al. Noninvasive deep
brain stimulation via temporally interfering electric fields. Cell. 2017; 169:1029–1041. [PubMed:
28575667]
81Antonino D, Teixeira AL, Maia-Lopes PM, Souza MC, Sabino-Carvalho JL, Murray AR, et al. Non-
invasive vagus nerve stimulation acutely improves spontaneous cardiac baroreflex sensitivity in
Author Manuscript
healthy young men: A randomized placebo-controlled trial. Brain Stimul. 2017; 10:875–881.
[PubMed: 28566194]
82Azevedo RT, Garfinkel SN, Critchley HD, Tsakiris M. Cardiac afferent activity modulates the
expression of racial stereotypes. Nat Commun. 2017; 8:13854. [PubMed: 28094772]
83Panitz C, Hermann C, Mueller EM. Conditioned and extinguished fear modulate functional
corticocardiac coupling in humans. Psychophysiology. 2015; 52:1351–1360. [PubMed: 26189975]
84Nemeroff CB, Mayberg HS, Krahl SE, McNamara J, Frazer A, Henry TR, et al. VNS therapy in
treatment-resistant depression: Clinical evidence and putative neurobiological mechanisms.
Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006; 31:1345–1355. [PubMed: 16641939]
85Mazzola L, Mauguiere F, Isnard J. Electrical stimulations of the human insula: Their contribution to
the ictal semiology of insular seizures. J Clin Neurophysiol. 2017; 34:307–314. [PubMed:
28644200]
86Kern M, Aertsen A, Schulze-Bonhage A, Ball T. Heart cycle-related effects on event-related
potentials, spectral power changes, and connectivity patterns in the human ECoG. Neuro Image.
2013; 81:178–190. [PubMed: 23684883]
Author Manuscript
87Park HD, Bernasconi F, Salomon R, Tallon-Baudry C, Spinelli L, Seeck M, et al. Neural sources and
underlying mechanisms of neural responses to heartbeats, and their role in bodily self-
consciousness: An intracranial EEG study. Cereb Cortex. 2017 published online ahead of print Jun
7.
88Young KD, Misaki M, Harmer CJ, Victor T, Zotev V, Phillips R, et al. Real-time functional
magnetic resonance imaging amygdala neurofeedback changes positive information processing in
major depressive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2017; 82:578–586. [PubMed: 28476207]
89Petersen S, Schroijen M, Molders C, Zenker S, Van den Bergh O. Categorical interoception:
Perceptual organization of sensations from inside. Psychol Sci. 2014; 25:1059–1066. [PubMed:
24570260]
90Seth AK, Critchley HD. Extending predictive processing to the body: Emotion as interoceptive
inference. Behav Brain Sci. 2013; 36:227–228. [PubMed: 23663284]
91Pezzulo G, Rigoli F, Friston K. Active inference, homeostatic regulation and adaptive behavioural
control. Prog Neurobiol. 2015; 134:17–35. [PubMed: 26365173]
Author Manuscript
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 July 20.
Khalsa et al. Page 17
96Friston K. The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010; 11:127–138.
[PubMed: 20068583]
Author Manuscript
97Paulus MP, Stein MB. Interoception in anxiety and depression. Brain Struct Funct. 2010; 214:451–
463. [PubMed: 20490545]
98Berner LA, Simmons AN, Wierenga CE, Bischoff-Grethe A, Paulus MP, Bailer UF, et al. Altered
interoceptive activation before, during, and after aversive breathing load in women remitted from
anorexia nervosa. Psychol Med. 2018; 48:142–154. [PubMed: 28714434]
99Kerr KL, Moseman SE, Avery JA, Bodurka J, Zucker NL, Simmons WK. Altered insula activity
during visceral interoception in weight-restored patients with anorexia nervosa.
Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016; 41:521–528. [PubMed: 26084229]
100Frank GK. Advances from neuroimaging studies in eating disorders. CNS Spectr. 2015; 20:391–
400. [PubMed: 25902917]
101Goldstein RZ, Craig AD, Bechara A, Garavan H, Childress AR, Paulus MP, et al. The
neurocircuitry of impaired insight in drug addiction. Trends Cogn Sci. 2009; 13:372–380.
[PubMed: 19716751]
102Naqvi NH, Bechara A. The hidden island of addiction: The insula. Trends Neurosci. 2009; 32:56–
Author Manuscript
Prenatal stress exposure is associated with increased dyspnoea perception in adulthood. Eur
Respir J. 2017; 50:1700642. [PubMed: 28818876]
115Ondobaka S, Kilner J, Friston K. The role of interoceptive inference in theory of mind. Brain
Cogn. 2017; 112:64–68. [PubMed: 26275633]
116Ehlers A. A 1-year prospective study of panic attacks: Clinical course and factors associated with
maintenance. J Abnorm Psychol. 1995; 104:164–172. [PubMed: 7897040]
117Sundermann B, Bode J, Lueken U, Westphal D, Gerlach AL, Straube B, et al. Support vector
machine analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging of interoception does not reliably
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 July 20.
Khalsa et al. Page 18
predict individual outcomes of cognitive behavioral therapy in panic disorder with agoraphobia.
Front Psychiatry. 2017; 8:99. [PubMed: 28649205]
Author Manuscript
118Craske MG, , Barlow DH. Mastery of Your Anxiety and Panic: Therapist Guide 4. Oxford, UK:
Oxford University Press; 2007
119Abelson JL, Khan S, Liberzon I, Erickson TM, Young EA. Effects of perceived control and
cognitive coping on endocrine stress responses to pharmacological activation. Biol Psychiatry.
2008; 64:701–707. [PubMed: 18571624]
120Richter J, Hamm AO, Pane-Farre CA, Gerlach AL, Gloster AT, Wittchen HU, et al. Dynamics of
defensive reactivity in patients with panic disorder and agoraphobia: Implications for the etiology
of panic disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2012; 72:512–520. [PubMed: 22621998]
121Beck JG, Shipherd JC, Zebb BJ. How does interoceptive exposure for panic disorder work? An
uncontrolled case study. J Anxiety Disord. 1997; 11:541–556. [PubMed: 9407272]
122Forsyth JP, Lejuez CW, Finlay C. Anxiogenic effects of repeated administrations of 20% CO2-
enriched air: Stability within sessions and habituation across time. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry.
2000; 31:103–121. [PubMed: 11132115]
123Deacon B, Kemp JJ, Dixon LJ, Sy JT, Farrell NR, Zhang AR. Maximizing the efficacy of
Author Manuscript
prolonged transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation on heart rate variability in healthy subjects.
Auton Neurosci. 2017; 203:88–96. [PubMed: 28017263]
134Krause E, Benke C, Koenig J, Thayer JF, Hamm AO, Pané-Farré CA. Dynamics of defensive
response mobilization to approaching external versus interoceptive threat. Biol Psychiatry Cogn
Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2018; 3:525–538. [PubMed: 29884283]
135Kleckner IR, Zhang J, Touroutoglou A, Chanes L, Xia C, Simmons WK, et al. Evidence for a
large-scale brain system supporting allostasis and interoception in humans. Nat Hum Behav.
2017; 1:0069. [PubMed: 28983518]
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 July 20.
Khalsa et al. Page 19
136Anderson DJ, Adolphs R. A framework for studying emotions across species. Cell. 2014; 157:187–
200. [PubMed: 24679535]
Author Manuscript
137Kent ML, Tighe PJ, Belfer I, Brennan TJ, Bruehl S, Brummett CM, et al. The ACTTION-APS-
AAPM Pain Taxonomy (AAAPT) multidimensional approach to classifying acute pain
conditions. J Pain. 2017; 18:479–489. [PubMed: 28495013]
138Cameron OG. Interoception: The inside story—A model for psychosomatic processes. Psychosom
Med. 2001; 63:697–710. [PubMed: 11573016]
139Olausson H, Lamarre Y, Backlund H, Morin C, Wallin BG, Starck G, et al. Unmyelinated tactile
afferents signal touch and project to insular cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2002; 5:900–904. [PubMed:
12145636]
140Gorman JM, Kent J, Martinez J, Browne S, Coplan J, Papp LA. Physiological changes during
carbon dioxide inhalation in patients with panic disorder, major depression, and premenstrual
dysphoric disorder: Evidence for a central fear mechanism. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001; 58:125–
131. [PubMed: 11177114]
141Stein MB, Asmundson GJ. Autonomic function in panic disorder: Cardiorespiratory and plasma
catecholamine responsivity to multiple challenges of the autonomic nervous system. Biol
Author Manuscript
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 July 20.
Khalsa et al. Page 20
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Figure 1.
(A) Number of English language publications per year on interoception from PubMed,
PsycINFO, and Institute for Science Information Web of Knowledge. The timeline starts in
Author Manuscript
1905, one year before the publication of Charles Sherrington’s book, The Integrative Action
of the Nervous System, which first defined the concept of interoception. Key historical
events relevant to interoception science are superimposed. (B) Publications per year on
interoception vs. those investigating features of interoception that do not specifically refer to
the term. These latter publications are more numerous and arise mainly from basic
neuroscience, physiology, and subspecialty disciplines within the biomedical field. Note the
use of a logarithmic scale in the second panel. [Figure reproduced and modified with
permission from Khalsa and Lapidus (33).]
Author Manuscript
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 July 20.
Khalsa et al. Page 21
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Figure 2.
(A) Cardiac interoceptive processing measures and feature loading. This illustrates how the
most commonly used heartbeat perception tasks differentially measure interoceptive
features. [Figure reproduced and modified with permission from Khalsa and Lapidus (33).]
(B) Multisystem interoceptive processing measures and feature loadings: Example of
hypothetical approaches to measuring interoceptive processing across multiple systems.
Author Manuscript
Approaches that perturb the state of the body are recommended, as are convergent
approaches such as combined assessments of interoceptive attention and perturbation. (C)
Central neural integration of interoceptive rhythms. Interoceptive rhythms vary considerably
across the different systems of the body. They exhibit complex characteristics and are
hierarchically integrated within discrete regions of the central nervous system. [Figure
modified, with permission, from Petzschner et al. (37).] (D) Interoceptive rhythms vary in
both amplitude and frequency. The top trace illustrates a hypothetical example of an
amplitude modulation signal superimposed on a static frequency. The middle trace illustrates
a hypothetical frequency modulation signal superimposed on a static amplitude. The bottom
trace illustrates a hypothetical signal change involving both amplitude and frequency
modulations that are temporally correlated. GI, gastrointestinal; GU, genitourinary.
Author Manuscript
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 July 20.
Khalsa et al. Page 22
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Figure 3.
(A) Example of one possible form of a general inference–control loop illustrated within a
hierarchical Bayesian model. (B) Highly schematic example of illustrating that both
interoceptive information and exteroceptive information are concurrently integrated to
inform perceptual representations and action selection with respect to internally directed
(e.g., visceromotor, autonomic) and externally directed (e.g., skeletomotor) actions. (C)
General nodes that comprise a peripheral and central neural circuit for hierarchically
integrating afferent interoceptive information into homeostatic reflexes, sensory and meta-
cognitive representations, and allostatic regulators (predictions). ACC, anterior cingulate
cortex; AIC, anterior insular cortex; MC, metacognitive layer; MIC, midinsular cortex; OFC,
orbitofrontal cortex; PE, prediction error; PIC, posterior insular cortex; SGC, subgenual
cortex. [Panels (A) and (B) reproduced, with permission, from Petzschner et al. (37). Panel
Author Manuscript
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 July 20.
Khalsa et al. Page 23
Author Manuscript
Figure 4.
(A) Active inference implementation according to the Embodied Predictive Interoception
Coding model. Agranular visceromotor cortices, including the cingulate cortex, posterior
Author Manuscript
ventral medial prefrontal cortex, posterior orbitofrontal cortex, and ventral anterior insula,
estimate the balance among autonomic, metabolic, and immunological resources available to
the body and its predicted requirements. These agranular visceromotor cortices issue
allostatic predictions to hypothalamus, brainstem, and spinal cord nuclei to maintain a
homeostatic internal milieu and simultaneously to the primary interoceptive sensory cortex
in the mid and posterior insula. The interoceptive sensory cortex in the granular mid and
posterior insula sends reciprocal prediction error signals back to the agranular visceromotor
regions to modify the predictions. Under usual circumstances, these agranular regions are
relatively insensitive to such feedback, which explains why interoceptive predictions are
fairly stable in the face of body fluctuations. One hypothesis of the role of interoception in
mental illness is that interoceptive input (i.e., posteriors) becomes increasingly decoupled
from interoceptive predictions issued by the agranular visceromotor cortex (priors), leading
Author Manuscript
to increased interoceptive prediction error signals. This decoupling may present in the brain
as “noisy afferent interoceptive inputs” (97). (B) Proposed intracortical architecture and
intercortical connectivity for interoceptive predictive coding. The granular cortex contains
six cell layers including granule cells, which are excitatory neurons that amplify and
distribute thalamocortical inputs throughout the column. The granular cortex is structurally
similar to the neocortex and therefore more recently evolved than the agranular and
dysgranular cortices. Within the insula, the granular cortex is present in the mid and
posterior sectors. AC, anterior cingulate; PL, prelimbic cortex. [Figures reproduced, with
permission, from Barrett and Simmons (45).]
Author Manuscript
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 July 20.
Khalsa et al. Page 24
Table 1
Nonpainful
Cardiovascular, respiratory, gastrointestinal (esophageal, gastric, intestinal, colorectal), bladder, hunger, thirst, blood/serum (pH, osmolality,
glucose), temperature, vasomotor flush, air hunger, muscle tension, shudder, itch, tickle, genital sensation, sensual touch, fatigue
Painful
Visceral: kidney stone, pleuritic, angina, pericardial, bowel ischemia, pelvic, sickle crisis
Several key distinctions are that interoceptive sensing 1) may be painful or nonpainful, 2) occurs across the spectra of high/low arousal and
negative/positive valence, 3) usually occurs outside of conscious awareness (with the exception of pain sensations), and 4) is often (but not always)
consciously experienced during instances of homeostatic perturbation.
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 July 20.
Khalsa et al. Page 25
Table 2
Feature Definition
Attention Observing internal body sensations
Discrimination Localize sensation to a specific channel or organ system and differentiate it from other sensations
For some examples of paradigms assessing each feature, see Supplemental Table S1.
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Author Manuscript
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 July 20.
Khalsa et al. Page 26
Table 3
Diagnostic Symptoms and Clinical Signs Indicating Interoceptive Dysfunction in Some Psychiatric Disorders
Author Manuscript
Depression Increased or decreased appetite, fatigue, lethargy Weight gain, weight loss, (142,143)
psychomotor slowing
Eating Disorders Hunger insensitivity, food anxiety, gastrointestinal Severe food restriction, (72,98)
complaints severe weight loss, binging,
purging, compulsive exercise
Somatic Symptom Disorders Multiple current physical and nociceptive Symptoms Medical observations do not (144,145)
correspond with symptom
report
Substance Use Disorders Physical symptoms associated with craving, intoxication, Elevated/decreased: heart (101,146,147)
and/or withdrawal (drug specific) rate, respiratory rate, and/or
blood pressure, pupil dilation/
Author Manuscript
Generalized Anxiety Disorder Muscle tension, headaches, fatigue, Gastrointestinal Trembling, twitching, (149,150)
complaints, pain shaking, sweating, nausea,
exaggerated startle
Depersonalization/Derealization Disorder Detachment from one’s body, head fullness, tingling, Physiological hyporeactivity (151,152)
lightheadedness to emotional stimuli
Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2018 July 20.