A Behavior Modification Approach
A Behavior Modification Approach
A Behavior Modification Approach
net/publication/18685897
CITATIONS READS
57 2,961
3 authors, including:
Robert Koegel
Stanford University
218 PUBLICATIONS 13,703 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by Robert Koegel on 13 July 2015.
Laura Schreibman
Claremont Men's College
Robert L. Koegel
University o f California, Santa Barbara
~Presented at the First International Kanner Colloquium in Chapel Hill, North Carolina,
October 3 t - N o v e m b e r 2, 1973 and to appear in Child Development, Deviations, and
Treatment. Plenum Publishing Corporation (in press). Portions of this paper have previ-
ously appeared in Chapter VII o f the Seventy-second Yearbook of the National Society for
the Study of Education, Behavior Modification h~ Education, 1973.
2The research in our laboratory was sponsored by USPHS Research Grant No. 11440. from
the National Institute of Mental Health, and EHA Title VI-B, No. 42-00000-0000832/025,
from the California Department of Education to the Office o f the County Superintendent
o f Schools, Santa Barbara, California.
Requests for reprints should be sent to Dr. O. Ivax Lovaas, Department of Psychology,
University of California, Los Angeles, California 90024.
111
Q 1 9 7 4 P l e n u m PuDlisrling C o r p o r a t i o n , 2 2 7 West 1 7 t h S t r e e t , N e w Y O r k , N . Y . 1 0 0 1 1 . NO
p a r t o f t h i s p u b l i c a t i o n m a y be r e p r o d u c e d , s t o r e d in a r e t r i e v a l s y s t e m , o r t r a n s m i t t e d , in
a n y f o r m , ; o r bAyllfany, fm~ainS~neleCtrmOn~C~nmn~h~2~akl p h o t o c o p y i n c j , m i c r o f i l m i n g . . . . . . ding,
112 Lovaas, Schreibman, and Koegel
RECENT DEVELOPMENTS
One of the most bizarre and most profoundly sick behaviors which one
will ever encounter is self-mutilation, which is characteristic of so many psychot-
ic and retarded children. One can see children who tear with their teeth large
amounts of tissue from their shoulders and arms, who chew off part of their
fingers, hit their heads so violently against the wall that they detach their retinas,
and accidentally would kill themselves unless they were somehow sedated or
restrained. Many of these children, although they may be only eight or nine
years old, have spent most of their lives in restraints, tied down both by their
feet and arms.
Irrational as these behaviors may appear, the studies which were con-
ducted to better understand these behaviors show self-mutilation to be a very
"lawful," understandable phenomenon. Rather than the self-destruction being
an expression of some tenuous internal state, such as a shattered, guilty, worth-
less self, we found it to be rather straightforward learned behavior. That is, we
first attempted to treat self-destruction using the treatment procedure most
often prescribed at the time. This treatment, based on the psychodynamic model
of psychopathology, suggested delivering affection, understanding, and sympa-
thetic comments to reassure the child of his self-worth, etc. We carefully mea-
A Behavior Modification Appzoach 117
I / m
j ~
4j
r
uJ
o3
z
5 -~ 2 I/2 MIN
sured the amount of self-destructive behavior emitted by the child and found
that when we gave sympathetic comments contingent upon the child hurting
himself, his self-destruction increased in strength, only to return to baseline
when these comments were no longer administered. Figure 1 presents the data
for one child, Gregory, indicating the change in his self-abusive behavior as a
function of the attention he received for such behavior. His data are presented as
cumulative curves. This means that if he did not hit himself, the line is flat
(i.e., horizontal); and each time he injured himself, it is recorded on a pen that
makes one small step upward for each response. The steeper the curve, the more
hits. Notice that we have in sessions 1 and 2, baseline data that tell us that his
118 Lovaas, Schreibman, and Koegei
self-destructive behavior is very low; the curves are almost flat. In session 3, we
started our treatment with him, expressing our concern and affection for him
when he hit himself. Note also that each time we treated him in this manner the
pen on the graph gave a signal by an upward-moving stroke or hatchmark. In
session 3, he showed a marked increase in the rate of self-destruction when we
treated him like that. In session 4, we removed the treatment and the curve
flattened out; he got better. In session 5 we reintroduced the treatment and he
hit himself some 200 times in the short period of 10 minutes. When we again
removed the treatment (sessions 6 and 7) we recovered his baseline. This, then, is
an ABABA design. Incidentally, it would have been quite possible for us to have
killed Greg through this treatment. All we needed to do was to thin out the
reinforcement schedule a bit (i.e., follow self-destructive acts by sympathetic
comments less than 100 percent of the time) and to have given love contingent
on more vicious blows, closer to the eyes or other vulnerable spots. In other
words, it looked like the self-mutilation was reinforceable, like operant behavior.
It is noteworthy that this is the first instance reported in the literature where
objective measures were taken to carefully assess the effects of treatment for
self-destruction. Clearly the emphasis on a research framework has enabled us to
see quite vividly the dangerous prospects of this form of treatment.
We hypothesized that if self-destruction were reinforceable, as operant
behavior is, then it should e x t i n ~ i s h if we made certain that no social conse-
quences were administered contingent upon its occurrence. This is exactly what
happened. When the children were left free to injure themselves without parents
or nursing staff intervening, the self-destructive behavior fell off in a very gradual
but lawful manner from, for example, a high of some 4000 self-destructive acts
in the first hour to 3500 the second hour, then 300, and then slowly to zero by
the tenth session. What most convinced us that self-mutilation was in fact
operant behavior pertained to its highly discriminated nature. For example, the
child would not hurt himself in a room where he had been "run to extinction"
but would resume self-destruction at full strength in another situation only feet
and seconds away if he had been comforted for self-destructive behavior there.
Clinically speaking, the children did not waste any blows unless there was a
payoff for it, and our data showed that they were incredibly discriminating as to
which situations paid off. Technically speaking we say that self-mutilation was
under the control of discriminative stimuli (SD).
Although extinction may work to reduce self-destruction, there are serious
limitations to extinction procedure since some of the children hurt themselves
severely during extinction. Clearly, the child who hits himself thousands of times
in an hour's time, or a child who tears flesh off his shoulder, is in danger, and
one takes too many chances letting such a child undergo extinction runs as we
have described them. Therefore, we tried out ways of delivering aversive stimuli
(such as painful electric shock) contingent upon self-destruction, in the hope of
stopping it more quickly. The data we reported (Lovaas & Simmons, 1969)
A Behavior Modification Approach 119
SelfiStimulatory Behavior
By far the most common form of behavior exhibited by psychotic and
retarded children centers on the self-stimulatory behavior we mentioned earlier.
This includes a great deal of spinning, twirling, rocking, and gazing as well as
other very repetitive and stereotyped movements which seem to have no partic-
ular relationship to what is happening in the child's day-to-day life.
Our understanding of this behavior class is very limited. From clinical
observation, and not so much based on systematic research, it appears that this
behavior disappears or is supplanted when more nonnal behaviors are acquired.
We do know also that the presence of certain forms of self-stimulation makes the
child more difficult to teach, apparently because he is less attentive to external
cues. For example, in a study by Lovaas, Litrownik, and Mann (1971) autistic
children were taught to respond to an auditory input in order to obtain candies
and other sweets. If the child was presented with this auditory stinmlus when he
was involved in self-stimulatory behavior, then frequently he would fail to
respond to the auditory input, either completely missing the opportunity to
obtain his reinforcer or responding after some delay. In another study by Koegel
and Covert (1972) autistic children were taught a very simple discrimination task
such as responding on a machine (depressing a lever) in the presence of a light
stimulus. They observed that the children failed to acquire this very simple
discrinlination as long as they were allowed to self-stimulate. When given disap-
proval (either verbal or with a slap on the hand) resulting in the suppression of
120 Lovaas, Schreibman, and Koegel
self-stimulation, the children did learn. It appears, then, that the self-stimulatory
behavior may interfere with one's efforts to teach these children; for the sake of
helping them acquire new behaviors one may initially decrease self-stimulatory
behavior.
Recent research in our lab, largely under the direction of Arnold Rincover
and Crighton Newsom, has proceeded on the notion that self-stimulatory
behavior is operant behavior and that it is different from other kinds of operants
in the sense that the reinforcement for such behavior consists of the sensory
feedback (kinesthetic, visual, auditory, etc.) which the behavior itself produces.
In that sense it is the child himself who controls that reinforcement, hence
shaping his own behavior. Given the extensive and durable self-stimulatory reper-
toires of autistic children, we suspect that these self-stimulatory reinforcers must
be very strong, and we are exploring ways of using self-stimulatory reinforcers
instead of food, etc., to build appropriate behaviors. So far research shows
self-stimulatory reinforcers to be much less satiable than the primary, appetitive
ones.
Since these children have such limited repertoires and since many are
essentially without any social or intellectual behavior, they present a great
challenge to educators. In a sense, one has the opportunity to start building a
person from the beginning in regard to social and emotional development as well
as intellectual development. In the short amount of space allotted here we can
only point to certain examples to illustrate the techniques which have been
employed in our attempts to build new behaviors.
Simultaneously with the suppression of self-stimulatory behaviors, the
teacher generally begins her work by attempting to establish some early forms of
stimulus control. The teacher may request of the child some simple behavior
such as sitting quietly in a chair. Since even such a minimal request often evokes
tantrums of self-destructive behavior, the establishment of this basic stimulus
control and the reduction of aggression and self-destructive behaviors generally
proceed together. It is generally impossible to work on the acquisition of
appropriate behaviors until one has achieved some reduction of the pathological
behaviors.
When one attempts to educate children who are so deficient in behavioral
development, theoretically one has two alternate paths to follow. For example,
instead of building behaviors piece by piece, one could decide to teach a child to
value interpersonal interactions, intellectual achievement, curiosity about the
world, and so on. Technically speaking, one could attempt to place these chil-
dren in a situation where one optimized the child's motivational system to help
them acquire conditioned reinforcers. We have pointed out before (Lovaas,
A Behavior Modification Approach 121
Freitag, Kinder, Ruberstein, Schaeffer, & Simmons, 1966) that in many ways
this would be an optimal strategy because the child, being properly motivated,
would learn much without explicit attempts to teach him. To illustrate this
point, if a child were strongly reinforced by a large range of social reinforcers, he
would probably learn to behave in social ways, that is, to talk, play with peers,
and so on, so as to come in contact with these stimulus events. It is unfortunate
that this alternate path to teaching is not open to us since we do not know how
to establish conditioned reinforcers or to build motivation in autistic children.
No doubt one of the great challenges in the years ahead will be to achieve a
better understanding of how motivation is acquired.
Given this limitation on the child's motivational system, the alternative is
to proceed to build behaviors with the reinforcers which are functional. One can
always fall back upon basic reinforcers which are functional. One can always fall
back upon basic reinforcers such as food and pain, and as one works with any
one child, one discovers idiosyncracies in his motivational structure, such as a
particular liking for a certain piece of music, a toy, etc., which can be parti-
tioned in various ways and delivered to the child contingent upon certain desir-
able behaviors. The acquisition of new behaviors is accomplished in a step-by-
step program. Let us turn to eftbrts to build language to ilhistrate how these
programs have worked out.
Building Language
Using a shaping procedure, several investigators (Hewett, 1965; Lovaas,
1966) have provided procedures for developing speech in previously mute
autistic children. The procedure relied heavily on developing imitative speech.
Let us illustrate the procedures from our language program. This program has
been presented on f'dm (Lovaas, 1969) and will appear in a book (Lovaas, in
press). Briefly described, the verbal imitation training involves four steps of
successive discriminations. In Step l the therapist increases the child's vocaliza-
tions by reinforcing him (usually with food) for such behavior. In Step 2 the
child's vocalizations are reinforced only if they are in response to the therapist's
speech (e.g., if they occur within five seconds of the therapist's speech) until he
can match the particular sound given by the therapist (e.g., "a"). In Step 4 the
therapist replicates Step 3 with another sound (e.g., "m"), demanding increas-
ingly f'me discriminations and reproduction from the child. In this manner,
starting with sounds which are discriminably different (such as " m " and "a"),
the child is taught to imitate an increasingly large range of sounds, words, and
sentences. Imitation, then, is a discrimination where the response resembles its
stimulus. Once he can imitate, the previously mute child becomes similar to
echolalic autistic children; they both imitate the speech of others, but neither
knows the meaning of the words he utters. Their speech exists without a con-
text.
122 Lovaas, Scltreibman, and Koegel
We now have some data which provide an estimation of the changes one
might expect in autistic children undergoing behavior therapy (Lovaas, Koegel,
Simmons, & Long, 1973). We examined three measures of the generality of
treatment effects: (a) stimulus generalization, the extent to which behavior
changes that occur in the treatment environment transfer to situations outside
that treatment; (b) response generalization, the extent to which changes in a
limited set of behaviors effect changes in a larger range of behaviors; and (c)
generalization over time (or durability), that is, how well the therapeutic effects
maintain themselves over time.
A Behavior Modification Approach 123
60
~_~stitution
50 I
I
L~J
l
I
(.~
!
zu J 40 I
OE i
n,-
0
30 l
i
I--- I
Z
~ 2o ,' ~)
13..
10 ~) "k~arents
I I I I I I l t I I I I L J J
B A F B A f B A F B A F B A F
Self Echololio Appropriate Social Appropriate
Stimulation Verbal Nonverbal PIo y
Fig. 2. Multiple response follow-up measures. Percent occurrence of the various behaviors is
plotted on the ordinate for Before (B) and After (A) treatment, and for the latest follow-up
(F) measures. 1 refers to the average results for the four children who were institutionalized,
and P refers to the average results for the nine children who were discharged to their
parents' care.
124 Lovaas, Sehreibman, and Koegel
to the six children who lived with their parents after their discharge from treat-
ment. For all the five behaviors the trends are the same: the children who were
discharged to a state hospital lost what they had gained in treatment with us: they
increased in their psychotic behavior (self-stimulation and echolalia), they appear
to have lost all they had gained of social nonverbal behavior, and they lost much
of their gains in the area of appropriate verbal and appropriate play. The children
who stayed with their parents, on the other hand, maintained their gains or im-
proved further. For the children who regressed in the state hospital, a brief re-
instatement of behavior therapy could temporarily reestablish the original
therapeutic gains.
Since we assessed these behaviors in environments other than that of the
treatment, we know that our procedures produced stimulus generalization. The
children's Stanford-Binet IQ scores and Vineland Social Quotient scores also
showed large gains during the course of treatment, and since we did not train the
children on these tests, they provide explicit measures of response generaliza-
tion.
These findings clearly emphasize an important point underlying the use of
principles of behavior modification. That is, there may be important differences
in the procedures for the production and maintenance of behavior. Thus, it does
not appear to be enough to help the child acquire appropriate behaviors and to
overcome the inappropriate ones; it is also important to provide maintaining
conditions which ensure that the improvements will last. Our follow-up paper
(Lovaas et al., 1973) discusses several of the strengths and weaknesses of a
behavior modification approach to the treatment of autistic children.
CONTEMPORARY RESEARCH
CLASSROOM TREATMENT
Little if any systematic research has been carried out on the education of
autistic children in classroom settings. Research to date has focused primarily on
the treatment of autistic children in a one-to-one teacher-child ratio. Economi-
cally, this makes the treatment unfeasible in many hospital and school situa-
tions. Therefore, we recently began a research program concerned with the
teaching of autistic children in groups (Koegel & Rincover, 1974). Let us illus-
trate this research by citing a study where the emphasis is on establishing a
teacher as a discriminative stimulus (SD) for appropriate behavior even though
the child is in a large group of children. To achieve this end we began working
with two children, using one teacher and two teacher aides. The teacher provides
commands and instructions and the aides deliver contingent rewards and punish-
A Behavior Modification Approach 127
ments. As the children become more and more proficient, reinforcement for
appropriate behaviors becomes increasingly intermittent and additional children
are introduced into the group, one at a time. If behaviors deteriorate at any one
time, the size of the class is immediately reduced, reinforcements become more
dense, and shortly thereafter we begin building again. Through a process like
this, then, it is hoped that the autistic child will be able to behave appropriately
in a more average (normal) classroom.
NEW DIRECTIONS
SUMMARY COMMENT
We know more about some behaviors than others. For example, we know
more about self-destructive behavior than self-stimulation. We were of more help
128 Lovaas, Schreibman, and Koegel
to some children than to others. If the child possessed a verbal topography at the
beginning o f treatment, even if it was socially nonfunctional (such as echolalia),
then we could help him substantially in developing a meaningful language. But
we were less proficient in establishing new behavioral topographies in autistic
children when none existed. For example, if the child was mute, his progress
with behavior modification procedures was very limited. Follow-up data showed
that in order to maintain the gains which the child made in treatment, he had to
remain within an extension o f the therapeutic environment. For example, for
the c l ~ d to continue showing improvement, his parents had to be taught behav-
ior modification principles. The delivery of contingent, functional reinforcers
has repeatedly been demonstrated to be a most significant feature b o t h in
providing for learning and in maintaining the behavioral gains.
As we worked with these children, it became more apparent that they
possessed certain deviations in perceptual functioning, particularly in responding
to multiple stimulus inputs, which necessitate revisions o f the usual manner o f
presenting educational m a t e r i a l Only additional research will enable us to struc-
ture such optimal learning environments.
The f r s t 12 years in behavior m o d i f i c a t i o n relied heavily on learning
principles derived from the animal laboratory and their ready application re-
sulted in rapid progress. It is likely that the next ten years will demand consider-
able new research directed specifically to the autistic child in order to help him
develop appreciably further.
REFERENCES