Simonton2012 - Teaching Creativity
Simonton2012 - Teaching Creativity
Simonton2012 - Teaching Creativity
Teaching of Psychology
39(3) 217-222
Teaching Creativity: Current Findings, ª The Author(s) 2012
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
Trends, and Controversies in the DOI: 10.1177/0098628312450444
http://top.sagepub.com
Psychology of Creativity
Abstract
In the past decade, the psychological study of creativity has accelerated greatly. To facilitate the teaching of creativity, I provide an
overview of the recent literature. The overview begins by discussing recent empirical results and research trends. This discussion
specifically treats creativity’s cognitive, differential, developmental, and social aspects. Then I outline central controversies in the
study of creativity. These debates concern the nature of creative thought (domain-specific vs. generic processes), creative devel-
opment (nature vs. nurture), and creative persons (psychopathology vs. mental health). The article closes by asking not just how
to teach creativity but also how to teach creativity creatively.
Keywords
creativity, intelligence
On the surface, creativity should inspire abundant research. After categories: cognitive, differential, developmental, and social
all, creativity concerns the psychological phenomenon where psychological.
someone comes up with an idea or product that is simultaneously
novel and useful, two positive and important attributes
Cognitive Psychology of Creativity
(Simonton, 2000a). Nonetheless, not so long ago, creativity was
a relatively neglected topic in psychology (Sternberg & Lubart, Naturally, cognitive psychologists are most interested in the
1996). That neglect is definitely not true today. Over the past mental processes or mechanisms involved in creative thought
decade, the topic has captured increased attention at an acceler- (Simonton & Damian, in press). An especially intriguing find-
ating pace (Hennessey & Amabile, 2010). Besides an increase in ing is that creativity is strongly associated with ‘‘defocused
the number of journals devoted to the topic—including the attention,’’ ‘‘cognitive disinhibition,’’ or what researchers tech-
American Psychological Association’s own Psychology of nically refer to as reduced latent inhibition (Carson, Peterson,
Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, which was first published & Higgins, 2003; Kéri, 2011). Highly creative people tend to
in 2006—the growth of interest is evident in the appearance of notice stimuli that they know are irrelevant, which enables
two recent annual review of psychology articles (Hennessey & them to ‘‘think outside the box’’ and to take advantage of ser-
Amabile, 2010; Runco, 2004), several comprehensive hand- endipitous events that they might otherwise ignore. A downside
books (e.g., Kaufman & Sternberg, 2006, 2009; Richards, to this cognitive propensity is that reduced latent inhibition is
Runco, & Moger, 2009), numerous introductory textbooks also associated with psychopathology (Carson, 2011; Eysenck,
(Kaufman, 2009; Runco, 2007; Sawyer, 2006; Weisberg, 1995). The psychotic mind is incessantly bombarded by stimuli
2006), and even the second edition of a 2-volume encyclopedia and associations that should be filtered out from the get go.
(Runco & Pritzker, 2011). Indeed, the breadth and depth of con- With the advent of the cognitive neurosciences, it comes as
temporary research are almost overwhelming. Because research no surprise that researchers have studied creative problem sol-
on creativity spans several of psychology’s subdisciplines, it is ving and insight using the latest techniques (Dietrich & Kanso,
not easy for any one researcher to keep up on advances in the 2010; Sawyer, 2011). Although this research has arrived at
field. Even so, I will do my best to review current findings and some potentially valuable findings, the results at present do not
trends and then turn to some contemporary controversies. I close
by discussing the teaching of creativity. 1
Department of Psychology, University of California, Davis,CA, USA
how untalented, will still be able to achieve at the highest lev- more prominent in Big-C Creativity and less conspicuous in
els. In short, inherited abilities or dispositions are irrelevant little-c creativity (Simonton, 2010). The two opposing sides
(Howe, 1999). Creators are just experts. of the debate are both right. One side is just talking about the
Although research indicates that psychologists often have a elephant’s head, while the other side is talking about the
higher citation impact if they adopt such extreme positions elephant’s tail. Finally, the two opposing sides agree on one
(Simonton, 2000b), there is no evidence that such extremism critical point: Even Big-C creators do not produce genius-
contributes to the advancement of psychological science. level work when they are outright insane. Instead, the psycho-
Research in behavioral genetics has provided strong empirical pathology observed stays at subclinical levels. For instance,
and theoretical reasons for believing that nature does indeed highly creative novelists score between the mentally ill and the
play a role in the emergence of creative talent (Simonton, normal on the clinical scales of the Minnesota Multiphasic
2008b). This is not to say that researchers have identified a sin- Personality Inventory (Barron, 1963; cf. Nettle, 2006).
gle gene for creativity. On the contrary, creativity depends on One final note, behavior genetics can help resolve both the
the participation of numerous cognitive and dispositional vari- nature–nurture and creativity–psychopathology controversies.
ables that also feature sizable heritability coefficients—variables Researchers have long known that highly creative people are
such as general intelligence and openness to experience. In addi- more likely to come from families that suffer higher than aver-
tion, the genetic influences operate in complex (multiplicative) age psychopathology (e.g., Karlsson, 1970). In fact, a recent
ways that unfold slowly over the course of childhood and adoles- study identified a unique gene that is found in both the highly
cence (Simonton, 1999). Finally, genetic factors do not render creative and the mentally ill (Kéri, 2009). As noted above, the
irrelevant the acquisition of domain-specific expertise through gene simply has to be inherited along with other traits that turn
deliberate practice. Instead, talent and training operate in tandem a potential cognitive liability into a decided cognitive asset.
(Simonton, 2008b). Innate talent speeds up expertise acquisition
(becoming better faster) and allows superior performance for a
given level of expertise acquisition (more bang for the buck).
Teaching Creativity
In brief, nature enhances nurture.
In some respects, teaching creativity is no different than teach-
ing any other subject in psychology. Hence, to a certain extent,
Creative Persons: Psychopathology Versus Mental
I teach creativity the same way I teach other undergraduate
Health? courses, such as my history of psychology course. Thus, in all
The ancient question of whether genius is ‘‘born or made’’ is of my classes, I use various devices to keep my students
complemented by the even older question of the ‘‘mad gen- interested even when I must engage in the traditional one-
ius’’—an issue that goes back to Plato and Aristotle. Stated way communication of information. For example, I always
in more inclusive terms, is creativity associated with psycho- wear a T-shirt with some picture or quotation that illustrates
pathology in such a way that the higher the magnitude of crea- a point somewhere in the lecture. But students cannot anticipate
tivity, the greater the frequency and intensity of adverse when the T-shirt will prove relevant, or even why. They have to
mental, emotional, or behavioral symptoms? The polarization pay attention!
on this controversy is every bit as fierce as the nature–nurture Nevertheless, one might think that a course, lecture, or sec-
issue. On one hand, a long tradition going back to early psy- tion on creativity would require something more. In other
chiatry and psychoanalysis has defended a positive connection words, one should not just teach creativity but also teach crea-
between psychopathology and creativity. On the other hand, a tivity creatively! That implies that instructional techniques
more recent tradition, starting with humanistic psychology and should be both novel and useful—a far more difficult goal to
continuing with the positive psychology movement, argues that achieve. Even after teaching a creativity class for more than
creativity is a sure sign of self-actualization and subjective two decades, I remain far from reaching that goal. Still, I have
well-being. found one method that seems to work very well—a ‘‘lecture’’
The resolution to this debate depends on addressing two fun- on creativity tests. Research on creativity tests has not reached
damental questions. First, in what domain is the creativity a consensus on the single best test for assessing creativity. For
going to take place? As noted earlier, psychopathology is more instance, some tests gauge requisite cognitive skills, such as
common in the arts than in the sciences. Even with a particular divergent thinking or remote association (Guilford, 1967; Med-
art or science, the rate and intensity of psychopathological nick, 1962); other instruments assess whether a person has a
symptoms can vary. Thus, mental illness is higher in poetry creative personality (Gough, 1979). Rather than describe each
than in any other artistic domain (Ludwig, 1992). Second, what measure in detail and review the psychometric literature on
degree of creativity are we talking about? Creativity research- each test’s reliability and validity, I project items from repre-
ers frequently make the distinction between ‘‘Big-C Creativ- sentative tests on a screen, and everybody, including me, takes
ity’’ and ‘‘little-c creativity’’ (Simonton, 2000a). The former the test together. Doing so requires us, for example, to come up
appellation applies to creative genius, whereas the latter ascrip- with unusual uses for a brick or paper clip (Guilford, 1967). I
tion applies to everyday creativity witnessed at home and in the then introduce the measurement issues in the context of a direct
workplace. This contrast is crucial because psychopathology is experience that we all shared.
Conclusion Dijksterhuis, A., & Meurs, T. (2006). Where creativity resides: The
generative power of unconscious thought. Consciousness and
I have provided an overview of what researchers know about Cognition, 15, 135-146.
creativity, with a focus on the most recent results, research Ericsson, K. A. (1996). The acquisition of expert performance: An
trends, and ongoing debates. To a more limited extent, I also introduction to some of the issues. In K. A. Ericsson (Ed.), The
offered some tips on how to teach creativity, including how road to expert performance: Empirical evidence from the arts and
to teach creativity creatively. It is my hope that this article sciences, sports, and games (pp. 1-50). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
inspires some instructors, at whatever level, to connect their Eysenck, H. J. (1995). Genius: The natural history of creativity.
students to this significant topic. Creativity is not only an Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
intellectually interesting subject, but also a phenomenon of Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic
immense practical importance. I personally believe that no creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 290-309.
student should receive a college degree without knowing some- Friedman, R., & Förster, J. (2001). The effects of promotion and
thing about creativity or without learning how to be creative. prevention cues on creativity. Journal of Personality and Social
When teaching about creativity, teachers must try to accom- Psychology, 81, 1001-1013.
plish both of these tasks. Galinsky, A., Gruenfeld, D., Magee, J., Whitson, J., & Liljenquist, K.
(2008). Power reduces the press of the situation: Implications for
Declaration of Conflicting Interests creativity, conformity and dissonance. Journal of Personality and
The author declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to Social Psychology, 95, 1450-1466.
the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Galton, F. (1874). English men of science: Their nature and nurture.
London: Macmillan.
Funding Gough, H. G. (1979). A Creative Personality Scale for the Adjective
The author received no financial support for the research, authorship, Check List. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37,
and/or publication of this article. 1398-1405.
Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York:
References McGraw-Hill.
Akinola, M., & Mendes, W. (2008). The dark side of creativity: Harris, J. A. (2004). Measured intelligence, achievement, openness to
Biological vulnerability and negative emotions lead to greater experience, and creativity. Personality and Individual Differences,
artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 913-929.
34, 1677-1686. Hayes, J. R. (1989). The complete problem solver (2nd ed.). Hillsdale,
Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context: Update to the social NJ: Erlbaum.
psychology of creativity. Boulder, CO: Westview. Hennessey, B. A., & Amabile, T. M. (2010). Creativity. Annual
Baas, M., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). A meta-analysis Review of Psychology, 61, 569-598.
of 25 years of mood-creativity research: Hedonic tone, activation, or Howe, M. J. A. (1999). Genius explained. Cambridge, England:
regulatory focus? Psychological Bulletin, 134, 779-806. Cambridge University Press.
Baer, J. (2011). Why grand theories of creativity distort, distract, and Karlsson, J. I. (1970). Genetic association of giftedness and creativity
disappoint. The International Journal of Creativity & Problem with schizophrenia. Hereditas, 66, 177-182.
Solving, 21, 73-100. Kaufman, J. C. (2009). Creativity 101. New York, NY: Springer.
Barron, F. X. (1963). Creativity and psychological health: Origins of Kaufman, J. C., & Baer, J. (2002). Could Steven Spielberg manage the
personal vitality and creative freedom. Princeton, NJ: Van Yankees? Creative thinking in different domains. Korean Journal
Nostrand. of Thinking & Problem Solving, 12, 5-14.
Campbell, D. T. (1960). Blind variation and selective retention in Kaufman, J. C., & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.). (2006). International hand-
creative thought as in other knowledge processes. Psychological book of creativity research. New York, NY: Cambridge University
Review, 67, 380-400. Press.
Carson, S. H. (2011). Creativity and psychopathology: A shared Kaufman, J. C., & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.). (2009). Cambridge hand-
vulnerability model. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 56, book of creativity. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
144-153. Kéri, S. (2009). Genes for psychosis and creativity: A promoter
Carson, S., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2003). Decreased latent polymorphism of the neuregulin 1 gene is related to creativity in
inhibition is associated with increased creative achievement in people with high intellectual achievement. Psychological Science,
high-functioning individuals. Journal of Personality and Social 20, 1070-1073.
Psychology, 85, 499-506. Kéri, S. (2011). Solitary minds and social capital: Latent inhibition,
Carson, S., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2005). Reliability, valid- general intellectual functions and social network size predict crea-
ity, and factor structure of the Creative Achievement Question- tive achievements. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the
naire. Creativity Research Journal, 17, 37-50. Arts, 5, 215-221. doi:10.1037/a0022000
Dietrich, A., & Kanso, R. (2010). A review of EEG, ERP, and neuroi- Leung, A. K., Maddux, W. W., Galinsky, A. D., & Chiu, C. (2008).
maging studies of creativity and insight. Psychological Bulletin, Multicultural experience enhances creativity: The when and how.
136, 822-848. American Psychologist, 63, 169-181.
Ludwig, A. M. (1992). Creative achievement and psychopathology: Simonton, D. K. (2000b). Methodological and theoretical orientation
Comparison among professions. American Journal of Psychother- and the long-term disciplinary impact of 54 eminent psychologists.
apy, 46, 330-356. Review of General Psychology, 4, 13-24.
McCrae, R. R. (1987). Creativity, divergent thinking, and openness to Simonton, D. K. (2003a). Creative cultures, nations, and civilizations:
experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, Strategies and results. In P. B. Paulus & B. A. Nijstad (Eds.),
1258-1265. Group creativity: Innovation through collaboration (pp.
Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. 304-328). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Psychological Review, 69, 220-232. Simonton, D. K. (2003b). Scientific creativity as constrained stochas-
Nemeth, C. J., & Nemeth-Brown, B. (2003). Better than individuals? tic behavior: The integration of product, process, and person
The potential benefits of dissent and diversity. In P. B. Paulus & B. perspectives. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 475-494.
A. Nijstad (Eds.), Group creativity: Innovation through collabora- Simonton, D. K. (2007). Creativity: Specialized expertise or general
tion (pp. 63-84). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. cognitive processes? In M. J. Roberts (Ed.), Integrating the mind:
Nettle, D. (2006). Schizotypy and mental health amongst poets, visual Domain general versus domain specific processes in higher cogni-
artists, and mathematicians. Journal of Research in Personality, tion (pp. 351-367). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
40, 876-890. Simonton, D. K. (2008a). Bilingualism and creativity. In J. Altarriba
Page, S. E. (2007). Difference: How the power of diversity creates bet- & R. R. Heredia (Eds.), An introduction to bilingualism: Principles
ter groups, firms, schools, and societies. Princeton, NJ: Princeton and processes (pp. 147-166). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
University Press. Simonton, D. K. (2008b). Scientific talent, training, and performance:
Paulus, P. B., & Nijstad, B. A. (Eds.). (2003). Group creativity: Inno- Intellect, personality, and genetic endowment. Review of General
vation through collaboration. New York, NY: Oxford University Psychology, 12, 28-46.
Press. Simonton, D. K. (2009). Varieties of (scientific) creativity: A hier-
Peterson, J. B., & Carson, S. (2000). Latent inhibition and openness to archical model of disposition, development, and achievement. Per-
experience in a high-achieving student population. Personality and spectives on Psychological Science, 4, 441-452.
Individual Differences, 28, 323-332. Simonton, D. K. (2010). So you want to become a creative genius?
Peterson, J. B., Smith, K. W., & Carson, S. (2002). Openness and You must be crazy! In D. Cropley, J. Kaufman, A. Cropley, &
extraversion are associated with reduced latent inhibition: Replica- M. Runco (Eds.), The dark side of creativity (pp. 218-234). New
tion and commentary. Personality and Individual Differences, 33, York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
1137-1147. Simonton, D. K. (2011a). Creativity and discovery as blind variation:
Richards, T., Runco, M., & Moger, S. (Eds.). (2009). Routledge com- Campbell’s (1960) BVSR model after the half-century mark.
panion to creativity. London, England: Taylor & Francis. Review of General Psychology, 15, 158-174.
Runco, M. (2004). Creativity. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, Simonton, D. K. (2011b). Great flicks: Scientific studies of cinematic
657-687. creativity and aesthetics. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Runco, M. (2007). Creativity, theories, and themes: Research, devel- Simonton, D. K. (2012). Creative productivity and aging: An age
opment, and practice. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier. decrement - or not? In S. K. Whitbourne & M. Sliwinski (Eds.),
Runco, M. A., & Pritzker, S. (Eds.). (2011). Encyclopedia of creativity The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of adult development and aging
(2nd ed.). Oxford, England: Elsevier. (pp. 477-496). New York: Wiley-Blackwell.
Sawyer, R. K. (2006). Explaining creativity: The science of human Simonton, D. K., & Damian, R. I. (in press). Creativity. In D. Reisberg
innovation. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. (Ed.), Oxford handbook of cognitive psychology. New York, NY:
Sawyer, R. K. (2007). Group genius: The creative power of collabora- Oxford University Press.
tion. New York, NY: Basic Books. Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1996). Investing in creativity.
Sawyer, R. K. (2011). The cognitive neuroscience of creativity: A critical American Psychologist, 51, 677-688.
review. Creativity Research Journal, 23, 137-154. Stroebe, W. (2010). The graying of academia: Will it reduce scientific
Simonton, D. K. (1987). Developmental antecedents of achieved productivity? American Psychologist, 65, 660-673.
eminence. Annals of Child Development, 5, 131-169. Stroebe, W., Nijstad, B. A., & Rietzschel, E. F. (2010). Beyond pro-
Simonton, D. K. (1997). Creative productivity: A predictive and ductivity loss in brainstorming groups: The evolution of a question.
explanatory model of career trajectories and landmarks. Psycholo- Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 157-203.
gical Review, 104, 66-89. Wallas, G. (1926). The art of thought. New York, NY: Harcourt,
Simonton, D. K. (1999). Talent and its development: An emergenic Brace.
and epigenetic model. Psychological Review, 106, 435-457. Weisberg, R. W. (2006). Creativity: Understanding innovation in
Simonton, D. K. (2000a). Creativity: Cognitive, developmental, problem solving, science, invention, and the arts. Hoboken, NJ:
personal, and social aspects. American Psychologist, 55, 151-158. Wiley.