Escalasbettmanjcr PDF
Escalasbettmanjcr PDF
Escalasbettmanjcr PDF
net/publication/23547268
CITATIONS READS
1,560 7,576
2 authors:
All content following this page was uploaded by James R Bettman on 17 October 2017.
themselves through their brand choices based on congruency the outgroup symbolism in constructing one’s possible self.
between brand image and self-image. Thus, the meaning For example, if I am not a member of a fraternity (and do
and value of a brand is not just its ability to express the not desire to be a member) and see fraternity members wear-
self, but also its role in helping consumers create and build ing Polo clothing, I may specifically choose not to wear
their self-identities (McCracken 1989). Polo clothing in an attempt to distance myself from the
Reference groups can be a critical source of brand mean- fraternity symbolism of the Polo brand. Thus, the type of
ings. Consumers use others as a source of information for group associated with the brand (ingroup versus outgroup)
arriving at and evaluating one’s beliefs about the world, will moderate the effect of brand associations on self-brand
particularly others who share beliefs and are similar on rel- connections.
evant dimensions. Consumer research on reference groups Similarly, if a brand is not typically associated with an
has demonstrated congruency between group membership ingroup (e.g., its image is incongruent with the group), this
and brand usage (e.g., Bearden and Etzel 1982; Bearden, may negatively affect self-brand connections. The same
Netemeyer, and Teel 1989; Burnkrant and Cousineau 1975; identification processes that lead to a connection with a
Childers and Rao 1992; Moschis 1985) and has defined brand associated with an ingroup lead to rejection of a brand
several types of social influence (e.g., Bearden and Etzel with an image incongruent with the ingroup. However, if a
1982; Park and Lessig 1977). We elaborate on value- brand’s image does not match an outgroup, the prediction
expressive reference group influences, characterized by the is not quite as clear. The lack of match may actually be
need for psychological association with a group either to viewed favorably, based on balance-theory considerations
resemble the group or due to a liking for the group. We (Heider 1946), thus leading to enhanced self-brand connec-
provide an empirical demonstration of these ideas by dem- tions. Alternatively, the lack of match may simply be viewed
onstrating that brand use by reference groups is a source of as irrelevant, leading to no effect on self-brand connections.
brand meaning. Consumers form associations between ref- We hypothesize that the balance-theory view will be more
erence groups and the brands they use and transfer these descriptive and that brand associations that are incongruent
meanings from brand to self by selecting brands with mean- with an outgroup will have a favorable impact on self-brand
ings relevant to an aspect of their current self-concept or connections. In sum, we have:
possible self.
H1a: Brand associations consistent with an ingroup
A critical distinction in terms of such self-construction will have a favorable effect on self-brand con-
processes is that between the use of brand associations de- nections, whereas brand associations inconsis-
riving from one’s own group (an ingroup) versus groups to tent with an ingroup will have an unfavorable
which one does not belong (an outgroup). Consumers are effect on self-brand connections.
likely to accept meanings from brands associated or con-
sistent with an ingroup and reject meanings associated or H1b: Brand associations consistent with an outgroup
consistent with an outgroup. Consumers form connections will have an unfavorable effect on self-brand
to brands that become meaningful through this process; self- connections, whereas brand associations incon-
brand connections are intended to measure the extent to sistent with an outgroup will have a favorable
which individuals have incorporated brands into their self- effect on self-brand connections.
concept (Escalas and Bettman 2003). Therefore, given our
focus on the self-construction processes using brands, rather Note that hypotheses 1a and 1b predict a two-way interaction
than information processing in response to an advertisement, of ingroup versus outgroup and the degree to which brand
the primary dependent variable in our studies is a measure associations are consistent (brand image matches) or incon-
of the degree to which consumers have formed a self-brand sistent (brand image does not match) with the group.
connection.
If reference groups use and become associated with par-
ticular brands (i.e., the brand’s image is consistent with or
The Role of Self-Construal
matches the group), such meaning may be appropriated by Brands become linked to the self when a brand is able to
consumers as they construct their self-identities. For ex- help consumers achieve goals that are motivated by the self.
ample, if I consider myself to be an intellectual and my For example, brands can be used to meet self-expression
member group of intellectuals tends to drive Volvo auto- needs, publicly or privately. They can serve as tools for
mobiles, I also may choose to drive a Volvo car as a symbol social integration or connecting us to the past. They may
of how intellectual I am. As a result, consumers may form act as symbols of personal accomplishment, provide self-
self-brand connections to the brands used by reference esteem, allow one to differentiate oneself and express in-
groups to which they belong. Conversely, consumers may dividuality, and help people through life transitions. A major
avoid associations derived from groups to which they do focus of our article is how self-construction goals differ
not belong. When outgroup members use a brand, consum- depending upon a consumer’s self-construal. We propose
ers may form associations about the brand that they would that the influence of ingroup and outgroup brand usage will
not like to have transferred to themselves. Nevertheless, the differ depending upon whether the consumer has a primarily
brand becomes meaningful through the process of avoiding independent or interdependent self-construal.
380 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH
Social cognition research on the self has developed a (Kampmeier and Simon 2001), we can make more detailed
variety of theoretical constructs to explain the complex na- predictions about the combined influence of independent
ture of self-knowledge and self-related behavior. The self is versus interdependent self-construal, ingroup versus out-
conceptualized as consisting of multiple aspects (Linville group, and whether a brand matches the image of a group.
1987), including social roles and personality traits, the most The most clear predictions relate to the case of outgroup
important of which are schematic self-aspects (Markus brand associations. Kampmeier and Simon (2001) show that
1977), and possible selves, that is, individuals’ ideas of what when the focus is on a comparison to an outgroup, the
they might become, what they would like to become, and differentiation aspect of the individual self is emphasized.
what they are afraid of becoming (Markus and Nurius 1986). Thus, for more independent individuals, comparison to the
In this article, we focus on two aspects of self, independent outgroup should lead to a heightened need to differentiate
and interdependent selves, and examine the effects of dif- from the outgroup to create a unique self-concept. However,
ferences between individuals in the extent to which they are people with more interdependent self-construals should be
schematic on these two aspects. more immune to outgroup brand associations, as their pri-
Although the self-concept often is considered to be dis- mary motivation stems from forming relationships within
tinct from other people’s self-concepts, recent cross-cultural the ingroup. This implies that a brand associated with the
evidence suggests that individuals’ mental representations outgroup should lead to lower self-brand connections for
of self may depend on social aspects of self, such as rela- more independent individuals than for more interdependent
tionships with others and membership in social groups individuals. There is not a clear argument for differential
(Brewer and Gardner 1996; Markus and Kitayama 1991; effects for the ingroup related to degree of independence
Triandis 1989). Such research indicates that on average, versus interdependence. Thus, we hypothesize:
Westerners tend to focus on the personal self, thinking of
H2: Brand associations consistent with an outgroup
themselves in terms of unique personal traits and attributes
will lead to lower self-brand connections for in-
and de-emphasizing others (independent self-construal),
dependent self-construals than for interdependent
whereas Easterners tend to focus on the social self and
self-construals.
how the self is related to other people (interdependent self-
construal; Markus and Kitayama 1991). For example, Trian- Note that hypothesis 2 implies a three-way interaction of
dis (1989) argues that more individualistic cultures are char- ingroup versus outgroup, independent versus interdepen-
acterized by more focus on the private self and less emphasis dent, and brand image matching versus not matching that
on the collective self, with increased emphasis on the col- of the group on self-brand connections, and the specific
lective self for less individualistic cultures. interaction we propose relies on both self-construal differ-
These two aspects of self can coexist within the individual ences and ingroup versus outgroup considerations.
(Aaker and Lee 2001; Brewer and Gardner 1996) and can
vary across ethnocultural background within Western so-
ciety. That is, individuals may have both independent and Brand Symbolism
interdependent aspects of self but may differ in the relative Our basic premise is that consumers appropriate the mean-
strength of those aspects on a chronic basis, leading to in- ing of brands as they construct their self-identities, partic-
dividual differences in self-construal that can be assessed ularly brand meaning that arises from reference group use
(Singelis 1994). There are also likely to be chronic differ- and nonuse of brands. Thus far we have treated all brands
ences in the relative strength of independent versus inter- equally in our hypotheses. However, some brands are better
dependent self-construals across ethnocultural backgrounds able than others to communicate something about the person
within American society. For example, compared to Asian using them. For example, prior consumer research proposes
Americans and Hispanic Americans, whites will be rela- that publicly consumed (vs. privately consumed) and luxury
tively more independent and less interdependent in their self- (vs. necessity) products are better able to convey symbolic
construals (Aaker and Schmitt 2001; Lee, Aaker, and Gard- meaning about an individual (Bearden and Etzel 1982). Ad-
ner 2000; Markus and Kitayama 1991; Triandis 1989). In ditionally, a brand that is very popular and used by many
this article, we focus on differences in self-construals due different types of people (e.g., a Honda Accord automobile)
to ethnocultural background differences and individual dif- may not communicate specific associations about the person
ferences (i.e., chronic tendencies). These differences are im- who uses it. Therefore, we expect the basic effect postulated
portant, because independent self-construals can lead to in hypotheses 1a and 1b to be moderated by the degree to
motivations different from interdependent self-construals. which a brand is perceived to be symbolic, that is, able to
Independent self-construal goals include both independence communicate something about the individual using the
(i.e., self-determination) and differentiation (i.e., distinc- brand. Consumers will be more likely to form self-brand
tiveness), whereas interdependent self-construal goals focus connections to symbolic brands with appropriate associa-
on aspects of self shared with some subset of others, en- tions as they construct their self-identities than with brands
hancing maintenance of relationships (Aaker and Schmitt that do not communicate much about the self-identity of the
2001; Kampmeier and Simon 2001). user. Conversely, consumers will be more likely to reject
By considering different facets of the independent self forming a self-brand connection with symbolic brands with
SELF-CONSTRUAL, REFERENCE GROUPS, AND BRAND MEANING 381
two groups based on their self-reported ethnicity. Asians ments” and “It is important for me to respect decisions made
and Hispanics make up the interdependent ethnocultural by the group,” a p .58) and two of the independent scale
group, while whites form the independent ethnocultural items (“I’d rather say ‘No’ directly than risk being misun-
group. Participants who checked African American or derstood” and “I enjoy being unique and different from
“other” (or left the question blank) were eliminated from others in many respects,” a p .44 ). These items were cho-
the data set, leaving a total of 243 participants. As noted sen based on their having the highest item-to-total corre-
above, during the study each participant entered two groups, lations with the corresponding twelve-item Singelis (1994)
an ingroup and an outgroup. Additionally, for each group, scale in previous research. Further checks measured the de-
participants entered a brand consistent with the image of the gree to which the participant belongs to the ingroup and the
group and a brand not consistent with the image of the group outgroup he/she entered, assessed using the average of three
(for examples of groups and brands listed, see table 1), items (“I consider myself to be this type of person,” “I
resulting in a set of four brands. The groups and brands are belong to this group,” and “I fit in with this group of people,”
idiosyncratic to each participant and are not of interest in anchored by strongly disagree [0]/strongly agree [100];
the analysis; the data are coded only by group type (ingroup a p .96).
vs. outgroup) and brand image match (image matches versus
image does not match). Thus, we have a 2 # 2 # 2 mixed
design in our study, with ethnocultural group (independent Results
vs. interdependent) as a between-subjects variable and group The model used in the analyses to predict self-brand con-
type and brand image match as within-subjects variables. nections is a mixed ANOVA model, with ethnocultural
Dependent Variable. Self-brand connections were group (white vs. Asian/Hispanic, or independent vs. inter-
measured using seven items (anchored by strongly disagree dependent) as a between-subjects factor and group type (in-
[0] to strongly agree [100]; see Escalas and Bettman 2003), group vs. outgroup) and brand image match (match vs. no
averaged to form one self-brand connection score per par- match) as within-subject factors. The dependent variable
ticipant per brand (a p .96): was self-brand connections.
1. This brand reflects who I am. Manipulation Checks. White participants exhibited
2. I can identify with this brand. significantly stronger independent selves on the two
3. I feel a personal connection to this brand. Singelis scale items compared to the Asian/Hispanic
4. I use this brand to communicate who I am to other participants (white p 65.69, Asian/Hispanic p 58.41,
people. F(1, 241) p 8.43, p ! .01). However, the Asian/Hispanic
5. I think this brand helps me become the type of person participants were only directionally higher on the inter-
I want to be. dependent-self measure compared to the white participants
6. I consider this brand to be “me” (it reflects who I (Asian/Hispanic p 61.02, white p 57.08, F(1, 241) p
consider myself to be or the way that I want to present 1.54, p p .22), making our tests more conservative. Par-
myself to others). ticipants considered themselves to belong to the ingroup
7. This brand suits me well. significantly more than they felt they belonged to the out-
group (ingroup p 82.74, outgroup p 15.90, F(1, 241) p
1687.31, p ! .001).
Manipulation Checks. To check whether our ethno-
cultural groups vary on the degree to which they have in- Hypotheses 1a and 1b. These hypotheses predict
dependent versus interdependent self-construals, we had par- a two-way interaction of group type and brand image
ticipants complete two of the Singelis (1994) interdependent match, such that whether or not brand associations match
scale items (“I often have the feeling that my relationships a group’s image has a different effect on self-brand con-
with others are more important than my own accomplish- nections depending on whether the group is an ingroup or
TABLE 1
outgroup brand associations on self-brand connections is from the data set due to incomplete or improper responses,
stronger for white (independent) consumers than for Asian/ leaving a total of 161 participants.
Hispanic (interdependent) consumers. As Kampmeier and
Simon (2001) argue, differentiation needs are more predom- Procedure. This study is virtually identical to that used
inant for the independent self when outgroups are the focus; in study 1, with four exceptions. First, half of the subjects
thus, whites, who tend to be more independent, show a completed the Singelis (1994) independent and interdepen-
stronger effect (hypothesis 2). dent self-construal scales at the beginning of the study, while
In study 1, we examined self-construal differences due half the subjects completed these scales at the end of the
to ethnocultural background. In study 2, we examine chron- study.2 Second, after participants rated the degree to which
ic tendencies toward interdependent and independent self- they fit within each group, they were asked to rate the extent
construals without reference to ethnicity, using the scales to which an association with each group type would com-
developed by Singelis (1994) as a measure of these two municate something positive or negative about them. Third,
types of self-construals. We expect the same general pattern after participants rated the degree to which they had formed
of results for this different way of categorizing chronic dif- self-brand connections with the four brands they had entered
ferences in independence and interdependence. We also ex- earlier (generated by the four combinations of ingroup vs.
amine hypothesis 3, that effects on self-brand connections outgroup and brand image matches vs. does not match),
are stronger for brands that are symbolic and communicate they were also asked to rate the brands on a number of
something about the user. Finally, we use thought protocols dimensions, including the degree to which the brand was
to examine our findings about outgroups in more detail. able to communicate something symbolic about the brand’s
user. This was followed by the collection of thought pro-
tocols, where participants were asked to retrospectively re-
STUDY 2 port the thoughts they had when they rated the degree to
which each brand reflected “you” versus “not you.” With
In this study, we again examine the influence of ingroups the exception that the filler task was slightly different, in all
and outgroups on self-brand connections, with the expec- other respects the study was the same as study 1. The entire
tation that the effects will differ for people with chronically procedure took approximately one half hour.
independent versus interdependent self-concepts. We ex-
amine individual differences in independent and interde- Independent Variables. Participants completed the
pendent self-construals without regard to ethnic background, entire Singelis (1994) scales for independent (12 items,
thus providing a test of the effect of independence versus a p .64) and interdependent (12 items, a p .62) chronic
interdependence unconfounded by ethnicity. We use the two self-concepts. Based on median splits, participants were
Singelis scales (1994) for independence and interdepen- divided into high and low groups for each self-construal
dences to identify participants who are high on one type of type. Participants who were high in independent and low in
self-construal, but low on the other. Additionally, we ask interdependent were considered to be schematic on inde-
participants to rate the degree to which the brands they have pendence, while participants who were high in interdepen-
listed are able to communicate something about one’s self- dence and low in independence were considered to be sche-
identity to test hypothesis 3. We propose that the extent to matic on interdependence. Participants who were high on
which a brand has such symbolic characteristics will mod- both or low on both scales were eliminated from the data
erate the basic effects postulated in hypotheses 1a and 1b, set, leaving a total of 75 participants.3 By construction, the
with stronger effects for more symbolic brands (i.e., those interdependent participants scored significantly higher on
brands better able to communicate about one’s self-identity). the mean of the interdependence items (69.37 vs. 54.35,
In addition, we collect thought protocols and other measures F(1, 743) p 102.62, p ! .001) and significantly lower on
to explore the processes underlying our findings. In partic- the mean of the independence items (57.21 vs. 73.65,
ular, we examine the degree to which participants explicitly F(1, 73) p 127.52, p ! .001) compared to the independent
report negative and positive links to the self for outgroups participants. Participants also rated the extent to which each
and the degree to which being associated with a group re- of their four brands was symbolic with two 100-point scale
flects positively or negatively on an individual. We expect items (“to what extent does this brand communicate some-
that we will find additional evidence that independents are thing specific about the person who uses it?” anchored by
more concerned with differentiating themselves from out- “does not communicate a lot/communicates a lot”; and “how
groups than are interdependents. much does this brand symbolize what kind of person uses
it?” anchored by “not at all symbolic/highly symbolic”;
Method 2
No order effects were found as a result of this counterbalancing measure.
3
We also ran the analyses reported for this study using a continuous
Participants. Two hundred and fourteen undergradu- index of the degree of independence relative to interdependence for each
individual. This index was constructed using the Singelis scales as (in-
ate students at a public university in the Southwest partic- dependent ⫺ interdependent)/(independent + interdependent). Using this
ipated in this study to meet an introductory marketing course index allowed us to retain all participants in the analysis. The results were
requirement. Fifty-three participants had to be eliminated virtually identical to those reported below.
SELF-CONSTRUAL, REFERENCE GROUPS, AND BRAND MEANING 385
FIGURE 5
vious consumer research on value-expressive social influ- listed a social group to which they did not belong, resulting
ence, that is, that consumers use brands whose images match in wide variance in the nature and specificity of these out-
reference groups to which they belong to establish a psy- groups (see table 1). An interesting issue for future research
chological association with those groups. Further, we find would be to explore the effects of different types of out-
that self-brand connections are lower for brands with images groups. It is very likely that there are both outgroups that
that are consistent with the image of an outgroup compared one does not belong to but are relatively unimportant and
to brands with images that are inconsistent with an outgroup. groups to which one does not belong but wants to be clearly
This finding that consumers reject the social meanings of distanced from. Different types of outgroups may have vary-
brands that arise from outgroup brand usage is an important ing effects on the extent to which consumers reject the social
contribution of this article. meaning arising from outgroup brand usage. In addition, the
We also show that independent versus interdependent self- degree to which a brand is strongly or weakly associated
construals interact with our congruency findings to deter- with an ingroup or outgroup may moderate our effects.
mine the level of self-brand connections using two different
approaches to operationalizing chronic differences in self- [Dawn Iacobucci served as editor and Joseph Priester
construal: ethnicity and Singelis’s (1994) scales. We find served as associate editor for this article.]
that the negative effect of outgroup brand associations on
self-brand connections is stronger for independent consum- REFERENCES
ers than for interdependent consumers. We argue and pro-
vide some empirical support for the notion that this is due Aaker, Jennifer L. and Angela Y. Lee (2001), “‘I’ Seek Pleasures
to the stronger needs of more independent consumers to and ‘We’ Avoid Pains: The Role of Self-Regulatory Goals in
Information Processing and Persuasion,” Journal of Con-
differentiate themselves from outgroups (Kampmeier and sumer Research, 28 (June), 33–49.
Simon 2001). We also find that our effects are moderated Aaker, Jennifer L. and Bernd Schmitt (2001), “Culture-Dependent
by brand symbolism, such that brands that communicate Assimilation and Differentiation of the Self: Preferences for
something about the user yield stronger effects than brands Consumption Symbols in the United States and China,” Jour-
that do not. In the case of ingroup associations, the positive nal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32 (September), 561–76.
effect of image congruency is stronger for those brands that Ball, A. Dwayne and Lori H. Tasaki (1992), “The Role and Mea-
are perceived to communicate something symbolic about surement of Attachment in Consumer Behavior,” Journal of
the brand’s user compared to those brands that do not. In Consumer Psychology, 1 (2), 155–72.
the case of outgroup associations, only symbolic brands are Bearden, William O. and Michael J. Etzel (1982), “Reference
used to differentiate oneself from outgroup associations; the Group Influence on Product and Brand Purchase Decisions,”
Journal of Consumer Research, 9 (September), 183–94.
effects disappear for brands that are considered to not com- Bearden, William O., Richard G. Netemeyer, and Jesse E. Teel
municate anything about the brand’s user. (1989), “Measurement of Consumer Susceptibility to Inter-
personal Influence,” Journal of Consumer Research, 15
Limitations and Directions for Future Research (March), 473–81.
Belk, Russell W. (1988), “Possessions and the Extended Self,”
Our article focuses on the constructs of independent ver- Journal of Consumer Research, 15 (September), 139–68.
sus interdependent self-construals, which bear a close re- Brewer, Marilynn B. and Wendi Gardner (1996), “Who Is This
semblance to the individualism-collectivism dimension of ‘We’? Levels of Collective Identity and Self Representations,”
culture proposed by Hofstede (1980) and others (e.g., Trian- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71 (July),
dis 1989). We find that independents are motivated to dif- 83–93.
ferentiate themselves from outgroups, whereas interdepen- Burnkrant, Robert E. and Alain Cousineau (1975), “Informational
dents are relatively unaffected by outgroups. However, and Normative Social Influence in Buyer Behavior,” Journal
of Consumer Research, 2 (December), 206–15.
research on individualism versus collectivism has found that Childers, Terry L. and Akshay R. Rao (1992), “The Influence of
collectivists may behave antagonistically toward outgroup Familial and Peer-Based Reference Groups on Consumer De-
members (e.g., by pursuing conflict with outgroup members; cisions,” Journal of Consumer Research, 19 (September),
Leung 1988). Although many of these studies use strangers 198–211.
rather than well-defined outgroups, this finding appears to Escalas, Jennifer Edson and James R. Bettman (2003), “You Are
be inconsistent with our finding that interdependents pay What They Eat: The Influence of Reference Groups on Con-
little attention to outgroups. Further research could explore sumer Connections to Brands,” Journal of Consumer Psy-
a number of possible ideas for how to reconcile this apparent chology, 13 (3), 339–48.
conflict, such as contrasting interpersonal behavior toward Fournier, Susan (1998), “Consumers and Their Brands: Developing
outgroup members with consumption behavior or exploring Relationship Theory in Consumer Research,” Journal of Con-
sumer Research, 24 (March), 343–73.
other constructs (e.g., interpersonal orientation, self-moni- Heider, Fritz (1946), “Attitudes and Cognitive Organization,” Jour-
toring, or social identity) that may underlie the independent- nal of Psychology, 21 (January), 107–12.
interdependent differences we find. Hofstede, Geert (1980), Culture’s Consequences: International
As mentioned above, many of the studies examining in- Differences in Work-Related Values, Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
dividualism versus collectivism use strangers as a proxy for Kampmeier, Claudia and Bernd Simon (2001), “Individuality and
well-defined outgroups. In our research, each participant Group Formation: The Role of Independence and Differen-
SELF-CONSTRUAL, REFERENCE GROUPS, AND BRAND MEANING 389
tiation,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81 McCracken, Grant (1988), Culture and Consumption, Blooming-
(September), 448–62. ton: Indiana University Press.
Kleine, Susan Schultz, Robert E. Kleine III, and Chris T. Allen ——— (1989), “Who Is the Celebrity Endorser? Cultural Foun-
(1995), “How Is a Possession ‘Me’ or ‘Not Me’? Character- dations of the Endorsement Process,” Journal of Consumer
izing Types and an Antecedent of Material Possession At- Research, 16 (December), 310–21.
tachment,” Journal of Consumer Research, 22 (December), Moschis, George P. (1985), “The Role of Family Communication
327–43. in Consumer Socialization of Children and Adolescents,”
Lee, Angela Y., Jennifer L. Aaker, and Wendi L. Gardner (2000), Journal of Consumer Research, 11 (March), 898–913.
“The Pleasures and Pains of Distinct Self-Construals: The Muniz, Albert M. and Thomas C. O’Guinn (2001), “Brand Com-
Role of Interdependence in Regulatory Focus,” Journal of munity,” Journal of Consumer Research, 27 (March), 412–32.
Personality and Social Psychology, 78 (June), 1122–34. Park, C. Whan and V. Parker Lessig (1977), “Students and House-
Leung, Kwok (1988), “Some Determinants of Conflict Avoidance,” wives: Differences in Susceptibility to Reference Group In-
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 19 (1), 125–36.
fluence,” Journal of Consumer Research, 4 (September),
Levy, Sidney J. (1959), “Symbols for Sale,” Harvard Business
102–10.
Review, 37 (July–August), 117–24.
Linville, Patricia W. (1987), “Self-Complexity as a Cognitive Richins, Marsha L. (1994), “Valuing Things: The Public and Pri-
Buffer against Stress-Related Illness and Depression,” Journal vate Meanings of Possessions,” Journal of Consumer Re-
of Personality and Social Psychology, 52 (April), 663–76. search, 21 (December), 504–21.
Markus, Hazel (1977), “Self-Schemata and Processing Information Singelis, Theodore M. (1994), “The Measurement of Independent
about the Self,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, and Interdependent Self-Construals,” Personality and Social
35 (August), 63–78. Psychology Bulletin, 20 (October), 580–91.
Markus, Hazel and Shinobu Kitayama (1991), “Culture and the Triandis, Harry C. (1989), “The Self and Behavior in Differing
Self: Implications for Cognition, Emotion, and Motivation,” Cultural Contexts,” Psychological Review, 96 (July), 506–20.
Psychological Review, 98 (April), 224–53. ——— (1993), “Collectivism and Individualism as Cultural Syn-
Markus, Hazel and Paula Nurius (1986), “Possible Selves,” Amer- dromes,” Cross-Cultural Research: The Journal of Compar-
ican Psychologist, 41 (September), 954–69. ative Social Science, 27 (August–November), 155–80.