Admin, Raubenheimeretal
Admin, Raubenheimeretal
Admin, Raubenheimeretal
by
and
C. Dianne Raubenheimer
North Carolina State University
Introduction
Traditional zoology laboratory activities include observation of slides,
observations of living and preserved organisms, and dissections of preserved
animals. While these activities are valuable, they have not consistently included
higher levels of intellectual challenge. For the zoology laboratory course studied,
learning was typically evaluated by performance on written laboratory reports and
on an end of the semester laboratory practical exam, where students essentially
had to regurgitate rather than use and synthesize information. However, Bloom’s
taxonomy suggests that the development of cognitive ability is hierarchical,
progressing from simple understanding to application and synthesis of that
knowledge, and that performance tasks undertaken by students should reflect
the range of cognitive skills (Reed & Bergemann, 2001). This led us to reconsider
how the zoology laboratory section was taught, using an action research
approach.
Effective teaching involves enabling students to develop a deep
understanding of the materials they are studying. This can be achieved through a
variety of thought-demanding tasks (Levin & Nolan, 2000), including having
student explain concepts in their own words, making predictions, doing drawings,
finding exemplars in new contexts and applying concepts to new situations
(Brandt, 1992). Zuber-Skerritt (1992a) concurred, stating
So far we have arrived at the position that the most appropriate mode of
learning and teaching in higher education is that of the alternative
paradigm which may be characteri zed by learner-centered, problem-
oriented, interdisciplinary, process-centered, and using an open, critical
approach. (p. 147)
Methods
Participants
The participants were 36 students (10 males, 24 females, 2 not reported),
enrolled in two different semesters of a freshman undergraduate Zoology course
in a small, private university of around 800 students. Data concerning prior
experience in college laboratory courses or prior knowledge of zoology was not
collected in this study. These students participated in the study as a part of the
normally scheduled laboratory, of which there is only one section offered in the
spring semester of each academic year.
Project Design
Due to past poor performance on the lab practical, Myka asked
Raubenheimer to help improve the zoology laboratory course. One of us,
70.0 70.0
60.0 60.0
50.0
50.0
40.0
40.0
30.0
30.0 20.0
20.0 10.0
10.0 0.0
0.0
an find
m lan
m ure
po te
rve
ze e
lat
hy nica
fy
aly siz
co as
e in t
se
y lat
ipu
/ju
ica
u
se ide ipu ex
ob un
m
n e
m
a m p
m m ex
co
Annelids 1 Molluscs 1
type of activity
Annelids 2 Molluscs 2
type of activity
Figure 1. Verb content analysis for Figure 2 . Verb content analysis for
Annelid activity. Mollusc activity.
Figure 3. Verb content analysis for Figure 4. Verb content analysis for
Arthropod activity. Echinoderm activity.
In all cases, the redesigned labs contained a greater range of tasks and
categories, while the original labs focused on some form of observation or
manipulation of the animals.
We then used the levels in Bloom’s taxonomy and associated verbs (Reed
& Bergmann, 2001) as the basis for further textual analysis, counting the number
of times particular verbs were associated with a particular level in the taxonomy.
Using Bloom’s taxonomy the total range of tasks (i.e. all the verbs used in the
laboratory manual in all four laboratory sessions) were classified into the six
levels in Bloom’s taxonomy, namely (a) knowledge, (b) comprehension, (c)
application, (d) analysis, (e) synthesis, and (f) evaluation. For instance, the
“manipulate” examples were then included in Bloom’s “application” category.
These data were transformed into percentages. These results are presented in
Figure 5.
93.7
100.0
80.0 72.5
60.0 Original
40.0 Redesigned
20.0 10 8.3
4.0 2.4 2.5 3.3 3.3
0.0
is
n
sis
on
ge
es
tio
n
aly
ati
led
sio
ca
nth
alu
An
ow
en
pli
Sy
Ev
Ap
reh
4.
Kn
5.
6.
mp
3.
1.
Co
2.
Bloom's classification
Redesigned Activities
1.) Redesigned Annelid activity
The annelid activity tested in this action research project led the students
through a series of observations of live earthworms, a cross-section slide,
demonstration materials, and preserved specimens. However, the redesigned
activity included a directed experiment on worm behavior, followed by a student-
designed experiment that resulted in a written laboratory report due one week
later. As shown in Figure 1, the redesigned activities led to a decrease of 8.1%
in explanations, but an increase of 8.9% in communication, both with a Bloom’s
taxonomy of level 2. However, the addition of the directed experiment was
particularly valuable in that the manipulations (Bloom’s level 1) were reduced by
15.7%, while experimentation (Bloom’s level 6) was 6.8%. A dissected
earthworm was on demonstration, and was observed by students, and a plastic
model was also available. These alternatives to student dissection were
implemented for two reasons: a.) the earthworm dissection was never very
successful in that the anaesthetized earthworms were difficult for students to
accurately dissect and they usually were not able to visualize the features
discussed in the lab handouts, and b.) we wanted to see if student performance
would be affected if they did not perform the dissection themselves, but used
alternatives to visualize specific features.
The original annelid activity directed students to observe living and
preserved earthworms and other annelids, and to design a simple experiment,
“DESIGN A SIMPLE EXPERIMENT that would show whether the
worm is positively or negatively geotactic (gravity). You need not
be able to do the experiment; just design it.”
In addition, the original activity asked students to anesthetize and dissect
a living earthworm. Myka had noted that the anesthetizing did not work
consistently, and that students often seemed disturbed by trying to vivisect the
earthworm in order to see the hearts beating. Indeed, Myka noted that only one
student group had actually been able to make this observation during the
previous five semesters in which it had been attempted; usually the worm died
before the students could dissect enough to observe the heart. In addition,
students often could not visualize the noted features in their own dissections, and
The first four rankings are the same for both the activities preferred for
learning and those selected for maximal enjoyment. These are (a) observation of
live animals, (b) doing own dissections, (c) looking at dissections, and (d)
designing and conducting investigations on live animals. Spearman’s rank order
correlation showed that the overall ranking of the two categories is significantly
correlated (ρ = .729, p = 0.01).
The data for 2003, presented in Table 2 also showed a there is an overall
similarity between student ranking of preferred learning activities (W = .255, p =
0.000), as well as for activities rated for enjoyment (W = .339, p = 0.000).
The first three rankings are the same for both the activities preferred for
learning and those selected for maximal enjoyment. These are (a) doing own
dissections, (b) looking at dissections and (c) observation of live animals. The
overall ranking of the two categories is also significantly correlated (Spearman’s
rank order correlation, ρ = .877, p = 0.01). The high correlations showed that
overall students felt they learned more from activities they enjoyed, and
conversely, that they learned less from activities they did not enjoy.
While the overall rank orders differed between 2002 and 2003, there is a
strong correlation between the two years for both rank preferences of activities
for learning (Spearman’s rank order correlation, ρ = .749, p = 0.01) and activities
enjoyed (Spearman’s rank order correlation, ρ = .877, p = 0.01). Therefore the
instructor change between 2002 and 2003 did not significantly alter student
rankings.
In terms of items for course improvement, we looked explicitly at activities
in which there was not an overall correspondence between enjoyment and
learning because these highlighted areas for improvement. For instance, in 2002,
‘observing specimens in bottles’ ranked 15th for learning preference but 7th for
enjoyment. This meant that in terms of learning potential we were not capitalizing
on the inherent interest students had in observing preserved specimens. This
indicated an area for future development. Other discrepancies were found in
100% No
Percentage of students
90% response
80%
70% Neutral
60%
50%
Didn't help
40% learning
30%
20% Helped
10% learning
0%
ns als els s s
ion ns els als ns pe
e ctio anim mod ation rvat e lides mod ms anim ectio osco les ke
ys
iss ve ic ig se cim es ng gra n g iss ic r ab us
t t
t d g li plas ves g ob spe cop truct i i d t
dia sify n t m on mo
ga in in in d s s nd las g ow g ligh uncti hoto
okin serv ing a own raw erve icro Con hs a C in in f
d gd ic
Lo Ob ook ting D res al m rap Do Us an
L uc in g p ssion otog tu re tuctin
nd rv e h c ns
Co se Prof gp str
u
Co
Ob w n
i
king
Vie Ma
Category of activity
90
80
70
60
50
40
30
20
10
0
Very Slightly Neutral Slightly More
positive positive negative negative
Category of opinion
25%
33% Hands-on
Enjoyment
Enhanced learning
42%
60
50
40
30
20
10
0
structure location function skills
Category of learning
Discussion
As is the intention of an action research process, our findings have
highlighted a number of changes that need to be made in instruction in order to
enhance student learning. A discussion of the results and the changes
suggested by the data follows.
Our results indicate that the revised activities asked students to perform
tasks with a higher level of intellectual challenge, as indicated by the increase in
higher Bloom’s taxonomy verbs determined by content analysis of the original
and revised activities (refer to Figures 1 through 5). Additionally, there was a
decrease of 21.2% in the use of terms from Bloom’s level 1, the knowledge
terminology. It may be possible to further increase the use of higher levels of
intellectual challenge, and further reduce the reliance on knowledge terms
(93.7% of original activities) by revising other activities in the semester-long
laboratory course. However, it should be noted that part of learning a subject like
zoology is to learn the “language” of zoology, and the authors do not suggest that
tasks at Bloom’s level 1 should be abandoned. Students need to be led through
the levels, so that they can learn and build upon what they learn, and they would
not be well served by eliminating this first step.
Bernard, H. R., & Ryan, G. W. (1998). Text analysis: Qualitative and quantitative
methods. In H. R. Bernard (Ed.), Handbook of Methods in Cultural
Anthropology (pp. 595-629). Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press.
Biggs, J., Kember, D., & Leung, D. Y. P. (2001). The revised two factor study
process questionnaire: R-SPQ-2F. British Journal of Educational Psychology,
71, 133-149.
Carr, W., & Kemmis, S. (1986). Becoming critical: Education, knowledge and
action Research. London: The Falmer Press.
Cross, K.P., & Steadman, M.H. (1996). Classroom research: Implementing the
scholarship of teaching. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Mills, G. E. (2000). Action research: A guide for the teacher researcher. Upper
Saddle River, NJ: Merrill Prentice Hall.
Prosser, M., & Trigwell, K. (1999). Understanding and learning in teaching: The
experience in higher education. Philadelphia, PA: Society for Research
into Higher Education & Open University Press.
Prosser, M., Trigwell, K., Hazel, E., & Waterhouse, F. (2000). Students’
experiences of studying physics concepts: The effect of disintegrated
perceptions and approaches. European Journal of Psychology of
Education, 15, 61-74.
Intended
Major:___________________ Level: Fre Sop Jun Snr
We are intere sted in finding out about your learning experiences during the General
Zoology BIO102L laboratory sessions. Please complete the following questionnaire
to help us enhance student learning in the future.
Please rank 1 – 15 which activities you Please rank 1 – 15 which activities you
learned the most from. 1 should be your enjoyed the most. 1 should be your first
first preference and 15 the activity you preference and 15 your least favorite
learned least from. activity.
3. Dissections
For each of the activities listed, please circle the appropriate answer. Please provide a written response for
your ratings.
SA = strongly agree; A = agree; DN = don’t know; DA = disagree; SDA = strongly disagree.
a) Looking at dissections helped my learning. i) Looking at photographs and diagrams helped my learning
SA A DN DA SDA SA A DN DA SDA
b) Observing live animal behavior helped my learning j) Classifying animals into classes helped my learning
SA A DN DA SDA SA A DN DA SDA
c) Looking at plastic models helped my learning k) Doing my own dissections helped my learning
SA A DN DA SDA SA A DN DA SDA
d) Designing and conducting my own investigation on live l) Observing using the light microscope helped my learning
animals helped my learning SA A DN DA SDA
SA A DN DA SDA
e) Drawing observations helped my learning m) Constructing tables of structure and function helped my
SA A DN DA SDA learning
SA A DN DA SDA
f) Observing specimens in bottles helped my learning n) Constructing dichotomous keys helped my learning
SA A DN DA SDA SA A DN DA SDA
g) Looking at professionally-prepared microscope slides o) Looking at microscope slides that I prepared myself helped my
helped my learning learning
SA A DN DA SDA SA A DN DA SDA
h) Constructing models helped my learning p) Please provide any additional comments in the space below.
SA A DN DA SDA
Dr. Jennifer Leigh Myka is an Associate Professor of Biology at Thomas More College
in Crestview Hills, Kentucky, where she currently teaches undergraduate courses in
Genetics, Molecular Genetics, Developmental Biology, Nutrition, and Anatomy and
Physiology. With a background in molecular/cellular biology and genetics, Jennifer has
also taught a variety of other undergraduate biology courses such as Zoology, Botany,
Biochemistry, Molecular Biology, Advanced Cell Biology, and non-majors courses such
as Introduction to Biology. Her research interests include science education research
with the goal of improving instruction in science courses at the undergraduate level and
in outreach activities for K-12, and also basic science research in chromosome
organization and genome structure in the relatives of the domestic horse in the Family
Equidae.