Chapter 4 Memory and Learning
Chapter 4 Memory and Learning
Chapter 4 Memory and Learning
of Memory and
Learning
This presentation explores the fundamental
aspects of how the human mind stores,
retrieves, and processes information, focusing
on the key principles and mechanisms
underlying memory and learning.
Memory Process
Understanding the processes of encoding, storage, and retrieval is crucial for effective
learning and memory formation. By exploring these key concepts, we can gain insights
into how the human mind encodes, stores, and retrieves information, which can have
important implications for education, training, and everyday cognitive functioning.
Memory Process
Encoding
Encoding is the process of getting information into
our brain.
Once we receive sensory information from the environment, our brains label or code it. We
organize the information with other similar information and connect new concepts to existing
concepts. Encoding information occurs through automatic processing and effortful processing.
The Psychology of Memory and Learning: Automatic Processing
Solving a Complex Math Problem Playing a New Musical Instrument Learning how to Drive
Working through each step of a challenging Learning to play an instrument for the first When you first learn to drive, you have to
math problem demands concentration and time involves focused practice and attention consciously think about each action, like
deliberate thinking. to each note and movement. steering, braking, and checking mirrors.
Effortful processing refers to the deliberate, conscious way our brains handle
tasks or information that require focused attention and mental effort.
3 Types of Encoding
Storage
Storage is the creation of a permanent record of
information.
In order for a memory to go into storage (i.e., long-term memory), it has to pass through three distinct stages: Sensory Memory,
Short-Term Memory, and finally Long-Term Memory. These stages were first proposed by Richard Atkinson and Richard Shiffrin
(1968). Their model of human memory, called Atkinson and Shiffrin's model, is based on the belief that we process memories in the
same way that a computer processes information.
What is a sensory memory?
Sensory memory
In the Atkinson-Shiffrin model, stimuli from the
environment are processed first in sensory
memory: storage of brief sensory events, such as
sights, sounds, and tastes. It is very brief storage
—up to a couple of seconds. We are constantly
bombarded with sensory information. We cannot
absorb all of it, or even most of it. And most of it
has no impact on our lives. Sensory information
about sights, sounds, smells, and even textures,
which we do not view as valuable information,
we discard.
What is Long-Term Memory?
Short-Term memory
Is a temporary storage system that processes incoming
sensory memory. The terms short-term and working
memory are sometimes used interchangeably, but they are
not exactly the same. Short-term memory is more accurately
described as a component of working memory. Short-term
memory takes information from sensory memory and
sometimes connects that memory to something already in
long-term memory. Short-term memory storage lasts 15 to
30 seconds.
How does short term memory go to long term memory?
Rehearsal
Rehearsal is essential for moving information from short-
term to long-term memory. Active rehearsal involves
repeating information until it is memorized, like children
learning the alphabet by singing the ABCs. Elaborative
rehearsal links new information to existing knowledge, such
as remembering a new phone number by associating it with
a familiar area code. Craik and Lockhart's (1972) levels of
processing hypothesis states that deeper thinking improves
memory retention.
What is Long-Term Memory?
Long-Term memory
Long-term memory (LTM) is the continuous storage of
information with an unlimited capacity. It includes
everything you can remember beyond a few minutes ago.
Memories in LTM are organized in semantic (associative)
networks, where related concepts are linked based on
experiences and frequency of association. Activating one
concept in a semantic network can partially activate related
concepts, making them easier to recall, a process known as
spreading activation.
2 Types of Long-Term Memory
Long-term memory
Continuous storage of information
Explicit Implicit
Needs conscious remembering Cannot be consciously recalled. i.e
observable behaviors
Episodic Procedural
Personal events experienced Information about how the way
to do something
Semantic Priming
Knowledge of words, concepts, Exposure to a stimulus affect the
and language response
Emotional Conditioning
Classically conditioned
emotional responses
Fun Fact!
Marilu Henner
Hyperthymesia is a rare condition where a person has an
exceptionally vivid and detailed memory of their personal
experiences.
Retrieval Recall
The process of accessing and bringing information The ability to remember information without any
from memory into conscious awareness. This can be external cues or prompts. This requires actively
through recall, recognition, or cued recall. reconstructing the information from memory.
Recognition Relearning
The ability to identify information or stimuli as having The process of learning something that has been
been previously encountered. It involves matching new previously learned and then forgotten. Relearning is
information to existing representations in memory. typically faster than initial learning due to the
established memory traces.
Memory Retrieval
Anterograde Amnesia
With anterograde amnesia, you cannot remember new information, although you can remember information and events that happened
prior to your injury. Many people with this form of amnesia are unable to form new episodic or semantic memories, but are still able to form
new procedural memories.
i.e. The brain damage from H. M.'s surgery caused anterograde amnesia, preventing him from forming new memories. He repeatedly read
the same magazine and failed to recognize people he met after the surgery. Despite not remembering prior encounters with a puzzle, he
solved it faster each day due to relearning.
Problems with Memory: Retrograde Amnesia
Retrograde Amnesia
Retrograde amnesia is loss of memory for events that occurred prior to the trauma. People with retrograde
amnesia cannot remember some or even all of their past.
Memory Construction and Reconstruction
Construction Reconstruction
The formulation of new memories. Process of bringing up old memories
Suggestibility
When someone witnesses a crime, that person’s memory of the details of the crime is very important in
catching the suspect. Because memory is so fragile, witnesses can be easily (and often accidentally) misled due
to the problem of suggestibility. Suggestibility describes the effects of misinformation from external sources
that leads to the creation of false memories.
i.e. In the fall of 2002, a sniper in the DC area terrorized the public for over three weeks, killing ten people in
various locations. Fear gripped the community, leading to over 140,000 tips and 35,000 potential suspects.
Most of the tips were dead ends, until a white van was spotted at the site of one of the shootings. The police
chief went on national television with a picture of the white van. After the news conference, several other
eyewitnesses called to say that they too had seen a white van fleeing from the scene of the shooting. At the
time, there were more than 70,000 white vans in the area. Police officers, as well as the general public, focused
almost exclusively on white vans because they believed the eyewitnesses. Other tips were ignored. When the
suspects were finally caught, they were driving a blue sedan.
Eyewitness
Misidentification Case
In studying cases where DNA evidence has exonerated
people from crimes, the Innocence Project discovered that
eyewitness misidentification is the leading cause of
wrongful convictions (Benjamin N. Cardozo School of Law,
Yeshiva University, 2009).
Discussion on the Criminal Case of Ronald Cotton
Preserving Eyewitness Memory:
The Elizabeth Smart Case
Contrast the Cotton case with what happened in the Elizabeth Smart case. When Elizabeth was 14 years old and fast asleep in
her bed at home, she was abducted at knifepoint. Her nine-year-old sister, Mary Katherine, was sleeping in the same bed and
watched, terrified, as her beloved older sister was abducted. Mary Katherine was the sole eyewitness to this crime and was very
fearful. In the following weeks, the Salt Lake City police and the FBI proceeded with caution with Mary Katherine. They did not
want to implant any false memories or mislead her in any way. They did not show her police line-ups or push her to do a
composite sketch of the abductor. They knew if they corrupted her memory, Elizabeth might never be found. For several months,
there was little or no progress on the case. Then, about 4 months after the kidnapping, Mary Katherine first recalled that she
had heard the abductor’s voice prior to that night (he had worked exactly one day as a handyman at the family’s home) and then
she was able to name the person whose voice it was. The family contacted the press and others recognized him—after a total of
nine months, the suspect was caught and Elizabeth Smart was returned to her family.
The Misinformation Effect Paradigm by: Elizabeth Loftus (Cognitive Psychologist)
The participants were shown films of car accidents and were asked to play the role of the eyewitness and describe what
happened. They were asked, “About how fast were the cars going when they (smashed, collided, bumped, hit, contacted)
each other?”
According to Loftus, an eyewitness’s memory of an event is highly flexible and susceptible to the misinformation effect. To
test this theory, Loftus and John Palmer (1974) conducted an experiment with 45 U.S. college students. These
participants were shown films of car accidents and asked to act as eyewitnesses, describing what happened. They were
specifically asked to estimate the speed of the cars using different verbs: "smashed," "collided," "bumped," "hit," and
"contacted."
The results showed that the verb used in the question significantly influenced the participants' speed estimates. Those
who heard the word "smashed" estimated the cars to be traveling at a much higher speed than those who heard
"contacted." This implied information about speed, based on the verb used, affected the participants’ memory of the
accident.
In a follow-up study conducted one week later, participants were asked if they saw any broken glass in the accident
scenes (none was actually shown in the films). Those who had been asked the "smashed" question were more than twice
as likely to incorrectly recall seeing broken glass compared to those who heard other verbs. Loftus and Palmer’s study
demonstrated that leading questions could not only influence eyewitnesses’ memory of speeds but also lead to false
memories of non-existent details, such as broken glass.
Participants who heard the word “smashed” estimated that the cars were traveling at a much higher speed than
participants who heard the word “contacted.” The implied information about speed, based on the verb they heard, had an
effect on the participants’ memory of the accident. In a follow-up one week later, participants were asked if they saw any
broken glass (none was shown in the accident pictures). Participants who had been in the “smashed” group were more than
twice as likely to indicate that they did remember seeing glass.
60
50
40
30
20
10
0
Contacted Hit Bumped Collided Smashed
Theory Prevalence
Some researchers believe that A study by John Briere and Jon
extremely traumatic and distressing Conte (1993) found that 59% of
experiences can be repressed for adults receiving treatment for
years and later recalled through childhood sexual abuse had
False Memory techniques like hypnosis and guided forgotten their experiences.
imagery (Devilly, 2007).
Syndrome
Refers to the phenomenon
where people recall entire
events that did not actually
happen, often related to
Ross Cheit (2007) suggested that The Recovered Memory Project
trauma, such as sexual abuse.
repressing these memories created was created so that victims of
psychological distress in adulthood childhood sexual abuse can recall
these memories and allow the
healing process to begin.
Loftu's Argument Example
Loftus is not saying that childhood For example, researchers Stephen
sexual abuse doesn’t happen, but Ceci and Maggie Brucks (1993,
she does question whether or not 1995) asked three-year-old
Criticism of those memories are accurate, and children to use an anatomically
Encoding Failure
To remember something, we must pay attention to the details and actively work to
process the information (effortful encoding)
i.e. Not remembering someone's name in a party because you're preoccupied with
current dialogue with friends.
The Psychology of Memory and Learning
Case Example:
The Central Park Five Background: In 1989, five Black and Latino teenagers
(Kevin Richardson, Raymond Santana, Antron McCray, Yusef Salaam, and Korey
Wise) were wrongfully convicted of the brutal assault and rape of a white female
jogger in Central Park, New York City.
Stereotypical Bias:
Racial Profiling: The media and public perception were influenced by racial
stereotypes, portraying the accused teenagers as inherently violent and
dangerous based on their race and ethnicity.
Proactive Interference:
Retroactive Interference
Reflexes Instincts
An automatic motor or neural reaction to a specific stimulus in the Are innate behaviors that are triggered by a broader range of events, such
environment. as maturation and the change of seasons.
What are the examples of reflexes?
Knee-Jerk reflex
A sudden kick movement of the lower leg in response to a tap on the patellar tendon.
Pupil Contraction
The pupils constricting in response to bright light.
Sneezing
Involuntary expulsion of air through the nose and mouth in response to nasal irritation.
Gag Reflex
Contraction of the throat muscles triggered by touching the back of the throat.
Withdrawal Reflex
Quickly pulling your hand away from a hot surface.
What are the examples of instincts?
Suckling
A sudden kick movement of the lower leg in response to a tap on the patellar tendon.
Crying
Involuntary expulsion of air through the nose and mouth in response to nasal irritation.
Rooting reflex
Contraction of the throat muscles triggered by touching the back of the throat.
Fight-or-flight respons
Quickly pulling your hand away from a hot surface.
Associative Learning
Learning involves a complex interplay of conscious and unconscious processes, particularly in the context of complex
tasks like surfing or studying psychology. Associative learning, a fundamental process in learning, occurs when the mind
connects events that happen together. This process underlies classical conditioning, operant conditioning, and
observational learning, which will be discussed in detail later.
Ivan Pavlov and Classical Conditioning
A Russian scientist known for his work on classical conditioning. Originally a physiologist studying the digestive system of
dogs.
Moisha’s experience with chemotherapy provides a clear example of classical conditioning in action. Initially, the
chemotherapy drugs (unconditioned stimulus) caused her to vomit (unconditioned response). Over time, the doctor’s
office, where she received the treatment, became associated with this unpleasant reaction, making her feel nauseous
(conditioned response) during check-ups. Additionally, Moisha learned to associate syringes, used for her medication,
with nausea because they were paired with the chemotherapy and the doctor’s office, demonstrating higher-order
conditioning. However, classical conditioning typically does not extend beyond second-order conditioning; for instance, a
bell rung during her treatment is unlikely to induce nausea on its own.
During their vacation in the Cayman Islands, Kate and her spouse visited Stingray City, where they interacted with
southern stingrays. Historically, people cleaning fish and conch (unconditioned stimulus) at a sandbar attracted stingrays
(unconditioned response), who came to eat the scraps. Over time, scuba divers feeding the stingrays by hand led to the
stingrays becoming classically conditioned. Now, the sound of a boat engine (neutral stimulus, which becomes a
conditioned stimulus) signals to the stingrays that food is coming (conditioned response). When Kate and her spouse
arrived, the stingrays, conditioned to associate the boat engine with feeding, eagerly approached them. They fed, petted,
and even kissed the stingrays, who then left after the food was gone.
Other Key Terms Classical Conditioning
Acquisition: Learning to associate a neutral stimulus with an unconditioned stimulus, leading to a conditioned response.
Advertising executives are pros at applying the principles of associative learning. Think about the car commercials you
have seen on television. Many of them feature an attractive model. By associating the model with the car being
advertised, you come to see the car as being desirable (Cialdini, 2008). You may be asking yourself, does this advertising
technique actually work? According to Cialdini (2008), men who viewed a car commercial that included an attractive
model later rated the car as being faster, more appealing, and better designed than did men who viewed an advertisement
for the same car minus the model.
Have you ever noticed how quickly advertisers cancel contracts with a famous athlete following a scandal? As far as the
advertiser is concerned, that athlete is no longer associated with positive feelings; therefore, the athlete cannot be used
as an unconditioned stimulus to condition the public to associate positive feelings (the unconditioned response) with
their product (the conditioned stimulus).
Operant Conditioning
In his operant conditioning experiments, Skinner used a technique called shaping to teach complex behaviors. Instead of only
rewarding the final target behavior, shaping involves reinforcing successive approximations of the desired behavior. This
method is necessary because organisms typically don’t display complex behaviors spontaneously. By breaking the behavior
into small, achievable steps, shaping gradually guides the organism toward the target behavior.
Reinforcing responses that increasingly resemble the target behavior, while discontinuing reinforcement for previous
steps.
Gradually reinforcing behaviors that come closer to the target until the desired behavior is achieved.
Shaping is especially useful for teaching complex or chained behaviors and is widely employed by animal trainers. An essential
part of shaping is stimulus discrimination, where the organism learns to distinguish between specific stimuli, similar to how
Pavlov’s dogs responded only to a particular bell tone. This ensures that the organism associates the behavior with the correct
stimulus in operant conditioning.
Primary and Secondary Reinforcers
Primary reinforcers are those that have innate, unlearned reinforcing qualities, such as food, water,
sleep, shelter, sex, and touch. These are things organisms naturally seek because they satisfy basic
physical needs or provide pleasure. For example, jumping into a cool lake on a hot day is inherently
reinforcing because it satisfies the need for physical comfort and provides pleasure.
On the other hand, secondary reinforcers have no inherent value but become reinforcing when
associated with primary reinforcers. Examples include praise or money. Praise, like saying "Great
shot!" to a soccer player, is reinforcing because it’s linked to affection and social approval. Money is
valuable because it can be exchanged for primary reinforcers like food or secondary reinforcers like
luxury items. Without this association, secondary reinforcers hold no value—for instance, money on a
remote island with no way to spend it would be useless. Similarly, things like stickers on a behavior
chart are secondary reinforcers because they only hold value when connected to some form of
reward.
Reinforcement Schedules
Strict behaviorists like Watson and Skinner focused on behavior while disregarding
cognition, with Skinner viewing the mind as a "black box," something completely
unknowable and, therefore, not worthy of study. In contrast, Edward C. Tolman
demonstrated that organisms can learn without immediate reinforcement, suggesting a
cognitive aspect to learning. His experiments with rats showed that they developed a
cognitive map (a mental representation of their environment) while exploring a maze
without rewards. Later, when food was introduced, these rats quickly navigated the maze,
illustrating latent learning (learning that occurs but is not immediately evident until there
is a reason to use it).
Observational Learning (Modeling)
In observational learning, we learn by watching and then imitating others, often referred to as models. This type of
learning is linked to specific neurons called mirror neurons, which are activated when we observe others performing
actions.
Humans and animals, including chimpanzees, are capable of observational learning. In a study, one group of chimpanzees
learned to drink juice more efficiently by observing another group use straws. The first group, which initially dipped their
straws, switched to sucking directly after watching the second group, demonstrating the effectiveness of modeling
behavior.
However, observational learning can have significant implications, as illustrated by Claire's experience with her son, Jay.
After struggling with his defiant behavior, Claire resorted to physical punishment. She later witnessed her younger
daughter, Anna, imitating this behavior with her teddy bear, leading Claire to realize the need for a different approach to
discipline. This example underscores the power of observational learning in shaping behavior, whether positively or
negatively.
Social Learning Theory
Like Tolman, whose experiments suggested a cognitive component to learning, psychologist Albert Bandura introduced a
different perspective with his social learning theory. This approach acknowledges the role of cognitive processes in
learning, arguing that pure behaviorism cannot explain how learning occurs without external reinforcement. Bandura
believed that internal mental states also play a crucial role and that observational learning is more complex than simple
imitation.
If the model was reinforced for their behavior, you are more likely to replicate it (known as vicarious
reinforcement). Conversely, if the model was punished, you are less inclined to imitate (known as
vicarious punishment).
Does Violent Media Cause Aggression?
The question of whether watching violent media or playing violent video games causes aggression
has been explored extensively. Albert Bandura's early studies indicated that television violence can
increase aggression in children, a finding that has been supported by more recent research. For
instance, studies conducted by Craig Anderson and colleagues suggest a causal relationship
between hours of exposure to violent media and subsequent aggressive thoughts and behaviors.
However, some researchers, such as Christopher Ferguson, argue that while a connection between
violent media exposure and aggression may exist, previous studies have often failed to consider
other significant risk factors, including mental health issues and family dynamics. Thus, while
violent media may contribute to aggressive behavior, it is essential to understand the broader
context of individual and environmental factors that also play a crucial role in aggression.
I often feel like a zookeeper
when I teach—there attention
swings from one topic to
another.