On Stress and Coping Mechanisms
On Stress and Coping Mechanisms
On Stress and Coping Mechanisms
To cite this article: Jean Tachè & Hans Selye (1985) On Stress and Coping Mechanisms, Issues
in Mental Health Nursing, 7:1-4, 3-24, DOI: 10.3109/01612848509009447
Jean Tache
Hans Selye
International Institute of Stress
Montreal, Quebec, Canada
This chapter was originally Chapter 1 in Volume 5 of this series, and the
figures are nuinbered accordingly.
3
Fig. 1-1. The typical triad of the alarm reaction. (a)
Adrenals. (b) Thymus. (c) A group of three lymph
nodes. (d) Inner surface of the stomach. On the left
are the organs of a normal rat, on the right, those
of a rat exposed to the frustrating psychological
4
ON STRESS A N D COPING MECHANISMS 5
7
8 J. TACHE AND H. SELYE
individual Differences
Although the word stressor usually refers to a stimulus, such as
an external insult, we must not forget that an effective stressor is
an agent perceived, psychologically or physiologically. What would
normally be an alarming situation, if unnoticed, would cause no
stress.
We often wonder why close friends show very different stress
manifestations when they experience a common stressful episode.
Why does one develop duodenal ulcers and another a cardiac
infarct, while a third is unaffected by the same difficulties that
overpowered the other people? We must recognize that each person
is unique and that, first, the very same stressor does not have the
same impact on all individuals and, second, even when it seems to
be felt with the same intensity, each person develops a unique set
of manifestations. Individual differences in reactions to stressors
are created by a combination of endogenous and exogenous
factors. The endogenous factors are generally inherited or acquired
traits: familial characteristics or diseases, proneness to certain types
of maladies, or weaknesses of certain organs. The exogenous
factors are usually various environmental conditions, including
social, intellectual, and psychological elements as well as climate,
physical surroundings, and nutrition.
Because every person is different, it is unlikely that a given
stimulus will be perceived in the same way by everybody. This is
obviously true when certain physical or chemical agents are
involved. For example, due t o exogenous and/or endogenous
factors, some individuals cannot cope with pollen, ragweed, dust,
and so on.
Stress Tests
Fig. 1-3. Dealing with stressors usually entails finding the right specific
response to the demand. Because endogenous and exogenous factors preside
over the stimulus-to-response process, assessment may be modified by
bringing a new outlook or by analyzing the situation from a different
viewpoint. The nonspecifically secreted hormones are also meant to play a
role in preparing for the specific response phase. With the human being’s
evolved neocortex and with the variety of specific responses now available,
however, these hormones may not be helpful in coping well with certain
aspects of the environment. Dotted arrows indicate possible feedback.
simplicity, these details are not included here, for they are not
essential for our discussion.
Stress (the nonspecific response of the body) is initiated at a
very early stage. In fact, nonspecific physiological adaptation starts
as soon as the body becomes aware of a demand, which could be
physiological as well as psychological, and this nonspecific response
grows with the magnitude of the demand. If for some reason
(internal conflict or lack of experience) the specific response is not
easy t o elicit, the stimulus-to-response process will be slowed down
or stopped as evaluation of the situation is prolonged and as
tentative, inadequate reactions are probed, thereby increasing
stress.
Nonspecific behavioral coping mechanisms may also be put into
action, such as distracting one’s attention from the problem or
orienting one’s attention t o a secondary problem that suddenly
seems t o become most important. In rats, mild tail pinching several
times a day leads t o immediate hyperphagia, and other behavior
patterns are also modified, for example, male infighting, sexual
intercourse, and maternal attention t o young pups are all increased
by this stress procedure (Rowland & Antelman, 1976). Normal
behavioral activity for this species becomes abnormal in that it
occurs more frequently. These responses are not agent specific
although they are goal oriented (and the goal is determined by the
environment ).
REFERENCES
Benson, H., Beary, J. F., & Carol, M. P. The relaxation response. Psychiatry,
1974, 37, 37-46.
Bloomfield, H. H., & Kory, R. B. Happiness: The TM program, psychiatry,
and enlightenment. New York: Simon & Schuster, Dawn Press, 1976.
Cannon, W. B. The wisdom o f the body. New York: Norton, 1939.
Collu, R., & JCquier, J. C. Pituitary response to auditory stress: Effect of
treatment with a-methyl-p-tyrosine. Usefulness of a factorial mixed design
for statistical analysis. Canadian Journal o f Physiology and Pharmacology,
1976, 5 4 , 594-602.
Dohrenwend, B. S., & Dohrenwend, B. P. (Eds.). Stressful life events: Their
nature and effects. New York: Wiley, 1974.
Elvidge, H., Challis, J. R. G., Robinson, J. S., Roper, C., & Thorburn, G. D.
Influence of handling and sedation on plasma cortisol in Rhesus monkeys
(Macaca mulatta). Journal o f Endocrinology, 1976, 70, 325-326.
Halkz, B., & Pupp, L. Hormone secretion of the anterior pituitary gland after
physical interruption of all nervous pathways to the hypophysiotrophic
area. Endocrinology, 1965, 77, 553-562.
Holmes, T. H., & Rahe, R. H. The social readjustment rating scale. Journal o f
Psychosomatic Research, 1967, 1 1 , 213-218.
Howard, J. H., Rechnitzer, P. A., & Cunningham, D. A. Coping with job
tension: Effective and ineffective methods. Public Personnel Management,
1 9 7 5 , 4, 317-325.
Lazarus, R. S . Psychological stress and coping in adaptation and illness.
International Journal o f Psychiatry and Medicine, 1974, 5 , 321-333.
Luthe, W. Autogenic therapy. New York: Grune & Stratton, 1969.
Mason, J. W. A re-evaluation of the concept of “non-specificity” in stress
theory. Journal of Psychiatry Research, 1971, 8 , 323-333.
Mason, J. W. Specificity in the organization of neuroendocrine response
profiles. In P. Seeman & G. M. Brown (Eds.), Frontiers in neurology and
neuroscience research. Toronto: University of Toronto, 1974, 68-80.
Mason, J. W. A historical view of the stress field. Journal of Humon Stress,
1975, 1 (l),6-12; 1 (2), 22-36.
Rowland, N. E., & Antelman, S. M. Stress-induced hyperphagia and obesity in
rats: A possible model for understanding human obesity. Science, 1976,
191, 310-312.
Salas, M., Tuchweber, B., Kourounakis, P., & Selye, H. Temperature-
dependence of stress-induced hepatic autophagy. Experientia, 1977, 33,
612-614.
Selye, H. Confusion and controversy in the stress field. Journal o f Human
Stress, 1975, 1(2), 37-44.
24 J. TACHE AND H.SELYE
Selye, H. The stress of life (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill, 1976. (a)
Selye, H. Stress in health and disease. Reading, Mass.: Butterworth, 1976. (b)
Spielberger, C. D. Trait-state anxiety and motor behavior. Journal of Motor
Behavior, 1971, 3 , 265-279.
Tach6, Y., Du Ruisseau, P., TachC, J., Selye, H., & Collu, R. Shift in
adenohypophyseal activity during chronic intermittent immobilization of
rats. Neuroendocrinology, 1976, 22, 325-336.