[1974]wittrock A Generative Model of Mathematics Learning
[1974]wittrock A Generative Model of Mathematics Learning
[1974]wittrock A Generative Model of Mathematics Learning
Author(s): M. C. Wittrock
Source: Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, Vol. 5, No. 4 (Nov., 1974), pp. 181-196
Published by: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/748845
Accessed: 13-03-2016 00:56 UTC
REFERENCES
Linked references are available on JSTOR for this article:
http://www.jstor.org/stable/748845?seq=1&cid=pdf-reference#references_tab_contents
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/
info/about/policies/terms.jsp
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content
in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship.
For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Journal
for Research in Mathematics Education.
http://www.jstor.org
This content downloaded from 160.36.178.25 on Sun, 13 Mar 2016 00:56:17 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
A GENERATIVE MODEL
OF MATHEMATICS LEARNING
M. C. WITTROCK
Introduction
meaning for mathematics education, this paper will begin with a hypothesis
about human learning that has been developed from the investigator's re-
then present a sample of the empirical studies that led to the generation of
the hypothesis. Finally, some of the meaning of this research for mathe-
The first point to emerge will be that we can be proud of the research
in when they learn mathematics; such as when they are adding, subtracting,
Analysis Center for Science, Mathematics, and Environmental Education and presented
at a session of the Special Interest Group for Research in Mathematics Education. The
session, held 28 February 1973, was in conjunction with the AERA Annual Meeting.
The paper was prepared pursuant to a contract with the Office of Education, U.S.
under government sponsorship are encouraged to express freely their judgment in pro-
fessional and technical matters. Points of view or opinions do not, therefore, necessarily
This content downloaded from 160.36.178.25 on Sun, 13 Mar 2016 00:56:17 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
dilemmas about nativism and the role of abstractions in memory, which
Plato and Aristotle wrote about. In the Meno, Socrates taught the slave boy
to prove that the diagonal of a square is equal to the side of a square twice
the area of the given square. To Plato, it seemed that the abstractions were
ticulars. He made sensory data and the particulars the focus of human
learning. The hypothesis and data presented here may appear to raise
again this ancient dilemma. The reader is left to decide whether or not
instruction causes the learner to do. Effective instruction causes the learner
perience.
tions. It is better to look beyond the nominal stimuli to the functional stimuli
they become for the learner. In the past, the importance of the environ-
ment and instruction on the learner has been emphasized. New stress must
From this point of view, there is no one best method of teaching all
students, although there may be one best logical organization of the sub-
learner's previous knowledge and upon what the instruction causes the
matter he is taught.
This content downloaded from 160.36.178.25 on Sun, 13 Mar 2016 00:56:17 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Demonstrations of learning as a generative process
strations, one perceptual and one verbal. For the first demonstration, look
at the black irregularly shaped objects in Figure 1. What does one see?
AA V IA
Fig. 1. Look at the black objects above. What are they? (adapted from W. Brown
One probably sees only black irregularly shaped objects because previous
and also because one was instructed to process the information as black
as background and the white as foreground. One will see the word PLAY
in capital letters. The point here is that the way in which one processes
it. The reader might reflect on this example the next time his students
don't get the point. They may be generating black objects instead of words.
For a demonstration from the verbal area, read the paragraph in Fig-
ure 2 and generate a title for it. The words seem to apply to many tasks,
The procedure is actually quite simple. First you arrange things into differ-
ent groups depending on their makeup. Of course, one pile may be sufficient
depending on how much there is to do. If you have to go somewhere else due
to lack of facilities that is the next step, otherwise you are pretty well set. It
too few things at once than too many. In the short run this may not seem
important, but complications from doing too many can easily arise. A mistake
whole procedure will seem complicated. Soon, however, it will become just
another facet of life. It is difficult to foresee any end to the necessity for this
task in the immediate future, but then one never can tell. [Bransford &
Johnson, 1972.]
Fig. 2
This content downloaded from 160.36.178.25 on Sun, 13 Mar 2016 00:56:17 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
but identifying a specific title is difficult for most readers. The paragraph
seems very vague-until one is told that the title of the passage is "Washing
Clothes." The title provides the cue that allows one to relate the vague
processes.
Recent Research
The recent research is divided into two sections: (a) structural organiza-
Structural organization
The example given above, which dealt with the washing of clothes, was
1972 issue of the Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. They
found (p. 723) that giving the title for the story before the story was read
greatly enhanced its comprehension and recall, while giving the title after
the story was read did not increase comprehension or retention. The
The digits of each string and their order remained the same across the
strings, while the grouping changed from the first to the second string.
The results showed that altering the organization of the string did not
improve learning across altered strings, although the digits and their order
tion, it seems that a new task is presented to the learner. In fact, proactive
interference could have been predicted in this task, based on the similarity
of the digits across the strings. Because proactive interference among digits
did not occur, it could mean that the groups of digits are treated as the
This content downloaded from 160.36.178.25 on Sun, 13 Mar 2016 00:56:17 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
hypothesis presented above, it was predicted that the semantic properties
of the words would have decided effects on story comprehension and reten-
TABLE 1
Test of Test of
Reading
High 73 48 67 47 58 36
Middle 74 47 57 29 53 31
Low 71 45 61 36 55 20
51 29
(14) (14)
ing of 20% of the words in the story from less-frequent ones (e.g., lad)
One implication is that a few unfamiliar terms may have a similar decided
"discover" the meanings of new words without defining them for him.
From Table 2 one can see the results from this research study, using
commonly available teaching materials. All children read the same stories
one that used highly meaningful words, and then read the same story but
with many new unfamiliar synonyms that did not change the meaning of
the story. The control group read the version with the low-meaningful
words twice. On the test of definitions of unfamiliar words, the group that
read the familiar version of the story first did much better than the control
group.
No words were ever defined for either group. Neither were any of the
This content downloaded from 160.36.178.25 on Sun, 13 Mar 2016 00:56:17 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
TABLE 2
Reading Listening
Reading
Levels Tests 1) High Freq. 2) Low Freq. 3) High Freq. 4) Low Freq.
The idea of a familiar organization can also be used to teach the definitions
the research of David Ausubel, who pioneered in this area with his books
work in this area with his classical research on the importance of proactive
approaches are capable of handling the data of these studies. In fact, when
these two approaches were put to an explicit test in a study dealing with
The controversy that Plato and Aristotle began over the roles of ab-
stractions and particulars in learning has not yet been resolved. No end
to it is in sight.
Processing of information
Under this heading are the research studies in which learners generate
groups or other large units, such as stories, sentences, and images, from the
stimuli they have been given. The results of these studies occasionally are
dramatic.
For example, Bower and Clark (1969) gave subjects 12 different lists
of 10 unrelated nouns. One group of subjects was asked to learn each list
in whatever manner each person wished, keeping the order of the words
the same. The experimental group was asked to make a meaningful story
from each list. The results need little elaboration. Statistical tests are not
This content downloaded from 160.36.178.25 on Sun, 13 Mar 2016 00:56:17 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
necessary either, because the mean gain from the control to the experi-
and facilitated the retention of the words and their serial order.
1970) have found that processing words into a sentence also facilitates
will not be discussed any further here, because of the apparent similarity
(1971) and others have recently studied extensively. Bull and Wittrock
90 fifth graders, using three learning conditions. The instructions for the
groups differed as follows: (a) generate (draw) an image of the word and
its definition; (b) trace a picture (image given) representing the word
and its definition; or (c) learn the verbal definition by copying it. The
group means for retention of the definition ranked from high to low in
the order given above. The group that generated their own images re-
membered the definitions best one week later. There was no statistically
significant difference between the other two groups. Again, the generative
processing hypothesis fits the data. It does not seem that the mode of
To test the latter possibility, Wittrock and Goldberg (in press) com-
bined into one study the above three different types of generative processes
commonly studied in the literature: (a) imagery, (b) stories, and (c)
The words varied in meaningfulness and in imagery value. The results are
the three generative processes, although the control group did less well
than the other three groups. However, we did find that the imagery and
Wittrock and Goldberg (in press) have replicated this study with
public school children. They found that verbal processing conditions, sen-
need to be primed more than the verbal ones. This issue is still being
This content downloaded from 160.36.178.25 on Sun, 13 Mar 2016 00:56:17 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
6
IMM 5.29
5.03
4.86
S IHML 5.10
TIMI 4.60
4 - IM 3.81
83.81
3.44 3.78
Instructional Sets
effects on retention.
ALUMINUM
This content downloaded from 160.36.178.25 on Sun, 13 Mar 2016 00:56:17 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
randomly ordered hierarchy for the concept of minerals. This hierarchy
can be rearranged into a properly ordered one, with the word "minerals"
MINERALS
GRASSHOPPER
This content downloaded from 160.36.178.25 on Sun, 13 Mar 2016 00:56:17 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
We gave these hierarchies to college subjects under two different proc-
essing conditions. In the first, or control condition, the learners were asked
"made sense."
The interesting results are found in Figure 7. With the rote processing
22 21.9 Generative
21.4 Processing
20-
S18-
16.9 Reproductive
16 C Processing
14
13.1
12 -
11.2
E 10
S8
6 6.1
4U
Hierarchical Organization
essing had the most marked effect when the organization was in the learn-
ers' repertoires, but not made explicit, as in the randomly ordered hier-
two types of factors as has been done in the outline of this paper. This
learning.
tion. It was this study that led to the conclusion that learning with
This content downloaded from 160.36.178.25 on Sun, 13 Mar 2016 00:56:17 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
The above line of research gives a new understanding of what is involved
or not the organization of the content is made explicit does not seem to
Individual differences
way to develop ATI research is to study the relevant processes that the
these processes and the development of protocols and specific tests for
Also, several interesting ATI studies on learners' abilities and verbal proc-
great promise.
Brain research
may tell something about how right or left hemispheres process informa-
tion differently from each other. This research may also tell how spatial
in the brain, especially after the age of five years. Some of the categories
in the different studies whose findings are the basis for Figure 8.
From such findings it seems that the brain has at least two modes of
This content downloaded from 160.36.178.25 on Sun, 13 Mar 2016 00:56:17 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Left Hemisphere Right Hemisphere
verbal perceptual
symbolic visiospatial
(Gestalt perception)
linear nonlinear
Higher-order processes
possible. The answer is also yes if one accepts the following data.
from the set of testable hypotheses. In that study, the children learned and
Motivation
effect.
being reinforced. If the student infers that his effort at learning mathe-
This content downloaded from 160.36.178.25 on Sun, 13 Mar 2016 00:56:17 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
is attributable to factors over which he has no control, such as his innate
assume his failure to learn is due to factors beyond his control, when addi-
tional effort would solve the mathematics problem and increase motiva-
Delay-retention effect
For many years, it has been believed that reinforcers should be given
chology have been taught those "truths" about using reinforcement and
& Yonge, 1969) indicate that there is now reason to doubt the impor-
tance of immediate feedback. One reason for the doubt is that immediate
processes.
determine, in part at least, what the effects of the reinforcer are and what
they will learn and remember. The research summarized above means
From the research and theory developed above, implications for teach-
tion for writing this section, many articles in issues of the JOURNAL FOR
of the excellent mathematics curricula that are now being used throughout
the public schools of the United States, such as the SMSG materials pre-
education are delving into the same issues that have been discussed pre-
The two implications for mathematics learning and teaching that will
This content downloaded from 160.36.178.25 on Sun, 13 Mar 2016 00:56:17 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
the carefully organized instructional materials prepared by SMSG and
around its most fundamental concepts and the teaching of them first to
with a statement such as "Let x = 2y," he should consider what his state-
ments will cause the learner to do. What is the purpose of letting x = 2y?
What is the learner to generate from the equation? How does the equation
focus on the details, but it is through the study of these specific proc-
Socrates taught the slave boy to construct a proof. The boy's thought
processes were engaged in the problem, even though Socrates closely di-
style of teaching for the very reason the author likes it. The instruc-
tion left some room for thought within an organized structure. It left
generate.
one cannot assume that students are at the same level for storing and
The author's research on human learning indicates that the ways teachers
introduce new material, the ways they relate it to the student's experi-
ences, and the ways they stimulate the student to generate meanings are
This content downloaded from 160.36.178.25 on Sun, 13 Mar 2016 00:56:17 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
crucially important to learning. With proper attention to the introduction
not involved in the research on the generative hypothesis. But the author's
research supports this hypothesis that the learner must actively construct
about human learning, Plato's ideas about teaching are still very much
REFERENCES
Aiken, L. R., Jr. Verbal factors and mathematics learning: a review of research.
Bogen, J. E. The other side of the brain II: an appositional mind. Bulletin of the
Bower, G. H., & Clark, M. C. Narrative stories as mediators for serial learning.
Bower, G. H., Clark, M. C., Lesgold, A. M., & Winzenz, D. Hierarchical retrieval
schemes in recall of categorized word lists. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal
Bower, G. H., & Winzenz, D. Group structure, coding, and memory for digit series.
Journal of Experimental Psychology Monographs, 1969, 80, (2, Pt. 2), 1-17.
Bull, B. L., & Wittrock, M. C. Imagery in the learning of verbal definitions. British
1151.
Marks, C. B., Doctorow, M. J., & Wittrock, M. C. Word frequency and reading
This content downloaded from 160.36.178.25 on Sun, 13 Mar 2016 00:56:17 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
O'Day, E. F., Kulhavy, R. W., Anderson, W., & Malczynski, R. J. Programmed
Paivio, A. Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston,
1971.
Rohwer, W. D., Jr. Children and adolescents: Should we teach them or let them
Rossi, Sheila & Wittrock, M. C. Clustering versus serial ordering in recall among
Rossi, Sheila & Wittrock, M. C. Developmental shifts in verbal recall between mental
Sassenrath, J. M., & Yonge, G. D. Effects of delayed information feedback and feed-
back cues in learning and retention. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1969, 60,
174-177.
196.
Press, 1972.
Weiner, B., Heckenhausen, H., Meyer, W. U., & Cook, R. E. Causal ascriptions and
Wittrock, M. C., & Goldberg, S. M. Imagery and meaningfulness in free recall: Word
Wittrock, M. C., Doctorow, M. J., & Marks, C. B. Story meaningfulness and com-
Wittrock).
Statement of ownership, management, and circulation (Act of August 12, 1970: Section 3685. Title 39.
United States Code). (1) Title of publication: The JOURNAL FOR RESEARCH IN MATHEMATICS
EDUCATION. (2) Date of filing: 1 October 1974. (3) Frequency of issue: Four times yearly-November,
January, March, and May. (4) Location of known office of publication: 1906 Association Drive, Reston,
Virginia 22091. (5) Location of the headquarters or general business offices of the publishers: Same as #4.
(6) Names and addresses of publisher, editor, and managing editor: Publisher, National Council of Teachers
of Mathematics, same as #4; Editor, J. F. Weaver, University of Wisconsin, Madison, Wisconsin 53706;
Manager Editor, none. (7) Owner: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, same as #4. (8) Known
bondholders, mortgagees, and other security holders owning or holding 1 percent or more of total amount
of bonds, mortgages, or other securities: none. (10) The purpose, function, and nonprofit status of this
organization and the exempt status for federal income tax purposes have not changed during preceding
12 months. (11) Extent and nature of circulation. Average no. copies each issue during preceding 12 months:
A. Total no. copies printed, 5,551; B. 1. Paid circulation sales through dealers and carriers, street vendors,
and counter sales, none; B. 2. Paid circulation, mail subscriptions, 5,159; C. Total paid circulation, 5,159;
D. Free distribution by mail, carrier, or other means, none; E. Total distribution, 5,159; F. Office use,
left-over, unaccounted, spoiled after printing, 392; G. Total, 5,551. Actual no. copies of single issue pub-
lished nearest to filing date: A. Total no. copies printed, May 1974, 5,424; B. 1. Paid circulation, sales
through dealers and carriers, street vendors, and counter sales, none; B. 2. Paid circulation, mail sub-
scriptions, 5,076; C. Total paid circulation, 5,076; D. Free distribution by mail, carrier, or other means,
none; E. Total distribution, 5,076; F. Office use, left-over, unaccounted, spoiled after printing, 348; G.
Total, 5,424. I certify that the statements made by me above are correct and complete: James D. Gates,
Business Manager.
This content downloaded from 160.36.178.25 on Sun, 13 Mar 2016 00:56:17 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions