Marko Prpić, MA
University of Zagreb, Faculty of Education and Rehabilitation Sciences
prpic.marcus@gmail.com
Robert Peacock, PhD
Florida international University, Department of Criminology and Criminal Justice
rpeacock@fiu.edu
THE POLICE CODE OF SILENCE
IN A COMPARATIVE PERSPECTIVE
By
Marko Prpic
University of Zagreb
and
Robert Peacock
Michigan State University
THE POLICE CODE OF SILENCE
IN A COMPARATIVE PERSPECTIVE
Abstract
The purpose of this paper is to determine the degree to which the code of silence is
a uniform phenomenon, spanning across different countries and police cultures.
Samples of police officers from Croatia (1,142 respondents), South Africa (871
respondents), and the United States (664 respondents) evaluated 11 scenarios
describing various forms of police misconduct. The results indicate that, in all 11
examined scenarios, there is a statistically significant correlation between
unwillingness to report misconduct at work and the country of origin of the
examined police officers. Own perception of misconduct and estimates of others’
willingness to report misconduct are significant predictors of the respondents’ own
willingness to report misconduct at work.
Introduction
The code of silence is the unwritten rule according to which police officers never provide
incriminating information about their colleagues and, as such, presents a huge obstacle in the
effective control of misconduct at work (Mollen Commission, 1994). Since William Westley
(1970) first profiled the police profession in his 1950s study of the Gary, Indiana Police
Department, the code of silence has been viewed as a persistent outcome in the socialization of
police. Westley (1970) and Bittner (1971) made the case that police work was dangerous and
officers were socialized to trust only their peers and protect other officers through the “blue
wall of silence.” This implicit code has likely existed nearly as long as law enforcement itself,
but substantial scholarship on the blue wall of silence did not materialize until the 1990s over
a period when the code was increasingly viewed as a leading hurdle to law enforcement reform
in both developed and developing countries.
Two public inquiries located at opposite ends of the globe were symbolic starting points for this
newly heightened concern for the obstacles to addressing police misconduct in the 1990s. The
Mollen Commission (1994, 53) found that the code of silence was pervasive throughout the
New York Police Department and was the “most significant barrier to effective corruption
control;” while the Wood Inquiry in Australia argued that the code of silence was an
“incontrovertible and universal product of police culture” that was preventing reform in the
New South Wales Police Service (Wood, 1997, 33). Over the same period, scholars and
1
practitioners were finding similar concerns in their studies of twenty-nine former communist
countries in Central and Eastern Europe that sought to transition from the secretive world of
security-focused militias (milicija) to become modern, democratic police agencies.
The parameters of the stated code vary among the police organizations in different countries
and even within a singular country or police administration. (Klockars et al., 2000). This is
especially true of police in transitional countries, such as Croatia and South Africa. Although
the code of silence problem is notably present in such countries, as a subject of serious empirical
research, it appears most often in stable democratic countries. This paper provides a
comparative approach to the code of silence in the police from three vastly different countries.
The paper examines two transitional and specific countries—Croatia and South Africa—
compared to the USA. The USA, a country with a historically stable democracy, is the reference
point for cross-country comparisons. On the other hand, Croatia is a transitional, as well as
post-conflict country, characterized by a high level of corruption. Like Croatia, South Africa is
a transitional country facing the legacy of apartheid, as well as a high corruption rate and
challenges in the protection of civil rights.
Literature Review
In terms of scholarship on the role of the code of silence in these agencies, a critical turning
point was the July 1996 National Symposium on Police Integrity in the United States that
gathered leading scholars and police departments to find new solutions to the “old problem” of
police misconduct (Greene, Piquero, Hickman & Lawson, 2004). The symposium planners
purposely chose to depart from the past singular concern for individual cases of corruption or
misconduct and instead emphasize a new research focus based around the goal of a healthy law
enforcement organization and work culture (Klockars, Kutnjak Ivković, and Haberfeld, 2006).
This symposium was the starting point of several large research studies on police integrity
including the first large-scale tests of what would become known as the Integrity Survey.
Klockars and Kutnjak Ivković (2004) designed the theory of police integrity and the Integrity
Survey to ameliorate the problems experienced in past studies asking direct questions about
police misconduct and the respondents’ involvement in such behavior. This survey would
instead measure integrity through police officers’ attitudes, perceptions, and self-reports on how
they would respond in hypothetical situations describing integrity challenges. Besides US tests
of the integrity instrument, the survey was soon adapted by scholars across the globe including
2
the first international use of the instrument in Croatia (Kutnjak Ivković, & Klockars, 1998).
Since the turn of this century, researchers have used the Integrity Survey to assess the health of
police agencies across the globe in more than 24 countries including Armenia, Australia,
Austria, Bosnia and Herzegovina, Canada, Croatia, Czech Republic, Finland, Hungary, Japan,
Malaysia, the Netherlands, Norway, Pakistan, Poland, Romania, Slovenia, South Africa, South
Korea, Sweden, Thailand, Turkey, the UK, and the USA.
Klockars and colleagues (see, e.g., Klockars and Kutnjak Ivković, 2004; Klockars et al., 1997;
Klockars, Kutnjak Ivković, and Haberfeld, 2004a) developed the Integrity Survey as part of a
theory of police integrity that incorporated the code of silence as a key dimension of the theory.
The survey measured the code of silence through questions on the respondents' willingness to
report a wide variety of police misconduct (e.g., Klockars et al., 2000; Pagon and Lobnikar,
2000; Micuccui and Gomme, 2005; Klockars, Kutnjak Ivković, and Haberfeld, 2006; Marche,
2009; Rothwell and Baldwin, 2007a, 2007b; Schafer and Martinelli, 2008; Kutnjak Ivković and
Shelley, 2008; Wolfe and Piquero, 2011; Long, Cross, Shelley, and Kutnjak Ivković, 2013;
Kutnjak Ivković, Haberfeld, and Peacock, 2013; Kutnjak Ivković and Sauerman, 2013).
In their first survey of 3,250 police officers from 30 US local police agencies, Klockars and
colleagues (2000) showed that the contours of the code vary greatly with different forms of
police corruption described in the questionnaire. The results showed that there are also major
differences in the contours of the code not only across scenarios, but also across police agencies
(Klockars et al., 2000). Even when Klockars and colleagues used the police integrity
questionnaire to survey police officers in three agencies of integrity (Klockars, Kutnjak Ivković,
and Haberfeld, 2006), they found that officers in one agency adhered to the code much more
than the officers in the others. The results of other US studies provided additional evidence of
the diverse adherence to the code of silence across different police agencies (e.g., Marche, 2009;
Micuccui and Gomme, 2005; Rothwell and Baldwin, 2007a, 2007b; Schafer and Martinelli,
2008; Wolfe and Piquero, 2011) and even within police agencies (e.g., Greene at al., 2004).
The most consistent observation across studies has been finding that the code of silence was
negatively related to the perceived seriousness of misconduct (Klockars et al., 1997, 2000;
Kutnjak Ivković, Haberfeld, and Peacock, 2013; Kutnjak Ivković, Haberfeld, and Peacock,
2015; Kutnjak Ivković and Sauerman, 2013; Long, Cross, Shelley, and Kutnjak Ivković, 2013).
Other studies suggest that the code of silence may vary by officer rank (e.g., Huon et al, 1995;
Weisburd and Greenspan, 2000). When there are differences, the supervisor code of silence
3
usually was found to be narrower than the line officer code (e.g., Huon et al., 1995; Klockars,
Kutnjak Ivković, and Haberfeld, 2006; Kremer, 2000; Kutnjak Ivković and Klockars, 1998;
Kutnjak Ivković and Shelley, 2008; Pagon and Lobnikar, 2000; Weisburd and Greenspan,
2000).
Comparative studies of the code of silence are rare. In the first such study, Klockars, Kutnjak
Ivković and Haberfeld (2004) analyzed the code in 12 countries, and found significant
differences among countries. They wrote that, in Croatia, Hungary, Pakistan, Poland, and South
Africa, the code of silence is so strong that officers are actually estimating just how unwilling
most officers are to report the misconduct described in the scenarios (Klockars, Kutnjak
Ivković, Haberfeld, 2004).
More recent comparative research conducted in ten countries (Armenia, Australia, Croatia,
Estonia, Russia, Slovenia, South Africa, South Korea, Thailand, USA) indicates that, according
to the results, the countries could be divided into four categories (Kutnjak Ivković, Haberfeld,
2015). The first category consists of Australia, Estonia, Slovenia, South Africa and the USA,
whose mean values are “on the reporting side of scale” (Kutnjak Ivković, Haberfeld, 2015).
The second category involves Armenia, Croatia and Thailand, whose mean values are also on
the reporting side of scale, but are somewhat lower and closer to the midpoint of the scale. This
would imply that codes in Armenia, Croatia and Thailand seem to be somewhat stronger than
the codes in Australia, Estonia, Slovenia, South Africa and the USA. The results also imply that
the codes are somewhat stronger in the third category of countries (South Korea). The fourth
category shows that the code of silence is by far the strongest in Russia, whose mean values in
all scenarios are predominantly grouped on the nonreporting side and the midpoint of the scale
(Kutnjak Ivković, Haberfeld, 2015).
The Code of Silence and Disciplinary Fairness
In the 1990s, Kutnjak Ivković and Klockars (1996, 1998) sought to shed greater light on the
code of silence by testing three theoretical models for a potential relationship between the code
of silence and disciplinary fairness. Specifically, they proposed the discipline indifference
model, the simple deterrence model, and the simple justice model.
The discipline indifference model does not expect a relationship between the code and
disciplinary fairness (Kutnjak Ivković and Klockars, 1998). This model assumes that officers
consider other factors that interfere with their weighing of discipline fairness and the code of
4
silence. For example, if a police agency rarely disciplines for misconduct, police officers will
accurately perceive that the certainty of discipline is low and the severity of the discipline would
be considered less of a risk. In work cultures with a strong code of silence, the threat of
discipline, particularly if exercised sporadically, may do very little to change the police officers'
intentions. Therefore, the discipline indifference model assumes that the percentage of police
officers willing to report misconduct would be unchanged regardless of differences in their
perceptions on whether the discipline is too harsh, fair, or too lenient.
The simple deterrence model expects that the code of silence and discipline severity are
negatively related. The harsher the discipline, the less likely police officers will participate in
the code of silence (Kutnjak Ivković and Klockars, 1998). This is a simplified model because
it takes into account only the severity of discipline and disregards the certainty and celerity of
discipline. Thus, the prediction of the simple deterrence model is that the percentage of police
officers who adhere to the code of silence is the lowest for the officers who perceive discipline
to be too harsh and the highest for the officers who perceive discipline to be too lenient.
The last of the three proposed models is a simple justice model that predicts that police officers'
motivation to report misconduct is linked with their desire to see misconduct disciplined
appropriately or justly (Kutnjak Ivković and Klockars, 1998). Therefore, this model expects
that police officers who perceive discipline as too harsh will stick to the code of silence.
However, the model is silent about the relation between those who view discipline as fair and
those who view discipline as too lenient. Kutnjak Ivković and Klockars (1998, p. 335) argued
that: "The motive of simple justice would offer no reason for increased reporting of misconduct
under conditions of excessive leniency.” Thus, the prediction of the simple justice model is that
the percentage of police officers who adhere to the code of silence is higher for the officers who
perceive discipline as too harsh than for officers who perceive discipline as fair. At the same
time, there is no prediction for the relation among police officers who perceive discipline as
fair or too lenient.
The three proposed models for evaluating the code of silence were first tested on Croatian police
(Kutnjak Ivković and Klockars, 1998) and followed up on with samples from Bosnia and
Herzegovina, the Czech Republic (Kutnjak Ivković & Shelley, 2005; 2010), and the United
States (Long, Cross, Shelley, and Kutnjak Ivković, 2013). These studies on the code of silence
were performed using the first version of the integrity survey that was generally focused on
corrupt acts, but scholars have increasingly turned to a second version of the integrity survey
5
that sought judgments on a broad spectrum of misconduct spanning code of conduct violations
to excessive force incidents (see Klockars and Kutnjak Ivković, 2004). Kutnjak Ivković and
Sauerman (2013) were the first to test the three models on a South African police survey.
The cross-cultural comparisons of the results of tests of the three models do not suggest a
uniform picture for the relation of the code and disciplinary fairness across respondents who
evaluated discipline as too harsh and those who evaluated it as fair. In Croatia (Kutnjak Ivković
and Klockars, 1998) and the Czech Republic (Kutnjak Ivković and Shelley, 2010) the simple
justice model was largely supported (10 out of 11 scenarios in the Croatian study fit the model;
10 out of 10 scenarios for supervisors and 7 out of 10 scenarios for non-supervisors in the Czech
study). The simple model found much weaker support in Bosnia and Herzegovina (4 out of 10
scenarios fit the model; Kutnjak Ivković and Shelley, 2005), the United States (2 out of 4
scenarios fit the model; Long, Cross, Shelley, and Kutnjak Ivković, 2013), and South Africa (6
out of 11 scenarios fit the model; Kutnjak Ivković and Sauerman, 2013).
The results were more uniform when the relation between the code and disciplinary fairness
were compared for the respondents who evaluated discipline as fair and those who evaluated
discipline as too lenient. In all four studies (Kutnjak Ivković and Klockars, 1998; Kutnjak
Ivković and Sauerman, 2013; Kutnjak Ivković and Shelley, 2005, 2010) the results suggest that
most of the scenarios fit the discipline indifference model (8 out of 11 in Croatia, Kutnjak
Ivković and Klockars, 1998; 7 out of 10 for the Czech respondents, Kutnjak Ivković and
Shelley, 2010; 8 out of 10 for the Bosnian respondents, Kutnjak Ivković and Shelley, 2005; and
10 out of 11 for the South African respondents, Kutnjak Ivković and Sauerman, 2013). In the
few cases in which differences were found among those judging discipline as fair and too
lenient, the findings mostly suggested the simple deterrence model (e.g., Kutnjak Ivković and
Sauerman, 2013; Kutnjak Ivković and Shelley, 2005), the simple justice model (e.g., Kutnjak
Ivković and Klockars, 1998), or both (Kutnjak Ivković and Shelley, 2010).
Methodology
Klockars and Kutnjak Ivković (2004) developed the theory of police integrity and the
accompanying methodology (e.g., Klockars and Kutnjak Ivković, 2004; Klockars, Kutnjak
Ivković, and Haberfeld, 2006). One dimension of the theory focuses on various techniques used
by police administrations to detect and research misconduct in the agency (Klockars, Kutnjak
Ivković, 2004).
6
The theoretical approach is combined with a new methodology that facilitates the examination
of police integrity as an organizational concept. It is precisely this organizational/occupational
approach to the study of police integrity that involves questions of fact and opinion that can be
explored directly, without arousing the resistance that direct inquiries about corrupt behavior
are likely to provoke. With this approach, it is possible to ask nonthreatening questions about
officers' knowledge of agency rules and their opinions about the seriousness of particular
violations.
Although it is possible to use an administrative/individual approach to measure corrupt
behavior, the methodological obstacles are significant. Using the organizational/occupational
approach, how seriously officers regard misconduct, how amiable they are to supporting
discipline, and how wiling they are to tolerate misconduct in silence can easily be measured.
The police integrity questionnaire contains 11 hypothetical scenarios describing a range of
police misconduct. Four scenarios describe the use of excessive force, ranging from verbal
abuse to the abuse of deadly force (Scenario 4, Scenario 6, Scenario 7, and Scenario 11).
Another two scenarios describe falsification of an official report (Scenario 10) and failure to
execute a felony warrant (Scenario 2). Five scenarios include examples of police corruption
(Scenario 1, Scenario 3, Scenario 5, Scenario 8, and Scenario 9).
Every scenario is accompanied by a series of seven questions, each measuring the normative
reflections of police integrity. These questions focus on factual issues and opinions, exploring
the respondents' views of misconduct seriousness, appropriate and expected discipline, and
willingness to report misconduct.
The dependent variables in our study are based on questions asking the respondents to assess
whether they would report misconduct (“Do you think YOU would report a fellow police officer
who engaged in this behavior”). Possible answers were provided in the form of a Likert scale
ranging from 1 to 5, where 1 signifies “definitely not”, and 5 “definitely yes”. Later, for the
requirements of bivariate analysis, the own reporting variable was recoded into two categories:
“would not report” and “would report”.
The independent variable in the study most relevant for our topic is the country itself. The
created independent variable included three countries, the USA, Croatia and South Africa. In
addition, we have included several organizational variables.
7
First, we measured how serious police officers consider the respective behaviors (“How serious
do YOU consider this behavior to be?”). Possible answers ranged on a Likert scale from 1 to 5,
with 1 signifying “not at all serious”, and 5 “very serious.” For the purpose of the bivariate and
regression analyses, the variable was dichotomized into “would not report” and “would report”.
Second, we measured the respondents’ estimates of actual discipline (“If an officer in your
agency engaged in this behavior and was discovered doing so, what, if any, discipline do YOU
think WOULD follow?”). The answers offered to the respondents depended upon the legal
norms in each country. In the United States, the options were: “none”, “verbal reprimand”,
“written reprimand”, “period of suspension without pay”, “demotion in rank” and “dismissal”
(Kutnjak Ivković et al., 2015). In Croatia, the options were: “written reprimand”, “fine up to
10% of salary”, “fine up to 20% of salary”, “no promotion”, “no career advancement”,
“reassignment to a lower position” and “dismissal” (Kutnjak Ivković, 2015). In South Africa,
these options were: “none”, “verbal reprimand”, “written reprimand”, “reassignment to a
different position”, “demotion in rank” and “dismissal” (Kutnjak Ivković and Sauerman, 2015).
In each country, the ends of the scale (“none” and “dismissal”) are the same. We coded all other
possible answers as one category (“some discipline other than dismissal”). Because few
respondents selected either “none” or “dismissal” in some scenarios, we had to drop a category
in these scenarios (e.g., in scenarios 1 and 7, the category “dismissal” was omitted; in scenarios
2, 3, 4, 9 and 10, the category “none” was omitted).
Third, to assess the perceptions of fairness, we have created the variable discipline fairness for
each scenario by subtracting the value for the expected discipline from the value for the
appropriate discipline. When the result was zero, we assumed that the respondents evaluated
expected discipline as fair. When the result was negative, we assumed that the respondents
evaluated expected discipline as too harsh. When the result was positive, we assumed that the
respondents evaluated expected discipline as too lenient.
Fourth, we measured the respondents’ views about colleagues’ willingness to report
unacceptable behavior (“Do you think MOST POLICE OFFICERS IN YOUR AGENCY
would report a fellow police officer who engaged in this behavior?”). Possible answers were
ranged on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 to 5, where 1 represented “definitely not”, and 5
“definitely yes”. With the purpose of later analyses, the variable was dichotomized into “would
not report” and “would report”.
Finally, our analyses include demographic variables as well.
8
First, the work experience was measured as the length of service in the police (“How many
years have you been a police officer?”). Possible answers were: “less than 1 year”, “1-2 years”,
“3-5 years”, “6-10 years”, “11-15 years”, “16-20 years”, and “over 20 years”. For the purposes
of further analyses, the answers were recoded into: “up to 5 years”, “6-15 years” and “over 15
years”.
Second, we also included a measure of the respondent's supervisory status. The answers were
“yes” and “no.”
The purpose of the paper is to determine the degree to which the code of silence is a uniform
phenomenon spanning across different countries and police cultures. In particular, we would
like to explore a connection between readiness to report and country of origin. Hence, our null
hypothesis is that there is no statistically significant relation between the code of silence and
the country of origin.
Sample
As there are 17,985 independent police organizations operating in the United States (BJS,
2011), it would be difficult to measure police integrity across the entire population (Kutnjak
Ivković et al., 2015). Instead of collecting data from the entire population, Kutnjak Ivković and
colleagues (2015) collected the data in 11 police agencies which generally represent the range
of police employees in large, medium and small cities in the USA. The potential respondents
were notified by email, and 37.4% of them participated in the study (Kutnjak Ivković et al.,
2015).
9
Table 1: USA respondents' demographic characteristics
Number of respondents
Percent of respondents
445
168
72.6%
27.4%
91
113
298
366
59
14.6%
12.3%
32.0%
39.5%
6.4%
314
94
21
74
66
55.2%
16.5%
3.7%
13.0%
11.6%
Supervisory role
Non-supervisor
Supervisors
Length of service
Up to 5 years
6-10 years
11-15 years
16-20 years
Over 20 years
Type of assignment
Patrol
Detective/investigative
Community policing officer
Special (vice, juvenile, etc.)
Administrative
The South African sample (Kutnjak Ivković and Sauerman, 2015) consisted of 871 police
officers, surveyed in 2010-2012. The response rate was 87.5% (Kutnjak Ivković, Sauerman,
2015). The sample includes police officers from three different police agencies, across the
South African provinces.
10
Table 2: South African respondents' demographic characteristics
Supervisory role
Non-supervisor
Supervisors
Length of service
Up to 5 years
6-10 years
11-15 years
16-20 years
Over 20 years
Type of assignment
Patrol
Detective/investigative
Communications
Traffic
Administrative
Community policing
Other
Gender
Male
Female
Number of respondents
Percent of respondents
544
324
62.5%
37.5%
157
230
72
117
295
18.0%
26.4%
8.3%
13.4%
33.9%
243
298
50
36
112
65
76
27.9%
33.2%
5.7%
4.1%
12.9%
7.5%
8.7%
611
254
70.8%
29.2%
The Croatian sample was a stratified sample of 996 respondents (Kutnjak Ivković, 2015). The
sample was selected to represent 20 police administrations (four categories) and about 200
police stations (three categories). The authors created a 4 × 3 table by cross-tabulating four
categories of police administrations with three categories of police stations (Kutnjak Ivković,
2015). The response rate was 88% (Kutnjak Ivković, 2015).
11
Table 3: Croatian respondents' demographic characteristics
Number of respondents
Supervisory role
Non-supervisor
Supervisors
Length of service
Up to 5 years
6-10 years
11-15 years
16-20 years
Over 20 years
Type of assignment
Patrol
Detective/investigative
Communications
Traffic
Administrative
Community policing
Other
Gender
Male
Female
Percent of respondents
742
175
81.0%
19.0%
113
91
298
366
59
12.3%
9.8%
32.0%
39.5%
6.4%
335
162
53
47
17
96
197
36.9%
17.9%
5.8%
5.2%
1.8%
10.6%
21.8%
809
107
88.2%
11.8%
The Croatian sample has the highest percentage of non-supervisors (81%; Kutnjak Ivković,
2015), followed by the U.S. sample (72.6%; Kutnjak Ivković et al., 2015) and the South African
sample (62.5%; Kutnjak Ivković and Sauerman, 2015). The South African sample is somewhat
less experienced (44% have 10 or fewer years of experience; Kutnjak Ivković, Sauerman,
2015), while the Croatian sample (22%; Kutnjak Ivković, 2015) and the U.S. sample (27%;
Kutnjak Ivković et al., 2015) have about one-quarter of the respondents with 10 years or
experience or fewer. While the Croatian and U.S. police officers are most frequently assigned
to patrol, their South African counterparts are mostly engaged in detective/investigative
assignments (Kutnjak Ivković, 2015, Kutnjak Ivković, Sauerman, 2015, Kutnjak Ivković et al.,
2015).
Results
Results of Bivariate Analysis
We began the analysis by researching the bivariate relation between the dependent variable
(country of origin) and their own willingness to report in all 11 cases.
12
Every scenario shows a statistically significant relation between willingness to report and the
country of origin (Table 4). In other words, the bivariate analyses suggest that the country is
strongly linked with the respondents' expressed willingness to report. Hence, the results of
bivariate analyses indicate that we should reject the null hypothesis.
Post-hoc tests, according to the Marascuillo procedure (Glass, Hopkins, 1984), yielded
statistically significant differences between the respective pairs in all 11 of the examined
scenarios. It is interesting that, in 10 of the 11 scenarios, the differences in the Croatia-South
Africa pairings are statistically significant including in the scenario describing the use of deadly
force—the most serious use of excessive force scenario. In 9 out of 10 scenarios with
statistically significant differences, Croatian police officers seemed more likely to say that they
would adhere to the code of silence than their South African counterparts. Similarly, there were
statistically significant differences in 9 out of 11 scenarios between the expressed willingness
to report between Croatian and US respondents. In all but one scenario, Croatian respondents
seem to be more likely to say that they would not report. The only scenario in which the U.S.
respondents seemed more likely to stick to the code of silence is Scenario 1 which describes
the acceptance of free gifts. Finally, in 9 out of 11 scenarios there were also statistically
significant differences between the expressed willingness to report by the South African and
the U.S. respondents. However, the differences were not uniformly spread out; in some
scenarios, the U.S. respondents were more likely to say that they would not report, while in
others, South African respondents were more likely to say the same.
13
Table 4: Bivariate analysis of willingness to report by country
Croatia
Would not
report
Rank
South Africa
Would not
Rank
report
USA
Would not
report
Rank
Chi-Square
Scenario 1 (Gifts)
24.1%
1
48.8%
10
59.8%
10
216.320***
Scenario 2 (Warrant)
52.2%
6
44.0%
8
61.7%
11
46.825***
Scenario 3 (Burglary)
46.7%
5
34.7%
7
20.0%
3
126.887***
Scenario 4 (Deadly
Force)
44.0%
4
32.6%
6
13,0%
1
172.164***
Scenario 5 (Supervisor 2)
37.6%
2
17.2%
3
22.8%
5
109.379***
Scenario 6 (Hurts)
56.2%
8
16.8%
2
22.0%
4
392.569***
Scenario 7 (Verbal)
53.8%
7
32.2%
5
45.8%
9
91.392***
Scenario 8 (Alcohol)
58,5%
9
30.4%
4
41.2%
8
157.996***
Scenario 9 (Auto)
61.9%
11
55.9%
11
40.9%
7
70.496***
Scenario 10 (Report)
60.3%
10
47.1%
9
24.6%
6
202.527***
Scenario 11 (Watch)
37.6%
2
15.7%
1
14.3%
2
170.529***
* p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001
14
Significant
pairs
CRO v. USA
SA v. USA
CRO v. SA
SA v. USA
CRO v. SA
CRO v. USA
SA v. USA
CRO v. SA
CRO v. USA
SA v. USA
CRO v. USA
CRO v. SA
CRO v. USA
SA v. USA
CRO v. SA
SA v. USA
CRO v. SA
CRO v. USA
SA v. USA
CRO v. SA
CRO v. USA
SA v. USA
CRO v. USA
SA v. USA
CRO v. SA
CRO v. USA
CRO v. SA
Phi
.287
.134
.220
.259
.205
.389
.188
.248
.165
.280
.258
Table 5: Logistic regression of expressed willingness to report by organizational variables and country
Scenario 1 (Gifts)
Supervisory status1
Length of service2
6-15 years
Over 15 years
Own seriousness3
Expected discipline4
Some discipline
Dismissal
Discipline fairness5
Too harsh
Too lenient
Others' reporting6
Country7
Croatia
South Africa
Constant
Model Nagelkerke R
Squared
Scenario 2 (Warrant)
Scenario 3 (Burglary)
Scenario 4 (Deadly Force)
B
.597
s.e.
.150
Odds
1.816***
B
.556
s.e.
.124
Odds
1.743***
B
.645
s.e.
.148
Odds
1.905***
B
.331
s.e.
.177
Odds
1.392
.386
.387
1.316
.178
.185
.125
1.472*
1.473*
3.729***
.668
.531
2.330
.156
.157
.155
1.950***
1.700***
10.283***
.496
.579
1.914
.173
.176
.209
1.641**
1.785**
6.782***
.490
.498
1.580
.208
.211
.168
1.632**
1.645**
4.856***
1.463
.185
4.321***
1.370
.141
3.937***
1.752
.130
5.765***
1.773
.159
5.889***
-1.999
1.158
2.542
.392
.230
.131
.136*** -1.030
3.184***
.937
12.707*** .757
.228
.209
.110
.357***
2.553***
2.132***
-1.470
1.261
3.010
.236
.197
.182
.230***
3.530***
20.289***
-1.238
1.373
3.930
.244
.332
.209
.290***
3.946***
50.883***
.282
1.134
-3.609
.153
.158
.262
1.326
3.108***
.027***
.145
.136
.237
.950
1.615***
.029
-.399
-.025
-3.059
.169
.174
.296
.671*
.976
.047
-.408
.227
-2.930
.239
.248
.333
.665
1.255
.053***
.571
-.052
.480
-3.544
.343
.539
* p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001
1 Reference category = "No";
2 Reference category = "Over 5 years";
3 Reference category = "Not serious";
4 Reference category = "None"/"Some Discipline“;
5 Reference category = "Fair";
6 Reference category = "Would not report";
7 Reference category = "USA";
15
.667
Scenario 5 (Supervisor 2)
Supervisory status1
Length of service2
6-15 years
Over 15 years
Own seriousness3
Expected discipline4
Some discipline
Dismissal
Discipline fairness5
Too harsh
Too lenient
Others' reporting6
Country7
Croatia
South Africa
Constant
Model Nagelkerke R
Squared
Scenario 6 (Hurts)
Scenario 7 (Verbal)
Scenario 8 (Alcohol)
B
s.e.
Odds
B
s.e.
Odds
B
s.e.
Odds
B
s.e.
Odds
.521
.186
1.683**
.912
.184
2.488***
.908
.179
2.480***
.788
.185
2.198***
-.383
-.166
1.002
.220
.227
.150
.682
.847
2.725***
-.277
-.395
1.327
.222
.228
.158
.758
.674
3.770***
-.384
-.293
1.992
.236
.237
.167
.681
.746
7.331***
.329
.153
1.626
.217
.223
.158
1.390
1.166
5.085***
1.984
.256
7.275***
2.094
3.842
.285
.172
8.115***
46.604***
1.540
.281
4.666***
1.734
1.596
.251
.364
5.663***
4.935***
-2.021
1.823
3.474
.489
.296
.154
.133*** -2.229
6.192*** 1.268
32.264*** 3.842
.378
.406
.172
.108***
3.553**
46.604***
-2.214
.866
4.187
.488
.446
.173
.109***
2.377
65.846***
-1.904
1.649
3.784
.296
.401
.161
.149***
5.201***
43.976***
-.375
.298
-2.817
.190
.205
.340
.202
.216
.337
.757
2.369***
.033***
-.487
.353
-3.880
.195
.198
.372
.614**
1.424
.021***
.109
.676
-4.341
.198
.206
.357
1.115
1.996**
.013***
.637
.687*
1.347
.060
-.278
.862
-3.397
.735
.753
* p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001
1 Reference category = "No";
2 Reference category = "Over 5 years“;
3 Reference category = "Not serious";
4 Reference category = "None"/„Some Discipline“;
5 Reference category = "Fair";
6 Reference category = "Would not report";
7 Reference category = "USA";
16
.743
Scenario 9 (Auto)
Supervisory status1
Length of service2
6-15 years
Over 15 years
Own seriousness3
Expected discipline4
Some discipline
Dismissal
Discipline fairness5
Too harsh
Too lenient
Others' reporting6
Country7
Croatia
South Africa
Constant
Model Nagelkerke R Squared
Scenario 10 (Report)
Scenario 11 (Watch)
B
s.e.
Odds
B
s.e.
Odds
B
s.e.
Odds
.471
.160
1.602**
.446
.153
1.562**
.934
.221
2.545***
.090
.243
2.620
.218
.217
.206
1.094
1.275
13.732***
.009
-.036
2.694
.203
.206
.226
1.009
.965
14.788***
-.644
-.603
1.230
.257
.271
.172
.525
.547
3.422***
1.727
.161
5.624***
1.280
.140
3.596***
1.974
.319
7.202***
-1.298
1.335
3.749
.311
.259
.232
.273***
3.800***
42.476***
-1.347
1.426
4.448
.293
.259
.290
.260***
4.161***
84.474***
-2.430
1.953
3.773
.387
.678
.182
.088***
7.049**
43.506***
-.702
-.504
-3.197
.173
.185
.293
.667
.495***
.604**
.041***
-.464
.017
-3.277
.174
.182
.310
.664
.629**
1.017
.038***
-.134
.431
-1.091
.243
.260
.317
.641
.875
1.539
.336**
* p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001
1 Reference category = "No";
2 Reference category = "Over 5 years“;
3 Reference category = "Not serious";
4 Reference category = "None"/„Some Discipline“;
5 Reference category = "Fair";
6 Reference category = "Would not report";
7 Reference category = "USA";
17
Results of Regression Analyses
To tease out the potential country effect on the code of silence when other variables are
controlled for, we performed a logistic regression of the respondents' own willingness to report.
Instead of summarizing the results for each scenario, we provide a summary of findings across
all 11 scenarios. We first start the analysis by exploring organizational factors (perceptions of
seriousness, expected discipline, discipline fairness, and others' reporting), followed up by the
respondents' demographic characteristics (supervisory position, length of service), and last but
not least important - country.
By far the most important predictor of the respondents' expressed willingness to report is their
assessment of whether other police officers would report (Table 5). In fact, estimated others’
willingness to report is identified as a statistically significant predictor of willingness to report
in all eleven scenarios. Accordingly, those respondents who think that others would report
misconduct are also more likely to state their willingness to report such behavior.
Another important organizational variable is the respondents' own assessment of misconduct
seriousness. Again, own perception of the seriousness of misconduct is a statistically significant
predictor of their own willingness to report. Those respondents who evaluated behavior to be
serious also appear more likely to say that they would indeed report it.
The anticipated disciplinary measure is also identified as an important predictor for own
reporting with this variable showing statistical significance in all 11 scenarios. However, for
methodological reasons (too few respondents), not all categories were used in all scenarios
(Table 5). When “no discipline” was used as a reference category (Scenario 1: free meals, gifts
from merchants, Scenario 5: supervisor offers holiday for errands, Scenario 6: officer strikes
prisoner, Scenario 7: verbal abuse of motorist, Scenario 8: cover-up of police DUI accident),
the results suggested that, compared to the respondents who thought that no discipline would
follow, the respondents who thought that some discipline would follow were more likely to say
that they would report. When “some discipline“ was used as a reference category (Scenario 2:
failure to arrest friend with warrant, Scenario 3: theft of watch from crime scene, Scenario 4:
unjustifiable use of deadly force, Scenario 6: officer strikes prisoner, Scenario 8: cover-up of
police DUI accident, Scenario 9: auto body shop 5% kickback, Scenario 10: false report of drug
possession, Scenario 11: Sgt. fails to halt beating of child abuser), compared to the respondents
who thought that “some discipline” would follow, the respondents who thought that dismissal
would follow were more likely to say that they would report.
18
We notice that discipline fairness is another extremely important predictor of willingness of
police officers to report unacceptable behavior at work, being statistically significant in all of
the 11 examined scenarios. Compared to those who perceived discipline to be fair, those who
perceived discipline to be harsh were less likely to say that they would report it. Compared to
those who perceived discipline to be fair, those who perceived discipline to be too lenient were
more likely to say that they would report in 10 of the 11 scenarios.
Our logistic regression models also included two demographic characteristics. Compared to the
employees with up to 5 years of work experience, employees with 6-15 years of work
experience seemed more likely to say that they would report in only 4 out of 11 scenarios
(Scenario 1: free meals, gifts from merchants, Scenario 2: failure to arrest friend with warrant,
Scenario 3: theft of watch from crime scene, and Scenario 4: unjustifiable use of deadly force).
Compared to the employees with up to 5 years of work experience, employees with over 15
years of work experience were also more likely to say that they would report in 4 out of 11
scenarios (Scenario 1: free meals, gifts from merchants, Scenario 2: failure to arrest friend with
warrant, Scenario 3: theft of watch from crime scene, and Scenario 4: unjustifiable use of deadly
force).
In this paper, supervisory status is recognized as a statistically significant predictor of
supervisor readiness to report in 10 out of the 11 scenarios. Compared to line officers,
supervisors were more likely to say that they would report. It should be noted that the only
scenario in which supervisory status is not significant, and does not affect willingness for own
reporting, is the abuse of deadly force scenario.
Finally, we explore the independent country effect, with the answers by the U.S. respondents
serving as the reference category. The results of our analyses show that the country effect is
significant in 9 out of 11 scenarios. The only two scenarios in which country is not a significant
predictor of the respondents' willingness to report are those evaluated as the most serious in all
three countries (Kutnjak Ivković, 2015, Kutnjak Ivković, Sauerman, 2015, Kutnjak Ivković et
al., 2015): abusing deadly force and stealing from a crime scene. In other words, our null
hypothesis has been rejected in 9 out of 11 scenarios in which we found the country effect.
Compared to the U.S. respondents, Croatian respondents seemed less likely to say that they
would report in 5 out of 11 scenarios (Scenario 3: theft of watch from crime scene, Scenario 5:
supervisor offers holiday for errands, Scenario 7: verbal abuse of motorist, Scenario 9: auto
body shop 5% kickback, Scenario 10: false report of drug possession). While there are
19
statistically significant differences between the South African respondents and the U.S.
respondents in 5 scenarios (Scenario 1: free meals, gifts from merchants, Scenario 2: failure to
arrest friend with warrant, Scenario 6: officer strikes prisoners, Scenario 8: cover-up of police
DUI accident, Scenario 9: auto body shop 5% kickback), police officers from South Africa
seemed more willing to say that they would report than police officers from the U.S.A. in 4 out
of 5 scenarios.
Discussion and Conclusion
The results of our analyses point out that the country of respondents’ origin is related to their
expressed probability of reporting in all except for the two most serious cases. Specifically, our
multivariate models indicate the persistence of different code of silence forms in different
countries, even when control variables are included in the model. In other words, our null
hypothesis of no country influence on the code of silence has been rejected in all but two
scenarios. These two scenarios are the most serious forms of misconduct—stealing from a crime
scene and abusing deadly force—and police officers across three countries differed very little
in how likely they were to say that they would report.
Our findings related to the two most serious scenarios are in line with prior comparative
research. In particular, Kutnjak Ivković and Haberfeld (2015) reported how the code of silence
is present in 10 countries in their research. However, Kutnjak Ivković and Haberfeld (2015)
stated that despite the importance of country as a predictor in a large number of scenarios, it
was noticed that the country was not an important predictor in serious situations, such as in
scenarios of unjustifiable use of deadly force and scenarios where police supervisors fail to
intervene when witnessing police officers under their supervision strike an arrested person. It
is obvious that in such situations, there are expectations similar to serious consequences for
such unacceptable behavior, and therefore respondents from various countries were all less
likely to state that they would not report.
Compared to a democratic country such as the United States, Croatia—a country in transition—
has a stronger code of silence. On the other hand, a comparison of the U.S. results with the
results from another transitional country—South Africa—shows differences as well, but the
direction of differences is not uniform. In some scenarios, respondents from South Africa
seemed more likely to say that they would report than the respondents from the United States,
while in other scenarios, respondents from South Africa seemed to be more likely to say that
20
they would report than their counterparts from the United States, a developed Western
democracy. The results demonstrate that the country of origin is an important predictor of code
of silence presence. However, one cannot unambiguously conclude that a transitional country
always produces a stronger code of silence.
The fairness perception of the expected discipline is a significant predictor in all scenarios tested
in this study. In relation to respondents who think that the disciplinary measures are too harsh,
respondents who consider the disciplinary measure fair were more likely to say that they would
report unacceptable behavior at work. This result is in accordance with expectations of one of
the theoretical models (Kutnjak Ivković, Klockars, 1998). More specifically, the motivation of
police officers for reporting misconduct is also related and influenced by their desire to see
misconduct at work disciplined in an appropriate and fair manner, which is in accordance with
the work of Kutnjak Ivković and Klockars (1998).
This paper represents further progress in police integrity research as it provides a detailed
analysis of the code of silence in three countries. However, limitations can be imagined,
including the possible non-representative nature of the U.S. and South African police officer
samples when compared to the entire police populations in the two countries. As a result, the
national differences represented in this study may be limited to the police administrations that
participated in the study. Furthermore, a comparative examination of officer perceptions of
disciplinary measures may be hindered by the different legal frameworks that officers operate
under in these three countries despite our preparation of scenarios to facilitate such country
comparisons. Furthermore, the different language adaptations of the questionnaire may be a
limiting factor in comparing officer responses, while the legal and social conditions under
which the agencies were formed could impact the functioning of the different police
administrations and the behavior of the police officers they employ (Kutnjak Ivković,
Haberfeld, 2015). Comparisons to date on police integrity research (for instance, Klockars,
Kutnjak Ivković, and Haberfeld, 2004; Kutnjak Ivković and Haberfeld, 2015) demonstrate
however that the questionnaire is suitable for comparative analyses.
21
References
Bittner, E. (1971). The Functions of the Police in Modern Society. Washington D.C.: U.S.
Government Printing Office.
Glass, G. V., Hopkins, K. D. (1984). Statistical Methods in Education and Psychology.
Boston: Allyn and Bacon., p. 391-392.
Greene, J. R., Piquero, A. R., Hickman, M. J. & Lawton, B. A. (2004). Police Integrity and
Accountability in Philadelphia: Predicting and Assessing Police Misconduct.
Retrieved at: https://www.ncjrs.gov/pdffiles1/nij/grants/207823.pdf.
Huon, G. F., Hesketh, B. L., Frank, M. G., McConkey, K. M. & McGrath, G. M. (1995).
Perceptions of ethical dilemmas: ethics and policing — Study 1, Report Series.
Klockars, C.B., Kutnjak Ivković, S. & Haberfeld, M.R. (2000). The measurement of police
integrity. Research in brief. Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice.
Klockars, C. B., & Kutnjak Ivković, S. (2004). Measuring police integrity. In M. Hickman, A.
R. Piquero & J. R. Greene (Eds.), Police Integrity and Ethics. Belmont, CA:
Wadsworth.
Klockars, C.B., Kutnjak Ivković, S., & Haberfeld, M.R. (2006). Enhancing police integrity.
Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer.
Kutnjak Ivković, S., Haberfeld, M.R. & Peacock, R. (2013). Rainless West: the integrity
survey’s role in agency accountability. Police Quarterly, 16(2), 148-176.
Kutnjak Ivković, S. & Klockars, C. B. (1998). The Code of Silence and the Croatian Police. In
Pagon, M. (Ed.). Policing in Central and Eastern Europe: Organizational, Managerial,
and Human Resource Aspects. Ljubljana, Slovenia: College of Police and Security
Studies.
Kutnjak Ivković, S. & O’Connor Shelley, T. (2010). The Code of Silence and Disciplinary
Fairness: A Comparison of Czech Police Supervisor and Line Officer Views. Policing:
An International Journal of Police Strategies and Management, 33, 548-574.
Kutnjak Ivković, S. & O’Connor Shelley, T. (2007). Police Integrity and the Czech Police
Officers, International Journal of Comparative and Applied Criminal Justice, 31, 2149.
Kutnjak Ivković, S. & O’Connor Shelley, T. (2005). The Bosnian Police and Police Integrity:
A Continuing Story, European Journal of Criminology, 2, 428-464.
Kutnjak Ivković, S. & Sauerman, A. (2013). Curtailing the code of silence among the South
African police. Policing: An International Journal of Police Strategies & Management,
36(1), 175-198.
22
Kutnjak Ivković, S. (2015). Studying Police Integrity in the United States. Measuring Police
Integrity Across the World (p. 1-36). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Kutnjak Ivković, S. (2015). Police Integrity Croatia. Measuring Police Integrity Across the
World (p. 97-124). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Kutnjak Ivković, S., Haberfeld, M. (2015). A comparative Perspective on Police Integrity.
Measuring Police Integrity Across the World (p. 329-368). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Kutnjak Ivković, S., Haberfeld, M. & Peacock, R. (2015). Police Integrity in the United States.
Measuring Police Integrity Across the World (p. 295-327). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Kutnjak Ivković, S., Sauerman, A. (2015). Police Integrity in South Africa. Measuring Police
Integrity Across the World (p. 213-240). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Long, M. A., Cross, J. E., O’Connor Shelley, T., & Kutnjak Ivković, S. (2013). The Normative
Order of Reporting Police Misconduct: Examining the Roles of Offense Seriousness,
Legitimacy, and Fairness, Social Psychology Quarterly, 76, 242-267.
Marche, G.E. (2009). Integrity, culture, and scale: An empirical test of the big bad police
agency. Crime, Law, and Social Change 51:463-486.
Micucci, A. J. & Gomme, I.M. (2009). American police and subcultural support for the use of
excessive force. Journal of Criminal Justice, 33, 487-500.
Mollen, M. (1994). Anatomy of Failure: A Path for Success, Mollen Report. New York:
Commissions to Investigate Allegations of Police Corruption and the Anti-Corruption
Procedures of the New York Police Department.
Pagon, M. & Lobnikar, B. (2004). Police integrity in Slovenia, in Klockars, C.B., Kutnjak
Ivković, S. & Haberfeld, M.R. (Eds.). The contours of police integrity. Thousand Oaks,
CA: Sage Publications.
Rothwell, G. R. & Baldwin, J. N. (2007). Ethical climate theory, whistle-blowing, and the code
of silence in police agencies in the state of Georgia. Journal of Business Ethics, 70 (4),
341-361.
Schafer, J.A. & Martinelli, T.J. (2008). First-line supervisor’s perceptions of police integrity.
Policing: An international journal of police strategies and management 31(2), 306-323.
Weisburd, D. & Greenspan, R. (2000). Police Attitudes Toward Abuse of Authority: Findings
from A National Study. Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice. Retrieved at:
https://www.ncjrs.gov/pdffiles1/nij/181312.pdf.
Westley, W. (1970) Violence and the Police: A Sociological Study of Law, Custom, and
Morality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Pres
Wolfe, S. E., & Piquero, A. R. (2011). Organizational justice and police misconduct. Criminal
Justice & Behavior, 38, 332–353.
23
Wood, J. (1997). Royal Commission into the New South Wales Police Service. Final Report,
Vol. 1. Sydney, Australia: Government of the State of New South Wales.
24