Reported Incidence Rates of Work-Related Sexual Harassment in The United States: Using Meta-Analysis To Explain Reported Rate Dispari...
Reported Incidence Rates of Work-Related Sexual Harassment in The United States: Using Meta-Analysis To Explain Reported Rate Dispari...
Reported Incidence Rates of Work-Related Sexual Harassment in The United States: Using Meta-Analysis To Explain Reported Rate Dispari...
net/publication/227604263
CITATIONS READS
160 3,499
4 authors, including:
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by Remus Ilies on 06 October 2017.
REMUS ILIES
Department of Management
Warrington College of Business Administration, University of Florida
NANCY HAUSERMAN
Department of Management and Organizations
Henry B. Tippie College of Business, University of Iowa
SUSAN SCHWOCHAU
Dickinson Wright PLLC
JOHN STIBAL
Equant N.V.
We thank Tim Judge, Huy Le, Sara Rynes, and Frank Schmidt for their helpful com-
ments on earlier versions of this paper
Correspondence and requests for reprints should be addressed to Remus Ilies, who
is now at the Department of Management, Eli Broad Graduate School of Management,
N475 North Business Complex, East Lansing, MI, 48824; admin@studies-online.org.
its extent in the workplace, its psychological underpinnings, and its an-
tecedents and consequences. Many of the studies on sexual harassment
purport to offer quantitative estimates of the incidence rate of sexual ha-
rassment in certain organizations or occupations. Information about the
level of sexual harassment is valuable; it can be used to assess whether
there is an increase or a decrease in the amount of sexual harassment
generally or in specific workplaces or occupations. Incidence data may
also be used to measure the success or failure of various training pro-
grams or the effect of specific, highly publicized cases or claims based
on sexual harassment (e.g., the Clarence Thomas/Anita Hill hearings in
1991). Furthermore, to the extent that legislation is expected to elimi-
nate or at least minimize sexual harassment, estimates of incidence rate
may say something about the effect of law (Gruber, 1990).
Because the incidence rate of sexual harassment may be used to draw
a variety of conclusions, it is critical that surveys attempting to assess
sexual harassment incidence use sound sampling design principles and
valid and reliable measures. But in fact, if one considers the studies
that attempted to measure the incidence rate of sexual harassment, it is
difficult to draw any firm conclusion about the actual incidence of such
behaviors (Lengnick-Hall, 1995). Empirical data show that reported
incidence rates vary widely across studies. Studies included in Gruber’s
(1990) review, for example, report incidence rates between .28 and .75.
Inconsistent results do not offer clear policy implications. Further-
more, gross variance in reports of the incidence of sexual harassment
may provide fodder for those who would argue that the variability in
empirical findingsshows the difficultyof identifyingwhat constitutes sex-
ual harassment and that reports of frequent occurrence are a reflection
of researcher bias or faulty study design. Luthar (1999, for example,
criticizes the sexual harassment research by saying that “policy makers
should look at the results from sexual harassment research with some
skepticism, as it is dominated by a feminist ideology, tainted by implicit
biases against males, and functions from a rather narrow paradigm”
(p. 271). Whereas the variability in incidence rates may be due to avari-
ety of rational factors and methodological problems or inconsistencies,
it is critical that we be able to identify those factors and problems. The
present study integrates empirical findings relevant to the incidence of
sexual harassment in the workplace and identifies major factors that in-
fluence the reported incidence rate.
Moderating Effects
Construct Definition
‘Though it minimizes perceptual bias, the behavioral experiences method does not
eliminate such bias because behavioral items such as “Suggestive stories and offensive
jokes” (Fitzgerald et al., 1988) implicitly require perceptual interpretation (i.e., what one
perceives to be “suggestive”or “offensive”will vary across respondents).
REMUS ILIES ET AL. 611
Design Issues
Several articles have commented on the problems inherent in the
sample selection bias associated with the reliance on convenience, non-
probability samples (Gutek, 1995; Lengnick-Hall, 1995; Pryor &
McKinney, 1995). Probability sampling designs involve a sampling plan
that gives each respondent from the sampling frame a nonzero known
probability of being surveyed. Results from probability samples can be
generalized to the population of interest (samplingframe). Convenience
samples do not allow generalization of findings (Lengnick-Hall, 1995)
and may provide biased estimates of the prevalence of sexual harass-
ment in the workplace through a selection-bias mechanism.
Whereas respondent self-selection processes are likely to be present
in both convenience and probability samples, convenience samples are
subject to another selection bias process: sample-selectionbias. Sample-
selection processes can bias the reported incidence rates in either di-
rection. First, it is possible that the selection of convenience samples
for sexual harassment research may be biased towards samples that are
likely to report higher incidence rates through an availabilitymechanism.
Samples of women who have filed grievances or sought help are simply
more available to researchers and they are also likely to report higher
incidence rates of sexual harassment. At the extreme, sampling from a
population of people who filed sexual harassment complaints will give
an estimate of sexual harassment incidence close to 1.00. An illustra-
tive example of the sample-selection bias is given by Loy and Stewart
(1984, p. 32):
Some estimates may be inflated because of the composition of the sample.
For example, women who attend a speak-out on sexual harassment (used
as the sample in the research by Silverman, 1976) may attend because they
have experienced sexual harassment themselves. The sexual harassment
frequency estimate of 90 percent in Silverman’s study may be inflated
because it comes from a biased sample.
Organizational Context
An organizational context dimension relevant to sexual harassment
is the relative power difference between organizational levels. As most
definitions of sexual harassment include inappropriate use of power
(e.g., Loy & Stewart, 1984; McKinney, 1990; Niebuhr, & Boyles, 1991;
Rosell, Miller, & Barber, 1995), and power relationships among organi-
zational members are largely determined by organizational structure, it
follows logically that “highly structured and stratified organizations are
more likely settings for harassment” (Niebuhr & Boyles, 1991, p. 448).
Other things being equal (policies, training, etc.), we would predict that
the extent of sexual harassment would be higher in highly structured, bu-
reaucratic organizations than in organizationswith a more organic struc-
ture (Bums & Stalker, 1961). Testing this prediction was the third goal
of this study.
To investigate the association between power differentials (deter-
mined by organizational structure) and sexual harassment with the
present data, we can examine whether the type of work environment
where sexual harassment incidence data were reported influences the
reported rates. We grouped studies according to the work environment
of the respondents into academic,private sector; government, and militaly
studies, assuming that organizational characteristics such as structure
and the nature of power relationships are more similar within groups
than across these groups.
As the military is highly structured with large power differentials
among organizational levels, we expect sexual harassment incidence
rates to be the highest in the military. Because academic organizations
Time Trenak
The year when a specific study has been conducted may influence the
reported incidence rate of sexual harassment. On the one hand, because
public awareness about sexual harassment has been on the increase over
the past decades, it is possible the sexual harassment incidence rates re-
ported by women have also been increasing because, over time, women
progressively changed their definition of sexual harassment to include
more behaviors. On the other hand, because normative measures aimed
at decreasing sexual harassment in organizations have been increasingly
implemented over the same period of time, it is possible that reported
incidence rates have been decreasing because of a substantive reduction
in the extent of sexual harassment. Because it is not clear how time in-
fluences the reported incidence rate, we will examine whether there are
time trends in the incidence data on an exploratory basis.
Method
Meta-Analysis Procedures
Using the Schmidt-Hunter psychometric meta-analysis method
(Hunter & Schmidt, 1990),we conducted a meta-analyticreview that cu-
REMUS ILIES ET AL. 615
3Samplesizes (n;)and the proportionof people who reported having experienced sexual
harassment (Pi)were available for each study. For each analysis the mean incidence
rate (proportion; P) was computed with the formula: F = C ( n i P , ) / h , . The observed
variance in the incidence rates was computed as: n i ( P ; - n 2 / C n i . The sample-weighted
mean sampling-errorvariance of incidence rates was obtained as: En; PiQ;)/Cni)/Cni=
(E[PiQi])/Eni. The true variance estimate was then computed as the differencebetween
the observed variance and the sample-weighted mean sampling error variance (this is an
upper-boundvalue because it includes variance due to artifacts not accounted for by the
meta-analytic procedure).
616 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
Literature Search
fense surveys (e.g., Hay & Elig, 1999; Martindale, 1990) were included
in the d a t a b a ~ e . ~
Following the suggestion of an anonymous reviewer, we examined
the reasons for which the PsychINFO-ERIC abstracts were excluded.
Based on a random 10% sample of the 2,231 abstracts that were selected
from PsychINFO and ERIC, the main reasons for excluding abstracts
were as follows: studies that did not contain empirically derived data,
81%; studies that focused on perceptions or involved experiments, 11%;
studies conducted outside the work environment (including those that
examined the harassment of students) or outside the U.S., 13% (these
percentages do not add up to 100%because some studies were excluded
for more than one reason).
For the 567 studies initially included in our study database (244 have
resulted from the PsycINFO and ERIC selections of abstracts, and 323
from Academic Search Elite), we examined each study to determine
whether it met the criteria detailed above and contained an estimate
of sexual harassment incidence rate based on an independent sample
of females (several studies reported data from the same sample, such
as the USMSPB samples; in such cases we included only the primary
study). We also examined the articles referenced in all studies selected
up to this point in an attempt to identify additional studies. Fourteen
referenced reports that met the criteria detailed above were included in
the database.
In total, 71 studies met our criteria. Nine studies reported data col-
lected from multiple independent samples, and several others reported
two incidence rates based on the same sample but on different types of
surveys (direct query and behavioral experiences). Thus, in all, 96 esti-
mates of sexual harassment incidence from 84 independent samples re-
ported in 71 studies were included in the analyses. All studies that con-
tained estimates included in the meta-analysis are listed as references.
‘From the military studies that were identified by the literature search, we could not
retrieve three unpublished reports.
618 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
Private Sector
Sampling
LL&
Behavioral
Convenience
'1 Military 1
I Overall
(___________________-----------------------
Academia I
I Private Sector I
I Probability
Sampling 1
tDirect Query ........................................
Convenience
*Note: The results for categories included in the areas delimited by dashed perimeters
are not reported
REMUS ILIES ET AL. 619
Results
51n all studies that used behavioral experiences surveys respondents who endorsed at
least one item were considered to have been sexually harassed.
‘For studies that did not report the year when the data were collected we entered the
year preceding their publication year.
7The overall weighted average contains both direct query and behavioral experiences
estimates. Thus,this estimate is not meaningful by itself. Following Hunter and Schmidt’s
(1990) recommendations for interpreting in hierarchical moderator analyses, we do not
interpret the overall estimate.
TABLE 1
Meta-Analyses of the Reported S a w 1 Harassment Incidence Rate in the Workplace: Methodological Moderators
Analysis K N Mean P sop SE 90% cv 95% CI
Overall 84 111,481 54 .22 .02 .18-.90 .49-.59
Studies that used direct query surveys 44 33,153 .35 .19 .03 .03-.67 .29-.41
Probability sampling 26 19,639 .24 .12 .02 .04-.43 .19-.28
Nonprobability sampling 18 13,514 .51 .16 .04 .24-.78 .44-.59
cd
Behavioral experiences surveys 52 81,268 .62 .18 .03 .32-.92 .57-.67 v1
Probability sampling 35 68,765 .58 .17 .03 .31-.84 .52-.63 4
A
'Within the behavioral experiences category, it is possible that the number of behaviors
listed on the survey moderates the reported incidence rate (e.g., surveysthat list a relatively
large number of behaviors may include less severe behaviors so more respondents are
likely to endorse at least on item from such surveys). To investigate this possibility, we
correlated the number of behaviors listed on the surveys with the reported incidence rate.
The correlation wasweak and nonsignificant ( r = .08, ns),which indicates that the number
of behaviors on the survey has little effect on the reported rate of sexual harassment.
TABLE 2
Meta-Analysesof the Reported Senral Harassment Incidence Rate in the Workplace: Organizational Context Moderator
slightly increased over time, whereas the incidence rate from conve-
nience samples actually decreased. That is, for probability samples,
within the direct query category the correlation between the year in
which the data were collected and the reported incidence rate was T = .ll
( N = 26), and within the behavioral experiences category the correla-
tion was T = .26 ( N = 35). In contrast, for convenience samples, the
correlations between the year in which the data were collected and the
reported incidence were T = -.33 ( N = 18) and T = -.36 ( N = 17) in
the direct query and behavioral experiences category, respectively.
We need to point out that in all meta-analyses conducted in this
study statistical artifacts explained modest proportions of the variance
observed in reported incidence rate. Given the nature of the data, it is
a result that should be expected. That is, sampling error did not explain
a great proportion of the observed variance because of the very large
samples reported in some studies (estimates from those samples contain
practically no sampling error).
Discussion
Contributions
Implications
Limitations
Conclusions
REFERENCES
References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis.
Alger A, Flanagan WG. (1996). Sexual politics: Sexual harassment in the workplace.
Forbes, 157,106-110.
Arvey RD, Cavanaugh MA. (1995). Using surveys to assess the prevalence of sexual
harassment: Some methodological problems. Journal of Social Issues, 51,3!%52.
'Bails CI:(1995). Females' reactions to serual harassment in the workplace and the impact
of race. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Temple University.
'Baker NL. (1990). S m l harassment and job satlsfactwn in traditionaland nontraditional
industrial occupations. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, California Schoolof Pro-
fessional Psychology.
*BaldridgeK, McLean G. (1980). Sexual harassment: How much of a probbm is it.. .really?
Journal of Business Education, 55,294-297.
'Blakely GL, Blakely EH, Moorman RH. (1995). The relationship between gender, per-
sonal experience,and perceptions of sexual harassment in the workplace. Empfoyee
Responsibilities and Rights Journal, 8,263-274.
Blumenthal JA. (1998). The reasonable woman standard: A meta-analytic review of
gender differences in perceptions of sexual harassment. Law and Human Behavior;
22,33-57.
Braine JD, Bless C, Fox PMC. (1995). How do students perceive sexual harassment? An
investigation on the University of Natal, Pietermaritzburg campus. South African
Journal of Pychology, 25,140-149.
628 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
*Bremer BA, Moore Cr,Bildersee EE (1991). Do you have to call it “sexual harassment”
to feel harassed? College Student Journal, 25,258-268.
*Brooks L, Perot AR. (1991). Reporting sexual harrassment: Exploring a predictive
model. Psychology of Women Quarter& 15,3147.
Bums T, Stalker GM. (1961). The management of innovation. London: Tavistock Press.
‘Cardinale V. (1992,March 9). Sexual harassment on the job. Drug Topics, 136,pp. 40,
43-46,49-50,53-54.
‘Carol1 L, Ellis KL. (1989). Faculty attitudes toward sexual harassment: Survey results,
survey process. Initiatives: Journal of the National Association for Women Deans,
Administrators,and Counselors, 52,3541.
“CulbertsonAL, Rosenfeld F! (1993). Understanding sexual harassment through organiza-
tional surveys. In Rosenfeld P, Edwards JE (Eds.), Improvingorganizationalsurveys:
New directions, methods, and applications (pp. 164-187). Newbury Park, C A Sage.
*DanskyBS, Kilpatrick DG. (1997). Theoretical explanations for the effects of sexual ha-
rassment. In O’DonohueW (Ed.), Sexual harassment theory, research, and treatment
(pp. 152-174). Needham Heights, M A Allyn & Bacon.
‘Daugherty S,Baldwin DC, Rowley BD. (1998). Learning, satisfaction, and mistreatment
during medical internship: A national survey of working conditions. J A M : Journal
of the American Medical Association, 279,11944199.
‘DeMayo RA. (1997). Patient sexual behavior and sexual harassment: A national survey
of female psychologists. Professional Psychology Research and Practice, 28,58-62.
‘Dey EL, Korn JS, Sax LJ. (1996). Betrayed by the academy: The sexual harassment of
women college faculty. Journal of Higher Education, 67,149-173.
*Duldt BW. (1982). Sexual harassment in nursing. Nursing Outlook, 30,336-343.
‘Fitzgerald LF, Shullman SL, Bailey N, Richards M, Swecker J, Gold Y,Ormerod AJ,
Weitzman L. (1988). The incidence and dimensions of sexual harassment in aca-
demia and the workplace. Journal of Vocational BehavioG 32,152-175.
Foulis D, McCabe MF! (1997). Sexual harassment: Factors affecting attitudes and per-
ceptions. Sex Roles, 37,773-798.
Gardner J, Allen E (1996). Sexual and gender harassment at university: Experiences and
perceptions of Australian women. Australian Psychologist, 31,210-216.
*Goldenhar LM, Swanson NG, Hurrell JJ Jr., Ruder A, Deddens J. (1998). Stressors and
adverse outcomes for female construction workers. Journal of OccupationalHealth
Psychology, 3,19-32.
*Goodwin MP, Roscoe B, Clarkson SE, Rose M. (1989). Sexual harassment experiences
of university employees. Initiatives: Journal of the National Associationfor Women
Deans, Administrators,and Counselors, 52,25-33.
*Grant SM. (2000). Incidence and risk factors of sexual harassment among working women:
Implications for counseling psychology and the workplace. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, University of California.
‘Grauerholz E. (1989). Sexual harassment of women professors by students: Exploring.
the dynamics of power, authority, and gender in a university setting. SexRoles, 21,
789-801.
*Grieco A. (1987). Scope and nature of sexual harassment in nursing. Journal of Ser
Research, 23,261-266.
*Griffin-ShelleyE. (1985). Sexual harassment: One organization’s response. Journal of
Counselingand Development, 64,72-73.
Gruber JE. (1990). Methodological problems and policy implications in sexual harass-
ment research. Population Research and Policy Review, 9,235-254.
‘Gruber JE, Bjom L. (1982). Blue-collar blues: The sexual harassment of women au-
toworkers. Work and Occupations, 9,271-298.
REMUS ILIES ET AL. 629
*Newell CE, Rosenfeld P, Culbertson AL. (1995). Sexual harassment experiences and
equal opportunity perceptions of Navy women. Sex Roles, 32,159-168.
Niebuhr RE, Boyles WR. (1991). Sexual harassment of military personnel: An exam-
ination of power differentials. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 15,
445-457.
*Piotrkowski CS.(1998). Gender harassment, job satisfaction, and distress among em-
ployed White and minority women. Journal of Occupational Health Pychology, 3,
33-43.
*Powell GN. (1983). Definition of sexual harassment and sexual attention experienced.
Journal of Psychology, 113,113-117.
Pryor JB. (1995). The phenomenology of sexual harassment: Why does sexual behav-
ior bother people in the workplace? Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and
Research, 47,160-168.
*Pryor JB, LaVite CM, Stoller LM. (1993). A social psychological analysis of sexual
harassment: The persodsituation interaction. Journal of Vocational Behavior; 42,
68-83.
Pryor JB, McKinney K. (1995). Research on sexual harassment: Lingering issues and
future directions. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 17,605-611.
*Ragins BR, Scandura TA. (1995). Antecedents and work-related correlates of reported
sexual harassment: An empirical investigation of competing hypotheses. Sex Roles,
32,429-455.
*Richman JA, Rospeda KM, Nawyn SJ, Flaherty JA, Fendrich M, Drum ML, Johnson
T€?(1999). Sexual harassment and generalized workplace abuse among university
employees: prevalence and mental health correlates. American Journal of Public
Health, 89,358-363.
'Rose11 E,Miller K, Barber K. (1995).Firefighting women and sexual harassment. Public
Personnel Management, 24,339-350.
'Rosen LN, Martin L. (1998). Incidence and perceptions of sexual harassment among
male and female U S . Army soldiers. Military Psychology, 10,239-257.
*Rosenberg J, Perlstadt H, Phillips WR.(1993). Now that we are here: Discrimination,
disparagement, and harassment at work and the experience of women lawyers.
Gender and Society, 7,415433.
Rotundo M, Nguyen DH, Sackett PR. (2001)A meta-afialytic review of gender percep-
tions of sexual harassment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86,914-922.
*Safran C. (1976). What men do to women on the job: A shocking look at sexual harass-
ment. Redbook, 149,217-223.
*Sandroff R. (1992). Sexual harassment: The inside story. Working Woman, 17,47-51.
*Sangamon State University. (1979). Study of unwanted sexual attention received by female
state employees: Preliminary data. Springfield, IL: Center for Policy Studies and
Program Evaluation.
*Schneider m, Swan S, Fitzgerald LE (1997). Job-related and psychological effect's of
sexual harassment in the workplace: Empirical evidence from two organizations.
Journal ofApplied Psychology, 82,401415.
Scott WR. (1998). Organizations: rational, natural, and open systems (4th ed.). Upper
Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
*Sigler RT, Johnson IM. (1986). Public perceptions of the need for criminalization of
sexual harassment. Journal of Criminal Justice, 14,229-237.
*Silverman D. (1976).Sexual harassment: Working women's dilemma. Quest: A Ferninkt
Quafie*, 3,15-24.
*SkinnerKM, Kressin N, Frayne S, 'Itipp TJ, Hankin CS, Miller DR, Sullivan LM. (2000).
The prevalence of military sexual assault among female Veterans' Administration
outpatients. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 15,291-310.
REMUS ILIES ET AL. 631
*Stank0 B, Werner CA. (1995). Sexual harassment: What is it? How to prevent it?
National Public Accountant, 40, 14-16.
*Stedham Y, Mitchell MC. (1998). Sexual harassment in casinos: Effects on employee
attitudes and behaviors. Journal of Gambling Studies, 14,381-400.
'Stockdale MS, Vaux A. (1993). What sexual harassment experiences lead respondents to
acknowledge being sexually harassed? A secondary analysis of a university survey.
Journal of Vocational Behavioc 43,221-234.
Stockdale M S , Vaux A, Cashin J. (1995). Acknowledging sexual harassment: A test of
alternative models. Basic and Applied Social Pychology, 17,469-496.
*United States Merit Systems Protection Board. (1981). Sexual harassment in the federal
government: Is it aproblem? Washington, DC: United States Government Printing
Office.
*United States Merit Systems Protection Board. (1987). Sexual harassment of federal
workers: An update. Washington,D C United States Government Printing Office.
*United States Merit Systems Protection Board. (1994). Sexualharassment in the federal
workplace: Trends, progress, and continuing challenges. Washington, DC: United
States Government Printing Office.
Viswesvaran C, Schmidt FL. (1992). A meta-analytic comparison of the effectiveness of
smoking cessation methods. Journal of Applied Pychology, 77,554-561.
*Vukovich MC. (1996). The prevalence of sexual harassment among female family prac-
tice residents in the United States. Violenceand Victims, 11,175-180.
'Walsh-Childers K, Chance J, Herzog K. (1996). Sexual harassment of women journalists.
Journalism and Mass Communication Quarter& 73,559-81.
*White JE. (1991). Sexual harassment in the workplace. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Kansas State University.
Whitener EM. (1990). Confusion of confidence intervals and credibility intervals in meta-
analysis. Journal ofApplied Pvchologx 75,315-321.
'Wolfe J, Sharkansky EJ,Read JP, Dawson R, Martin JA, Ouimette PC. (1998). Sexual
harassment and assault as predictors of PTSD symptomatology among U.S. female
Persian Gulf War military personnel. Journal of Interpersonal Violence,13,40-57.
'Wong MM. (1984). Serual harassment at work: Female police oficers. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, University of Pittsburgh.
'Wyat GE, Riederle M. (1995). The prevalence and context of sexual harassment among
African American and White American women. Journal of Interpersonal Violence,
10,306-319.
*Yoder JD, Aniakudo F! (1996). When pranks become harassment: The case of African
American women firefighters. Sex Roles, 35,253-270.