Hoe Mann 1971
Hoe Mann 1971
Hoe Mann 1971
REFERENCES
Linked references are available on JSTOR for this article:
http://www.jstor.org/stable/1127077?seq=1&cid=pdf-reference#references_tab_contents
You may need to log in to JSTOR to access the linked references.
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
http://about.jstor.org/terms
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted
digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about
JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.
Society for Research in Child Development, Wiley are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize,
preserve and extend access to Child Development
This content downloaded from 128.163.2.206 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 09:40:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
CHILDREN'S UNDERSTANDING OF PROBABILITY
CONCEPTS
Several recent studies (Davies 1965; Goldberg 1966; Yost, Siegel, &
Andrews 1962) have ostensibly shown that under favorable conditions
children as young as CA 4 and 5 show some understanding of probability
concepts. This result is in opposition to Piaget and Inhelder (1951) who
attribute the beginning of probability knowledge only to children at the
[Child Development, 1971, 42, 9.1-936. ? 1971 by the Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.
All rights reserved.]
This content downloaded from 128.163.2.206 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 09:40:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
CHILD DEVELOPMENT
222
This content downloaded from 128.163.2.206 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 09:40:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
HARRY W. HOEMANN AND BRUCE M. ROSS
EXPERIMENT 1
Method
223
This content downloaded from 128.163.2.206 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 09:40:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
CHILD DEVELOPMENT
black and white wedges constituting the disks; the remaining trials had
the black and white segments of each disk broken up into several alter-
nating wedges so that all of the black or white in a given disk was not
contiguous. Specifically, for the other 18 trials a given black-white pro-
portionality in one circle was divided into two wedges of each color in
one circle and four wedges of each color in the other. All wedges of the
same color in the same disk were of the same size. A multi-wedge example
of a versus , comparison with black in the majority displayed four al-
ternating wedges of -I- black and - - white in one circle versus eight alter-
nating wedges of - black and - white in the other circle. The variable
of number-of-wedges in the disks was interspersed randomly in the trial
sequence.
Apparatus and procedure.-The disks were 20 pairs of 12-inch circles
of heavy drawing paper composed of black and white wedges, with India
ink used in coloring the black wedges. The disks were stacked on two
adjacent pieces of 14-inch square plywood on a table in front of S. In-
serted dowels were - X 3 inches in length, and the spinners were plastic
circles 4 inches in diameter with a black point painted against a white
background. The first two pairs of disks were used for instruction and six
practice trials. The pairs used for practice included disks having gray
wedges with the color of the alternating wedges white on one disk and
black on the other. Thus possible versus impossible choices of both
"black" and "white" were included in the practice trials. The S was in-
formed that these initial trials were practice trials. Each of the 18 re-
maining disk pairs appeared twice in the course of the 36 experimental
trials, once with black and once with white as the E-specified color.
Either a black or a white 3 X 4-inch card was held up by E to accompany
the verbal designation of the specified color for each trial.
All Ss in this and subsequent experiments were tested individually.
Under both probability and proportionality instructions S was told at
the beginning of a session that he was to play a game with E.
With probability instructions children were told: "I want you to look
at the two spinners very carefully and show me which one you will spin to
make the pointer point to black (white)." Although verbal responses were
permitted, pointing to the chosen spinner was also required of Ss. The S
then spun the spinner on the disk he had pointed to. Responses and out-
comes were recorded by E, and favorable outcomes, even when incorrect
from a probability standpoint, were verbally acknowledged by E as suc-
cessful predictions. With proportionality instructions where there were no
spinners present, children were told: "I want you to look at these two
circles very carefully and show which circle has the most black (white)."
No indication as to whether S was correct on any trial was made by E
with proportionality instructions so that knowledge of results would not
affect judgments. Instead general encouragement was given the child
224
This content downloaded from 128.163.2.206 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 09:40:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
HARRY W. HOEMANN AND BRUCE M. ROSS
Results
TABLE 1
INSTRUCTIONS
41 6 7? 101
I.
4'
Proport...... 80 95 97 99
Prob..... . . . (80) (87) (94) (97)
Diff....... 0 8 3 2
Proport...... 72 88 94 96
Prob...... (62) (82) (89) (93)
Diff........ 10 6 5 3
Combined:
Proport...... 81 94 97 99
Prob......... (75) (87) (94) (97)
Diff....... 6 7 3 2
225
This content downloaded from 128.163.2.206 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 09:40:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
CHILD DEVELOPMENT
226
This content downloaded from 128.163.2.206 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 09:40:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
HARRY W. HOEMANN AND BRUCE M. ROSS
EXPERIMENT 2
Method
Results
227
This content downloaded from 128.163.2.206 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 09:40:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
CHILD DEVELOPMENT
TABLE 2
CHRONOLOGICAL AGE
ODDS DIFFERENCE
AND INSTRUCTIONS 4 5 6 7 8 12 13
Proport ............. 68 82 99 93 98
Prob.............. (61) (57) (71) (70) (73) (84) (88)
Diff............... 7 25 28 23 25 ...
Combined:
Proport ............. 70 90 99 97 99
Prob............... (56) (64) (77) (79) (77) (88) (90)
Diffr.............. 14 26 22 18 22
228
This content downloaded from 128.163.2.206 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 09:40:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
HARRY W. HOEMANN AND BRUCE M. ROSS
229
This content downloaded from 128.163.2.206 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 09:40:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
CHILD DEVELOPMENT
EXPERIMENT 3
The only procedural change for the two-spinner task in this experi-
ment as compared to Experiment 1 was that on each trial Ss were re-
quired by E to compare different rather than the same colors in the two
circles (e.g., spin for black in the left circle or spin for white in the right
circle). Faced with this task, the child's easiest course to obtain a correct
solution is simply to compare the magnitudes of the E-designated unlike
colors in each circle. Therefore performance with the modified double-
spinner task can be just as good as in Experiment 1.
The main point is, however, that to justify direct magnitude compar-
isons between circles the child must have made some use, if only implicit,
of fractional comparisons. For direct comparison of designated magni-
tudes of unlike colors is only legitimate if the child has some realization
that the denominators in the designated-color to complete-circle ratios of
each disk are equal. This fact should be especially transparent with the
present task since the two disks are always the same size. Nevertheless, it
was our hypothesis that even this minimal formulation of geometrically
represented fractions goes beyond the operations used by the younger
children in Experiment 1. Thus we predicted that results with the modi-
fied two-spinner task should be more like the results obtained with the
single-spinner task of Experiment 2 than the double-spinner task of
Experiment 1.
Method
230
This content downloaded from 128.163.2.206 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 09:40:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
HARRY W. HOEMANN AND BRUCE M. ROSS
black [E placed 1-inch square pieces of black poster board on the black
wedges of the designated disk] or here for white? [E placed 1-inch square
pieces of white poster board on the white wedges of the designated disk]."
The position and color of the correct response were in a random sequence
with odds differences and number of wedges occurring in the same order
as in Experiment 1. The procedure for the Single Spinner Condition was
exactly the same as in Experiment 2 except that there were eight trials
each at 1, , , and -L difference levels administered in a random order.
Results
EXPERIMENT 4
The possibility still remains that the reported results apply only to
the spinner situation. Experiment 4 was administered to extend the pres-
ent findings to a situation with two collections of discrete objects, making
TABLE 3
231
This content downloaded from 128.163.2.206 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 09:40:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
CHILD DEVELOPMENT
Method
232
This content downloaded from 128.163.2.206 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 09:40:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
HARRY W. HOEMANN AND BRUCE M. ROSS
tice trials with even choices (e.g., two pink and two green in the left jar
and four pink and four green in the right jar) administered to make sure
Ss understood the instructions. On all trials a star was recorded on the
answer sheet in view of the child whenever he drew out the specified
color.
With deaf children special care was taken to insure that they under-
stood instructions. Printed labels of "pink ball" and "green ball" were
matched to a ball of the appropriate color initially, and on every subse-
quent trial a pink ball was put in front of the jar where S was required to
predict "pink" and a green ball in front of the jar where he was required
to predict "green" by pointing.
Results
DISCUSSION
233
This content downloaded from 128.163.2.206 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 09:40:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
CHILD DEVELOPMENT
234
This content downloaded from 128.163.2.206 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 09:40:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
HARRY W. HOEMANN AND BRUCE M. ROSS
235
This content downloaded from 128.163.2.206 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 09:40:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
CHILD DEVELOPMENT
REFERENCES
236
This content downloaded from 128.163.2.206 on Mon, 27 Jun 2016 09:40:30 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms