European Psychiatry: T.M. Lincoln, N. Marin, E.S. Jaya
European Psychiatry: T.M. Lincoln, N. Marin, E.S. Jaya
European Psychiatry: T.M. Lincoln, N. Marin, E.S. Jaya
European Psychiatry
journal homepage: http://www.europsy-journal.com
Original article
A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T
Article history: Background: The causal role of childhood trauma for psychosis is well established, but the mechanisms
Received 14 October 2016 that link trauma to psychosis are largely unknown. Since childhood trauma is known to cause difficulties
Received in revised form 13 December 2016 in emotion regulation (ER) and patients with psychosis show impaired ER, we hypothesize that impaired
Accepted 18 December 2016
ER explains why people with a background of trauma are prone to psychotic experiences.
Available online 2 January 2017
Methods: The study used a longitudinal cohort design based on a community sample (N = 562) from
Germany, Indonesia, and the United States. Childhood trauma was assessed at baseline. ER and psychotic
Keywords:
experiences (defined as positive symptom frequency and related distress) were measured repeatedly at a
Schizophrenia and psychosis
Symptom distress
4-, 8-, and 12-month follow-up. Cross-lagged panel and longitudinal mediation analyses with structural
Social adversity equation modeling were used to test the predictive value of ER on psychotic experiences and its
Mediation mediating role in the association of childhood trauma and psychotic experiences.
Structural equation modeling Results: The cross-lagged paths from impaired ER to symptom distress (but not frequency) were
Prospective design significant. However, there was also evidence for the reverse causation from symptom frequency and
distress to impaired ER. ER partially mediated the significant prospective paths from childhood trauma
to symptom distress.
Conclusion: The findings demonstrate that ER plays a role in translating childhood trauma into
distressing psychotic experiences in later life. Moreover, the findings point to a maintenance mechanism
in which difficulties in ER and symptom distress exacerbate each other. Thus, ER could be a promising
target for interventions aimed at prevention of psychosis.
C 2016 Elsevier Masson SAS. All rights reserved.
The risk of psychosis is almost threefold for people with a psychosis develops, it can also help us to intervene earlier and
background of childhood trauma (CT) [1]. About 26% to 34% of the more effectively.
people diagnosed with a psychotic disorder have experienced CT Several studies have found negative affect, such as anxiety and
[2] that commonly includes sexual abuse [3,4], emotional abuse depression, to link different types of trauma to positive symptoms
[5–8] and physical abuse [9]. CT is associated with frequency and [4,15–17]. It seems intuitive to assume that persistent negative
severity of positive symptoms [10–12] and with a ten-fold increase affect might result from difficulties in emotion regulation (ER),
in symptom related distress [13]. Moreover, it has been found to which has been defined as the ‘‘processes responsible for
predict transition to psychosis in high risk samples [14]. The monitoring, evaluating, and modifying emotional reactions,
increasingly clear evidence for a causal role of CT in the especially their intensive and temporal features, to accomplish
development of psychosis has begun to inspire research on the one’s goals.’’ [18]. Building on this definition and synthesizing
putative mediating mechanisms. Knowing these mechanisms is established ER theories [19], Berking and colleagues [20]
not only crucial to a comprehensive understanding of how conceptualized adaptive ER as the ability to consciously process
emotions, to support oneself in emotionally distressing situations,
to actively modify negative emotions, to accept and tolerate
emotions and to confront emotionally distressing situations in
* Corresponding author at: Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Institute of
order to attain important goals.
Psychology, University of Hamburg, Von-Melle-Park 5, 20146 Hamburg, Germany.
Tel.: +49 40 42838 8087; fax: +49 40 42838 6170. Developmental and attachment theories point to various
E-mail address: jap.edo.sebastian.jaya@studium.uni-hamburg.de (E.S. Jaya). mechanisms underlying the development of ER, including
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2016.12.010
0924-9338/ C 2016 Elsevier Masson SAS. All rights reserved.
112 T.M. Lincoln et al. / European Psychiatry 42 (2017) 111–119
observational learning, modeling and social referencing [21], complete a follow-up survey after 4 (T1), 8 (T2), and 12 months
parenting style [22] and attachment relationships [23]. For (T3). The follow-up surveys were protected by password to ensure
instance, parental punishment or neglect of a child’s emotional that only participants who completed the baseline survey at T0
displays have been linked to maladaptive ER [24]. Prospective received the invitation for further participation. Recruitment was
studies show that early attachment predicts effective regulation conducted through Crowdflower and other websites (e.g. internet
strategies [25] and CT has been found to fundamentally disrupt forums and social networking websites). Crowdflower is a
attachment to the maltreating caregiver [26]. Unsurprisingly, thus, crowdsourcing Internet marketplace, similar to Amazon MTurk,
there is a bulk of research showing that CT compromises an on which people complete paid jobs. Participants recruited from
individual’s ability to regulate emotions effectively [27,28]. How- Crowdflower received 0.50 US$ for completing the baseline survey
ever, the causal inferences of these findings are limited due to the analog to the median hourly wage in Amazon MTurk [44]. In order
cross-sectional designs used. to motivate participants to complete the follow-up surveys, the
Psychosis has repeatedly been found related to difficulties in ER payment increased with each survey (T1, 0.60 US$; T2, 0.80 US$;
[29]. ‘‘Several studies have found psychosis to be associated with T3, 1.00 US$). Participants recruited from other websites were not
difficulties in being aware of [30,31], understanding, tolerating and given compensation for reasons of data security. Participants had
accepting one’s emotions [32–34], with using less functional to be at least 18 years old and provide written informed consent
strategies, such as reappraising the situation in a functional before entering the study. This study received ethical approval
manner [30,31,35,36] and with more avoidance or suppression of from the ethical commission of the German Psychological Society
emotions [33,34]. Moreover, difficulties in ER have also been (DGPs, TL062014_2).
associated with increased frequency of symptoms as well as There were 2501 completed baseline survey entries of which
exacerbated symptom distress [29,37]. It is noteworthy that these 151 were excluded due to duplicate entries (n = 98), longstring
difficulties are also prevalent in people at risk of psychosis, who (i.e. providing the same answer consecutively for 50 items,
have found to be characterized by lower emotion awareness n = 46 [45]), and inconsistent answers (n = 7). The baseline
[38,39], less use of reappraisal strategies [39,40] and more sample thus consisted of 2350 participants of whom 720 com-
suppression of emotions [39]. This indicates that difficulties in pleted the English, 786 the German and 844 the Indonesian
ER seem to precede the disorder and might contribute to its version of the survey. Of those participants, 432 completed
development. However, only few studies have looked into the first the follow-up (response rate = 18.4%), 300 completed the
temporal relationship between ER and psychosis. A small second follow-up (response rate = 12.8%), 256 completed the
community based study found maladaptive ER to prospectively third follow-up (response rate = 10.9%) and 139 completed all
predict psychotic symptoms from baseline to a 1-month follow-up follow-ups (response rate = 5.9%). A detailed participant flow-
assessment [36]. Moreover, ER-skills have been found to predict chart following the STROBE (Strengthening the Reporting of
increases in subjective distress and psychotic symptoms following Observational Studies in Epidemiology) guideline is available in
a stressor in individuals with psychosis [41]. Thus, there is Supplementary Fig. 1.
preliminary evidence for a causal role of ER in the development of There were 2501 completed baseline survey entries of which
psychosis, but longitudinal studies are needed to further corrobo- 151 were excluded due to duplicate entries (n = 98), longstring (i.e.
rate the postulated causal direction. providing the same answer consecutively for 50 items, n = 46 [45]),
To sum up: As CT appears to have a significant influence on ER and inconsistent answers (n = 7). The baseline sample thus
and difficulties in ER are evidently related to psychosis it seems consisted of 2350 participants of whom 720 completed the
reasonable to postulate that the ability to effectively regulate English, 786 the German and 844 the Indonesian version of the
emotions could at least partially explain the relationship between survey. Of those participants, 432 completed first follow-
CT and psychosis, especially as ER has been shown to mediate the up (response rate = 18.4%), 300 completed the second follow-up
association between CT and other psychopathologies (e.g. eating (response rate = 12.8%), 256 completed the third follow-
disorder [42] and depression [43]). Using a longitudinal design up (response rate = 10.9%) and 139 completed all follow-ups
with four assessment time-points we hypothesized that (1) CT will (response rate = 5.9%). A detailed participant flowchart following
significantly predict ER and psychotic experiences (defined as the STROBE (Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies
frequency of positive symptoms and related distress), (2) that the in Epidemiology) guideline is available in Supplementary Fig. 1.
inability to regulate emotions at one time-point will predict In total, 562 participants completed at least one follow-up
psychotic experiences at the following time-point, (3) and that the survey, fulfilled the inclusion criteria (i.e. complete entry, no
relationship between CT and psychotic experiences will be at least longstring, and ID match) and were included in the analyses.
partially mediated by ER.
1.2. Measures
1.2.2. Psychotic experiences All analyses were conducted with structural equation modeling
Subthreshold and clinically relevant psychotic experiences at (SEM) using the sem function in lavaan ver. 0.5–20 [52] in R, version
T1 through T3 were assessed with the Community Assessment of 3.2.3. Reported path coefficients are completely standardized.
Psychic Experiences (CAPE) [47], a 42-item self-report question- Reported overall total effect, overall direct effect, and overall
naire. The CAPE assesses experiences related to positive symptoms, indirect effect coefficients are unstandardized. The following
negative symptoms, and depression. The 20-item positive symp- indices and cut-off criteria were used to assess the fit between
tom subscale was used for the purpose of this study. It assesses a hypothesized models and the data: CFI 0.90, SRMR 0.08 [53];
range of psychotic experiences, such as unusual or persecutory RMSEA 0.08 [54]. Maximum likelihood procedure with robust
beliefs (e.g. ‘‘Do you ever feel as if you are under the control of some standard errors and a scaled test statistic, asymptotically equaling
force or power other than yourself?’’, ‘‘Have you ever felt that you Yuan–Bentler test statistic, was used to correct for non-normal
were being persecuted in any way?’’) or hallucinatory phenomena distribution. As we found that the frequency of psychotic
(e.g. ‘‘Have your thoughts ever been so vivid that you were worried experiences across measurement points was not missing complete-
other people would hear them?’’, ‘‘Do you ever hear voices when ly at random (MCAR; x2(16) = 101.73, P < .001), subsequent
you are alone?’’) in the past four weeks on both a frequency scale analyses used full information maximum likelihood (FIML) with
(ranging from 1 = never to 4 = nearly always) and a distress scale a missing at random (MAR) assumption. Variable inter-correlations
(i.e. ‘‘Please indicate how distressed you are by this experience’’; and missing status are shown in Supplementary Table 1.
ranging from 1 = not distressed, 4 = very distressed). Previous Before testing the mediation hypotheses, we computed an
studies have demonstrated good convergent and discriminative autocorrelation model to test the precondition that CT, ER and
validity [48] and the positive symptom subscale has good retest psychotic experiences are correlated and within-construct auto-
reliability (r = 0.63) [49]. regressive models to test the precondition that the values of
variables at future time points depend at least in part on their
1.2.3. Emotion regulation respective values at earlier time points.
Functional ER-skills at T1 through T3 were assessed with the The mediation analysis was based on Baron and Kenny’s [55]
Emotion Regulation Skills Questionnaire (ERSQ) [50] derived from principles for mediation and followed suggestions by Cole and
the theoretical conceptualization by Berking et al. [20]. The ERSQ is Maxwell [56] for mediation testing in longitudinal designs. First,
a 27-item self-report measure that assesses the application of ER- we examined the prospective paths from CT at T0 to frequency and
skills during the previous week on a five-point Likert-type scale distress of psychotic experiences at T1–T3 (path c), CT to ER at T1–
(1 = not at all, 5 = almost always). It contains nine scales that T3 (path a), and ER at T1–T3 to frequency and distress of psychotic
correspond to the following nine skills: (a) consciously process experiences at T1–T3 (path b) in separate models. Specifically, we
emotions/be aware of emotions, (b) identify and label emotions, (c) examined path b with a cross-lagged panel model to take into
interpret emotion-related body sensations correctly, (d) under- account various sources of error such as the stability of the
stand the prompts of emotions, (e) support oneself in emotionally variables, cross-sectional associations, prior associations and the
distressing situations, (f) actively modify negative emotions in possibility of a reverse pathway.
order to feel better, (g) accept emotions, (h) be resilient to/tolerate According to Cole and Maxwell [56], path c (in this case the path
negative emotions, and (i) confront emotionally distressing from CT-PE to the final follow-up at T3) needs to be significant in
situations in order to attain important goals. Both the total score order to conduct the longitudinal mediation analysis and compute
and the subscales of the ERSQ show good internal consistencies the overall total, direct and indirect effect coefficients. These effect
(Cronbach’s a = 0.90, and 0.68–0.81, respectively) and adequate coefficients were considered significant if the bias-corrected
retest-reliability (rtt = 0.75 and 0.48–0.74, respectively). All scales bootstrap 95% confidence interval (BCa CI) did not include zero
have demonstrated convergent and discriminate validity, includ- [57]. ER was considered a significant mediator if the indirect effect
ing strong positive correlations with constructs related to ER [50]. coefficient was significant [58].
Table 1
Participants’ characteristics.
Table 2
Fit indices of the tested models.
CT on symptom frequency (T1) 25.76 13 0.02 0.04 [0.020–0.063] 0.02 0.99 7290
CT on symptom distress (T1) 21.19 13 0.07 0.03 [0.008–0.054] 0.04 0.99 6775
CT on symptom frequency (T2) 21.55 13 0.06 0.03 [0.004–0.057] 0.03 0.99 7194
CT on symptom distress (T2) 22.94 13 0.04 0.04 [0.014–0.057] 0.03 0.99 6644
CT on symptom frequency (T3) 26.33 13 0.02 0.04 [0.022–0.063] 0.02 0.99 7028
CT on symptom distress (T3) 21.85 13 0.06 0.04 [0.010–0.056] 0.03 0.99 6611
CT on ER (T1) 22.97 13 0.04 0.04 [0.010–0.060] 0.03 0.99 8005
CT on ER (T2) 25.69 13 0.02 0.04 [0.019–0.063] 0.03 0.99 7627
CT on ER (T3) 16.09 13 0.24 0.02 [0.000–0.046] 0.03 0.99 7441
ER and symptom frequency 517.93 124 <0.01 0.08 [0.069–0.082] 0.08 0.93 3554
ER and symptom distress 418.81 124 <0.01 0.07 [0.059–0.072] 0.09 0.95 2239
Longitudinal mediation model of CT, ER, and symptom frequency 611.55 193 <0.01 0.06 [0.057–0.068] 0.07 0.94 10 262
Longitudinal mediation model of CT, ER, and symptom distress 508.69 192 <0.01 0.05 [0.054–0.066] 0.08 0.95 8930
Note. CT: childhood trauma; ER: emotion regulation; T0: baseline; T1: month 4; T2: month 8; T3: month 12; RMSEA: Root Mean Square Error of Approximation; SRMR:
Standardized Root Mean Square Residual; CFI: Comparative Fit Index; AIC: Akaike Information Criterion; 90%CI: 90% confidence interval.
a
Rounded to the next integer.
Additionally, all fit indices were met, indicating that the significant path from CT to symptom distress (g = 0.11, P = .12) at T3
parameters can be interpreted (see Table 2). was now non-significant. Furthermore, there was a significant path
from ER (T2) to symptom distress (T3, b = 0.09, P < .01) indicating
2.3. Prospective association of childhood trauma, emotional a possible mediation of ER in the relationship between CT and
regulation, and psychotic experiences distress. Note, however, that there was also a significant path from
both symptom frequency (b = 0.12, P < .05) and distress
There were significant prospective paths from CT (as assessed at (b = 0.16, P < .01) at T1 to ER at T2, indicating that psychotic
T0) to symptom frequency and distress at T3, and thus a significant experiences do not just follow but also precede difficulties in ER.
path c (see Fig. 1a for details). Moreover, there was a significant Since the hypothesized paths from ER to symptom frequency
prospective path from CT to ER at T2, and thus a significant path a were not significant, the overall total, direct and indirect effect was
(see Fig. 1b for details). only computed for symptom distress. The overall total effect of CT
The prospective association between ER and psychotic expe- on symptom distress at final follow up was significant at 0.0747
riences was tested with a cross lagged panel model (see Fig. 1c). (95% BCa CI, 0.0311, 0.1184). This effect consisted of a significant
Here we found a significant prospective path from ER at T2 to overall direct effect of 0.0694 (95% BCa CI, 0.0269, 0.1119) and a
symptom distress at T3, but no significant prospective paths from significant overall indirect effect of 0.0053 (95% BCa CI, 0.0002,
ER to symptom frequency. Thus, the path b was only significant for 0.0104). The mediation proportion [59] showed that ER mediated
symptom distress. 7.1% of the overall total effect of CT on symptom distress.
In the longitudinal mediation model (see Fig. 2), which included In order to explore whether the associations of interest differ
all time-points and the putative mediator ER, the previously across symptoms, we repeated the analyses using the hallucination
T.M. Lincoln et al. / European Psychiatry 42 (2017) 111–119 115
Fig. 2. Longitudinal mediation analysis of childhood trauma, emotion regulation, and psychotic experiences. Note. (distress); [frequencies]; path coefficients are completely
standardized; +P < .10, *P < .05, **P < .001. Dotted lines indicate non-significant paths. CT: childhood trauma; ER: emotion regulation; PE: psychotic experiences; T0:
baseline; T1: month 4; T2: month 8; T3: month 12.
116 T.M. Lincoln et al. / European Psychiatry 42 (2017) 111–119
0.049, 0.149) and a significant overall indirect effect at 0.010 (95% ties in which people appear to be getting caught up in a vicious
CI, 0.001, 0.020). Thus, ER was a significant partial mediator in the circle of psychotic experiences, negative affect and difficulties in
association between CT and paranoia frequency and distress. down-regulating negative emotions. This may explain symptom
Furthermore, we investigated differences between countries in exacerbation over longer time-periods.
our variables of interest. We did not find significant mean The significant overall indirect effect indicates that ER partially
differences between countries in regard to CT, ER, and symptom explains the link between CT and symptom distress. This has not
distress. However, we did find significant differences between been investigated in regard to psychosis so far and thus makes a
countries in symptom frequency (F(2,429) = 14.60, P < 0.01), with novel contribution to the field. However, the mediating effect was
post hoc t-tests with Bonferroni correction indicating psychotic only found for symptom distress, not for symptom frequency.
experiences to be significantly more frequent in participants from Taken together with research showing that ER also mediates
Indonesia (M = 1.60) than in those from Germany (M = 1.36) and between CT and other affective disorders [65] one could speculate
the United States (M = 1.44). that ER may be more relevant to the affective symptomatology
Finally, in order to rule out that ER was merely a proxy for associated with psychosis than to the specific psychotic symp-
substance abuse, we analyzed correlations between ER and the use tomatology as such. Thus, the specificity warrants further research.
of alcohol, cannabis, and other substances, which were all found to Moreover, the mediation was partial, explaining 7.1% of the total
be non-significant. relationship between trauma and symptom distress. This indicates
that other putative mediators should be considered, candidates
being negative schema [15,66], social cognition (e.g. impaired
3. Discussion theory of mind [67,68]), neurocognitive impairment [69], sub-
stance abuse [70], and ongoing social adversities [71]. In addition
3.1. Summary of findings and general discussion to the reduced functional ER strategies as assessed in this study
(e.g. acceptance, tolerance and clarity of emotions), increased
This study tested for a significant association of CT and dysfunctional ER strategies (e.g. rumination, suppression, or
psychotic experiences, a predictive effect of ER on psychotic avoidance), might also contribute to mediating the relationship
experiences and a mediating role of ER in the relationship of CT and of CT and symptom distress, in particular because these types of ER
psychotic experiences. As hypothesized, and in line with the have been found to be linked to CT [43,72] and to exacerbated
extensive literature highlighting maltreatment as a major risk symptom distress in psychosis [29,73,74]. Notably, the focus on
factor for later psychosis [9] we found CT to be significantly related functional rather than dysfunctional ER in our study might explain
to psychotic experiences. Confirming part of our second hypothe- why we did not find a significant correlation between substance
sis, we found that the ability to regulate emotions significantly and use and ER as has been found in other studies [70].
prospectively predicted fluctuations in symptom distress over Our additional analyses of the effects for paranoia and
time. Thus, the fewer ER skills an individual endorsed at a given hallucinations suggest that although CT was linked to both
time-point, the higher the likelihood that this individual would be paranoia and hallucinations the mediating role of ER may be
distressed by psychotic experiences at the next time-point. This specific to paranoia. However, the insignificant finding for
finding adds to the preliminary evidence indicating a covariation of hallucinations needs to be interpreted with caution because the
at risk-mental states and ER [38–40], a causal influence of ER on hallucination subscale only has four items. Thus, the question of
psychotic symptoms and distress [36,41], and suggests that the specificity warrants further research with more suitable measures
ability to effectively regulate emotions is crucial to arrive at low for this purpose.
levels of symptom distress.
Given the overwhelming evidence for affective pathways to 3.2. Limitations
psychosis [61,62], we were surprised by the absence of a significant
path from ER to symptom frequency. Possibly, the mere presence The participants were drawn from a pool of people with access
of non-distressing low-threshold delusional beliefs or perceptual to the Internet. These participants tended to be highly educated
phenomenon as assessed in the frequency domain of the CAPE and to have middle to low income. Similar sample characteristics
might not have sufficient pathological character to find significant have been reported by other researchers who used crowdsourcing
associations with putative risk factors for psychosis. Psychotic websites for recruitment [75]. Due to the slightly reduced
symptom distress has been found to be a better discriminator variability of the participants’ demographics, the strength of the
between clinical and non-clinical samples than frequency [63] and observed relationships might have been underestimated. Howev-
to improve the prediction of high risk status relative to frequency er, the sample was sufficiently heterogeneous in regard to the
of psychotic experiences on their own [64]. Thus, it appears to be a variables of interest: childhood trauma and psychotic experiences.
more valid indicator of relevant subclinical symptomatology. Moreover, the inclusion of individuals from three countries adds to
However, this assumption warrants further research that includes the representativeness of the sample.
measures of clinical relevance (e.g. functioning). Another limitation is that only 23% of the baseline-group
Moreover, interestingly, our test of the ‘‘reverse causation participated in the follow-ups and only 5.9% of the initial sample
model’’ revealed that symptom frequency and related distress at completed all assessments. Although we used FIML procedure in
one time-point also significantly predicted an impaired ability to all analyses to take into account the missing data, we cannot rule
regulate emotions at the next time-point. This could be explicable out that the high attrition rate influenced the results. The
by the fact that psychotic experiences demand attention, leaving correlates of missing data reported in Supplementary Table 1
less attentional capacity to deal with distressing emotions, an suggest that participants with more severe symptoms were more
effortful process it itself. Moreover, people who are feeling likely to miss follow-up assessments. Selective drop-out at the
distressed by psychotic symptoms are dealing with higher levels higher end of the symptom spectrum could have resulted in
of negative affect that are more difficult to down-regulate (causing reduced variability and thus in attenuated effect sizes [76].
them to report less success in ER) than the milder levels of Another limitation is that only 23% of the baseline-group
emotional distress people without disturbing psychotic experien- participated in the follow-ups and only 5.9% of the initial sample
ces are faced with. The longitudinal pattern of associations speaks completed all assessments. Although we used FIML procedure in
for reciprocal exacerbations of symptom distress and ER difficul- all analyses to take into account the missing data, we cannot rule
T.M. Lincoln et al. / European Psychiatry 42 (2017) 111–119 117
out that the high attrition rate influenced the results. The pathway linking trauma to psychosis by heightened emotional
correlates of missing data reported in Supplementary Table 1 distress, anxiety and depression [16,17]. Our findings extend this
suggest that participants with more severe symptoms were more line of research by pointing to the role of ER as a possible
likely to miss follow-up assessments. Selective drop-out at the psychological mechanism involved in this process. Although this
higher end of the symptom spectrum could have resulted in mechanism is unlikely to be specific to psychosis, the fact that
reduced variability and thus in attenuated effect sizes [76]. difficulties in ER precede and follow from symptom distress render
Another issue is that the retrospective assessment of CT is prone ER a promising target for interventions aimed at increasing well-
to recall bias [77]. Similarly, the self-report diagnosis cannot be being and preventing psychotic symptom exacerbation.
externally verified. However, the minimal reward for survey
termination, the application of data verification procedures and
Disclosure of interest
the similarity of prevalence rates of life-time mental diagnosis
(32.1%) to representative samples from epidemiological studies
The authors declare that they have no competing interest.
(e.g. 29.4% [78]) speak for the validity of the data. Furthermore, the
mean CAPE scores for frequency of positive symptoms across all
assessment time-points are comparable to those found in a large
data set of previously published data [60]. People who reported no Appendix A. Supplementary data
mental disorder in our study had a mean score of M = 1.44
(compared to M = 1.41 in other population samples [60]) and those Supplementary data associated with this article can be found, in
who reported a psychotic disorder had a mean value of M = 1.52 the online version, at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2016.12.
(compared to a mean value of M = 1.73 for participants with a 010.
psychotic disorder in other studies [60]).
Moreover, our measure of ER assessed what the participant References
remembered to have done in response to emotions when they
arose during the past week rather than assessing ‘‘online ER’’ (e.g. [1] Varese F, Smeets F, Drukker M, Lieverse R, Lataster T, Viechtbauer W, et al.
by using experience sampling [79] or online ER in a standardized Childhood adversities increase the risk of psychosis: a meta-analysis of
patient-control, prospective- and cross-sectional cohort studies. Schizophr
paradigm [80], which might have led to more variability in ER over Bull 2012;38:661–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbs050.
time. Nevertheless, we found a sufficient variation in the use of ER [2] Bonoldi I, Simeone E, Rocchetti M, Codjoe L, Rossi G, Gambi F, et al. Prevalence
strategies (with correlations between time-points of around 0.7, of self-reported childhood abuse in psychosis: a meta-analysis of retrospective
studies. Psychiatry Res 2013;210:8–15.
see Fig. 1c), which was similar to the stability found for psychotic
[3] Bebbington P, Bhugra D, Brugha T, Singleton N, Farrell M, Jenkins R, et al.
experiences and can be considered sufficient to investigate Psychosis, victimisation and childhood disadvantage. Br J Psychiatry
whether the inability to regulate emotions at one time-point will 2004;185:220–6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1192/bjp.185.3.220.
predict psychotic experiences at the following time-point as [4] Bebbington P, Jonas S, Kuipers E, King M, Cooper C, Brugha T, et al. Childhood
sexual abuse and psychosis: data from a cross-sectional national psychiatric
hypothesized. survey in England. Br J Psychiatry 2011;199:29–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1192/
Lastly, the analytical strategy may have overestimated the bjp.bp.110.083642.
stability of ER and symptom frequency and distress while [5] Ackner S, Skeate A, Patterson P, Neal A. Emotional abuse and psychosis: a
recent review of the literature. J Aggress Maltreatment Trauma
underestimating cross-lagged effects [81]. This is partly due to 2013;22:1032–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10926771.2013.837132.
the shared method factor (i.e. psychotic experiences at all time- [6] Ashcroft K, Kingdon DG, Chadwick P. Persecutory delusions and childhood
points were measured with the exact same items). However, we emotional abuse in people with a diagnosis of schizophrenia. Psychosis
2012;4:168–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17522439.2011.619012.
opted for this more conservative approach keeping the probability [7] Duhig M, Patterson S, Connell M, Foley S, Capra C, Dark F, et al. The prevalence
of type I error low because the study was not exploratory in nature. and correlates of childhood trauma in patients with early psychosis. Aust N Z J
Psychiatry 2015;49:651–9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0004867415575379.
[8] Morrison AP, Petersen T. Trauma, metacognition and predisposition to hallu-
3.3. Therapeutic implications
cinations in non-patients. Behav Cogn Psychother 2003;31:235–46. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1352465803003011.
The results indicate that targeting ER in victims of childhood [9] Arseneault L, Cannon M, Fisher HL, Polanczyk G, Moffitt TE, Caspi A. Childhood
trauma and children’s emerging psychotic symptoms: a genetically sensitive
trauma and in those at risk of psychosis could be a promising way
longitudinal cohort study. Am J Psychiatry 2011;168:65–72. http://dx.doi.org/
to interrupt the cascade of negative affect, psychotic experiences 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10040567.
and distress. There are several approaches available that can be [10] Bentall RP, Wickham S, Shevlin M, Varese F. Do specific early-life adversities
employed or used to inform novel interventions. For example, lead to specific symptoms of psychosis? A study from the 2007 the adult
psychiatric morbidity survey. Schizophr Bull 2012;sbs049. http://dx.doi.org/
third-wave approaches, such as Acceptance and Commitment 10.1093/schbul/sbs049.
Therapy (ACT) [82] and Emotion Focused Therapy (EFT) [83] aim at [11] Kilcommons AM, Morrison AP, Knight A, Lobban F. Psychotic experiences in
enhancing the ability to tolerate negative affective states and to people who have been sexually assaulted. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol
2008;43:602–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00127-007-0303-z.
accept the presence of difficult thoughts or feelings [83,84] and [12] Sitko K, Bentall RP, Shevlin M, O’Sullivan N, Sellwood W. Associations between
have shown promising effects in the treatment of psychosis specific psychotic symptoms and specific childhood adversities are mediated
[82,85]. Moreover, there are programs that focus solely on ER by by attachment styles: an analysis of the National Comorbidity Survey. Psy-
chiatry Res 2014;217:202–9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2014.
training adaptive ER-skills such as acceptance, tolerance, reap- 03.019.
praisal and problem solving [20,86] and these programs have been [13] Bak M, Krabbendam L, Janssen I, Graaf R, Vollebergh W, Os J. Early trauma may
found to be promising in various inpatient populations increase the risk for psychotic experiences by impacting on emotional re-
sponse and perception of control. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2005;112:360–6.
[20,87]. These types of trainings could potentially be beneficial
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00646.x.
to reducing transition to psychosis – possibly as an add-on to [14] Thompson AD, Nelson B, Yuen HP, Lin A, Amminger GP, McGorry PD, et al.
existing approaches – especially if they are tailored to improve a Sexual trauma increases the risk of developing psychosis in an ultra high-risk
‘‘prodromal’’ population. Schizophr Bull 2014;40:697–706. http://dx.doi.org/
person’s individual profile of ER skills.
10.1093/schbul/sbt032.
[15] Fisher HL, Appiah-Kusi E, Grant C. Anxiety and negative self-schemas mediate
4. Conclusion the association between childhood maltreatment and paranoia. Psychiatry Res
2012;196:323–4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.12.004.
[16] Freeman D, Fowler D. Routes to psychotic symptoms: trauma, anxiety and
Researchers have worked toward identifying pathways from CT psychosis-like experiences. Psychiatry Res 2009;169:107–12. http://
to psychosis [88] and have put forward the idea of an affective dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2008.07.009.
118 T.M. Lincoln et al. / European Psychiatry 42 (2017) 111–119
[17] Marwaha S, Bebbington P. Mood as a mediator of the link between child sexual contributing to depression? J Affect Disord 2015;174:287–95. http://
abuse and psychosis. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2014;1–3. http:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2014.11.028.
dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00127-014-0966-1. [44] Buhrmester M, Kwang T, Gosling SD. Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: a new source
[18] Thompson RA. Emotion regulation: a theme in search of definition. Monogr of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspect Psychol Sci 2011;6:3–5.
Soc Res Child Dev 1994;59:25–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5834. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691610393980.
1994.tb01276.x. [45] Johnson JA. Ascertaining the validity of individual protocols from Web-based
[19] Gross JJ. The emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative review. Rev personality inventories. J Res Personal 2005;39:103–29. http://dx.doi.org/
Gen Psychol 1998;2:271–99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271. 10.1016/j.jrp.2004.09.009.
[20] Berking M, Wupperman P, Reichardt A, Pejic T, Dippel A, Znoj H. Emotion- [46] Janssen I, Krabbendam L, Bak M, Hanssen M, Vollebergh W, de Graaf R, et al.
regulation skills as a treatment target in psychotherapy. Behav Res Ther Childhood abuse as a risk factor for psychotic experiences. Acta Psychiatr Scand
2008;46:1230–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2008.08.005. 2004;109:38–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.0001-690X. 2003.00217.x.
[21] Morris AS, Silk JS, Steinberg L, Myers SS, Robinson LR. The role of the family [47] Stefanis NC, Hanssen M, Smirnis NK, Avramopoulos DA, Evdokimidis IK,
context in the development of emotion regulation. Soc Dev 2007;16:361–88. Stefanis CN, et al. Evidence that three dimensions of psychosis have a distri-
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x. bution in the general population. Psychol Med 2002;32:347–58.
[22] Eisenberg N, Cumberland A, Spinrad TL. Parental socialization of emotion. [48] Hanssen M, Peeters F, Krabbendam L, Radstake S, Verdoux H, van Os J. How
Psychol Inq 1998;9:241–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli0904_1. psychotic are individuals with non-psychotic disorders? Soc Psychiatry Psy-
[23] Harder S. Attachment in schizophrenia—implications for research, prevention, chiatr Epidemiol 2003;38:149–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00127-003-
and treatment. Schizophr Bull 2014;sbu133. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ 0622-7.
schbul/sbu133. [49] Konings M, Bak M, Hanssen M, van Os J, Krabbendam L. Validity and reliability
[24] Waller G, Corstorphine E, Mountford V. The role of emotional abuse in the of the CAPE: a self-report instrument for the measurement of psychotic
eating disorders: implications for treatment. Eat Disord 2007;15:317–31. experiences in the general population. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2006;114:55–
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10640260701454337. 61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00741.x.
[25] Gilliom M, Shaw DS, Beck JE, Schonberg MA, Lukon JL. Anger regulation in [50] Berking M, Znoj H. Entwicklung und Validierung eines Fragebogens zur
disadvantaged preschool boys: strategies, antecedents, and the development standardisierten Selbsteinschätzung emotionaler Kompetenzen (SEK-27). Z
of self-control. Dev Psychol 2002;38:222–35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012- Für Psychiatr Psychol Psychother 2008;56:141–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1024/
1649.38.2.222. 1661-4747.56.2.141.
[26] Davies PT, Cummings EM. Marital conflict and child adjustment: an emotional [51] Smeets R, Dingemans P. Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI).
security hypothesis. Psychol Bull 1994;116:387–411. http://dx.doi.org/ Version 1.1. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 1993.
10.1037/0033-2909.116.3.387. [52] Rosseel Y. lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. J Stat Softw
[27] Gratz KL, Bornovalova MA, Delany-Brumsey A, Nick B, Lejuez CW. A laborato- 2012;48:1–36.
ry-based study of the relationship between childhood abuse and experiential [53] Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis:
avoidance among inner-city substance users: the role of emotional nonaccep- conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Model Multidiscip J
tance. Behav Ther 2007;38:256–68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2006. 1999;6:1–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118.
08.006. [54] MacCallum RC, Browne MW, Sugawara HM. Power analysis and determination
[28] Shields A, Cicchetti D. Reactive aggression among maltreated children: the of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psychol Methods
contributions of attention and emotion dysregulation. J Clin Child Psychol 1996;1:130–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.1.2.130.
1998;27:381–95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp2704_2. [55] Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social
[29] Khoury B, Lecomte T. Emotion regulation and schizophrenia. Int J Cogn Ther psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J
2012;5:67–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/ijct.2012.5.1.67. Pers Soc Psychol 1986;51:1173–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.
[30] Kimhy D, Vakhrusheva J, Jobson-Ahmed L, Tarrier N, Malaspina D, Gross JJ. 51.6.1173.
Emotion awareness and regulation in individuals with schizophrenia: impli- [56] Cole DA, Maxwell SE. Testing mediational models with longitudinal data:
cations for social functioning. Psychiatry Res 2012;200:193–201. http:// questions and tips in the use of structural equation modeling. J Abnorm
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.05.029. Psychol 2003;112:558–77. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.112.4.558.
[31] van der Meer L, van’t Wout M, Aleman A. Emotion regulation strategies in [57] Cheung GW, Lau RS. Testing mediation and suppression effects of latent
patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 2009;170:108–13. http:// variables bootstrapping with structural equation models. Organ Res Methods
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2009.07.010. 2008;11:296–325. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1094428107300343.
[32] Lincoln TM, Hartmann M, Köther U, Moritz S. Do people with psychosis have [58] Mayer A, Thoemmes F, Rose N, Steyer R, West SG. Theory and analysis of total,
specific difficulties regulating emotions? Clin Psychol Psychother 2014. http:// direct, and indirect causal effects. Multivar Behav Res 2014;49:425–42. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1002/cpp.1923. dx.doi.org/10.1080/00273171.2014.931797.
[33] Perry Y, Henry JD, Grisham JR. The habitual use of emotion regulation strate- [59] Ditlevsen S, Christensen U, Lynch J, Damsgaard MT, Keiding N. The mediation
gies in schizophrenia. Br J Clin Psychol 2011;50:217–22. http://dx.doi.org/ proportion: a structural equation approach for estimating the proportion of
10.1111/j.2044-8260.2010.02001.x. exposure effect on outcome explained by an intermediate variable. Epidemiol
[34] Perry Y, Henry JD, Nangle MR, Grisham JR. Regulation of negative affect in Camb Mass 2005;16:114–20.
schizophrenia: the effectiveness of acceptance versus reappraisal and sup- [60] Schlier B, Jaya ES, Moritz S, Lincoln TM. The Community Assessment of Psychic
pression. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2012;34:497–508. http://dx.doi.org/ Experiences measures nine clusters of psychosis-like experiences: a validation
10.1080/13803395.2012.661405. of the German version of the CAPE. Schizophr Res 2015;169:274–9. http://
[35] Livingstone K, Harper S, Gillanders D. An exploration of emotion regulation in dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2015.10.034.
psychosis. Clin Psychol Psychother 2009;16:418–30. http://dx.doi.org/ [61] Myin-Germeys I, van Os J. Stress-reactivity in psychosis: evidence for an
10.1002/cpp.635. affective pathway to psychosis. Clin Psychol Rev 2007;27:409–24. http://
[36] Westermann S, Boden MT, Gross JJ, Lincoln TM. Maladaptive cognitive emo- dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2006.09.005.
tion regulation prospectively predicts subclinical paranoia. Cogn Ther Res [62] Freeman D, Stahl D, McManus S, Meltzer H, Brugha T, Wiles N, et al. Insomnia,
2013;37:881–5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10608-013-9523-6. worry, anxiety and depression as predictors of the occurrence and persistence
[37] Badcock JC, Paulik G, Maybery MT. The role of emotion regulation in auditory of paranoid thinking. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2011;47:1195–203.
hallucinations. Psychiatry Res 2011;185:303–8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00127-011-0433-1.
j.psychres.2010.07.011. [63] Lincoln TM. Relevant dimensions of delusions: continuing the continuum
[38] van Rijn S, Schothorst P, van’t Wout M, Sprong M, Ziermans T, van Engeland H, versus category debate. Schizophr Res 2007;93:211–20. http://dx.doi.org/
et al. Affective dysfunctions in adolescents at risk for psychosis: emotion 10.1016/j.schres.2007.02.013.
awareness and social functioning. Psychiatry Res 2011;187:100–5. http:// [64] Kline E, Thompson E, Bussell K, Pitts SC, Reeves G, Schiffman J. Psychosis-like
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2010.10.007. experiences and distress among adolescents using mental health services.
[39] Kimhy D, Gill KE, Brucato G, Vakhrusheva J, Arndt L, Gross JJ, et al. The impact Schizophr Res 2014;152:498–502. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2013.
of emotion awareness and regulation on social functioning in individuals at 12.012.
clinical high risk for psychosis. Psychol Med 2016;46:2907–18. http:// [65] Carvalho Fernando S, Beblo T, Schlosser N, Terfehr K, Otte C, Löwe B, et al. The
dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291716000490. impact of self-reported childhood trauma on emotion regulation in borderline
[40] van der Velde J, Opmeer EM, Liemburg EJ, Bruggeman R, Nieboer R, Wunderink personality disorder and major depression. J Trauma Dissociation
L, et al. Lower prefrontal activation during emotion regulation in subjects at 2014;15:384–401. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15299732.2013.863262.
ultrahigh risk for psychosis: an fMRI-study. NPJ Schizophr 2015;1:15026. [66] Jaya ES, Ascone L, Lincoln TM. Social adversity and psychosis: the mediating
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/npjschz.2015.26. role of cognitive vulnerability. Schizophr Bull 2016;sbw104. http://dx.doi.org/
[41] Lincoln TM, Hartmann M, Köther U, Moritz S. Dealing with feeling: specific 10.1093/schbul/sbw104.
emotion regulation skills predict responses to stress in psychosis. Psychiatry [67] Allen P, Freeman D, McGuire P, Garety P, Kuipers E, Fowler D, et al. The
Res 2015;228:216–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2015.04.003. prediction of hallucinatory predisposition in non-clinical individuals: exam-
[42] Racine SE, Wildes JE. Emotion dysregulation and anorexia nervosa: an explo- ining the contribution of emotion and reasoning. Br J Clin Psychol
ration of the role of childhood abuse. Int J Eat Disord 2014;48:55–8. http:// 2005;44:127–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/014466504x20044.
dx.doi.org/10.1002/eat.22364. [68] Garety PA, Kuipers E, Fowler D, Freeman D, Bebbington P. A cognitive model of
[43] O’Mahen HA, Karl A, Moberly N, Fedock G. The association between childhood the positive symptoms of psychosis. Psychol Med 2001;31:189–95. http://
maltreatment and emotion regulation: two different mechanisms dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291701003312.
T.M. Lincoln et al. / European Psychiatry 42 (2017) 111–119 119
[69] Lysaker P, Meyer P, Evans J, Marks K. Neurocognitive and symptom correlates [79] Catterson AD, Eldesouky L, John OP. An experience sampling approach to
of self-reported childhood sexual abuse in schizophrenia spectrum emotion regulation: situational suppression use and social hierarchy. J Res
disorders. Ann Clin Psychiatry 2001;13:89–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/ Personal 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2016.04.004.
10401230109148953. [80] Sheppes G, Meiran N. Better late than never? On the dynamics of
[70] Mandavia A, Robinson GGN, Bradley B, Ressler KJ, Powers A. Exposure to online regulation of sadness using distraction and cognitive reappraisal.
childhood abuse and later substance use: indirect effects of emotion dysre- Pers Soc Psychol Bull 2007;33:1518–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
gulation and exposure to trauma. J Trauma Stress 2016;29:422–9. http:// 0146167207305537.
dx.doi.org/10.1002/jts.22131. [81] Cole DA, Martin NC, Steiger JH. Empirical and conceptual problems with
[71] Jaya ES, Lincoln TM. Social adversities and psychotic symptoms: a test of longitudinal trait-state models: introducing a trait-state-occasion model.
predictions derived from the social defeat hypothesis. Psychiatry Res Psychol Methods 2005;10:3–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.10.1.3.
2016;245:466–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2016.09.002. [82] Bach P, Hayes SC. The use of acceptance and commitment therapy to prevent
[72] Heleniak C, Jenness JL, Vander Stoep A, McCauley E, McLaughlin KA. Childhood the rehospitalization of psychotic patients: a randomized controlled trial. J
maltreatment exposure and disruptions in emotion regulation: a transdia- Consult Clin Psychol 2002;70:1129–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-
gnostic pathway to adolescent internalizing and externalizing psychopathol- 006x.70.5.1129.
ogy. Cogn Ther Res 2015;40:394–415. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10608-015- [83] Greenberg LS. Emotion-focused therapy: coaching clients to work through
9735-z. their feelings, vol. xvi. Washington, DC, USA: American Psychological Associ-
[73] Morrison AP, Haddock G, Tarrier N. Intrusive thoughts and auditory halluci- ation; 2002.
nations: a cognitive approach. Behav Cogn Psychother 1995;23:265. http:// [84] Hayes SC, Strosahl KD, Wilson KG. Acceptance and commitment therapy: an
dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1352465800015873. experiential approach to behavior change, vol. xvi. New York, NY, USA:
[74] Morrison AP, Wells A. Relationships between worry, psychotic experiences Guilford Press; 1999.
and emotional distress in patients with schizophrenia spectrum diagnoses and [85] White R, Gumley A, McTaggart J, Rattrie L, McConville D, Cleare S, et al. A
comparisons with anxious and non-patient groups. Behav Res Ther feasibility study of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy for emotional
2007;45:1593–600. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2006.11.010. dysfunction following psychosis. Behav Res Ther 2011;49:901–7. http://
[75] Shapiro DN, Chandler J, Mueller PA. Using mechanical turk to study clinical dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2011.09.003.
populations. Clin Psychol Sci 2013;1:213–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ [86] Barnow S, Löw C, Dodek A, Stopsack M. Gefühle im Griff – Emotionen intelli-
2167702612469015. gent regulieren. PPmP – Psychother Psychosom Med Psychol 2014;64:284–
[76] Terluin B, de Boer MR, de Vet HCW. Differences in connection strength 9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1055/s-0033-1363683.
between mental symptoms might be explained by differences in variance: [87] Berking M, Ebert D, Cuijpers P, Hofmann SG. Emotion regulation skills training
reanalysis of network data did not confirm staging. PLOS ONE enhances the efficacy of inpatient cognitive behavioral therapy for major
2016;11:e0155205. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0155205. depressive disorder: a randomized controlled trial. Psychother Psychosom
[77] Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Woodward LJ. The stability of child abuse reports: 2013;82:234–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000348448.
a longitudinal study of the reporting behaviour of young adults. Psychol Med [88] Isvoranu A-M, van Borkulo CD, Boyette L-L, Wigman JTW, Vinkers CH, Bors-
2000;30:529–44. boom D, et al. A network approach to psychosis: pathways between childhood
[78] Kessler RC, Demler O, Frank RG, Olfson M, Pincus HA, Walters EE, et al. trauma and psychotic symptoms. Schizophr Bull 2016;sbw055. http://
Prevalence and treatment of mental disorders, 1990 to 2003. N Engl J Med dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbw055.
2005;352:2515–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1056/NEJMsa043266.