Hennessey 2003 The Social Psychology of Creativity
Hennessey 2003 The Social Psychology of Creativity
Hennessey 2003 The Social Psychology of Creativity
Beth A. Hennessey
To cite this article: Beth A. Hennessey (2003) The Social Psychology of Creativity, Scandinavian
Journal of Educational Research, 47:3, 253-271, DOI: 10.1080/00313830308601
ABSTRACT Motivation plays a crucial role in the creative process. It is not enough to have
unusually high levels of skill or a deep conceptual understanding. In order for students to reach
their creative potential, they must approach a task with intrinsic motivation; they must engage
in that task for the sheer pleasure and enjoyment of the activity itself rather than for some
external goal. Researchers and theorists now understand that there is a direct relation between
the motivational orientation brought to a task and the likelihood of creativity at that task. And
it is particular features of the school environment and students’ daily routine that in large part
determine that motivation. The present paper outlines investigations revealing that the typical
classroom is fraught with teaching practices and programme features that kill intrinsic motiv-
ation and creativity. Research designed to immunise students against the negative effects of these
damaging classroom elements is reviewed. The argument is made that the undermining of
intrinsic motivation and creativity of performance may be largely driven by an affective, rather
than a cognitive, mechanism, and recent cross-cultural data gathered in a non-western
educational setting are reviewed.
INTRODUCTION
The empirical study of creativity has a long and distinguished history. As early as
1870, Galton published a study of the biographies and autobiographies of well-
known creative figures and set out to identify the unique qualities of intellect and
personality that differentiated this group from their less creative peers (Galton,
1870). This emphasis on the individual difference variables that contribute to high
levels of creativity was later fueled by J.P. Guilford who, in 1950, in his presidential
address to the American Psychological Association, argued that ‘the psychologist’s
problem is that of creative personality’ (Guilford, 1950, p. 444). This proclamation
prompted many investigators to carry out intensive laboratory studies of creative
persons (see, for example, MacKinnon, 1962), while a second group of researchers
and theorists had begun to focus their attention on the creative process, attempting
among other things to specify a universal sequence of steps involved in creative
production (see, for example, Wallas, 1926) or the cognitive skills necessary for
creative performance (Newell et al., 1962). Implicit in much of this work has been
ISSN 0031-3831 print; ISSN 1430-1170 online/03/030253-19 2003 Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research
DOI: 10.1080/0031383032000079236
254 B. A. Hennessey
sense of control: when a person perceives her task engagement as externally con-
trolled, she is extrinsically rather than intrinsically motivated.
In sum, researchers and theorists tell us that a large number of features of task
engagement contribute to an intrinsically motivated orientation. The individual may
be curious about or in some other way stimulated by the presentation of the activity.
With task participation come feelings of competence, mastery or self-efficacy. And,
perhaps most significantly, while engaging in a task that they find intrinsically
interesting, individuals feel that their involvement is free of strong external control:
they get the sense that they are playing rather than working. Importantly, each of
these hallmarks of intrinsic motivation focuses on the individual’s inner phenomeno-
logical state. Whether prompted by just the right amount of novelty, feelings of
competence or a sense of control, the intrinsically motivated state comes about as
the result of an internal, very individualised process, the complexities of which we
are only beginning to appreciate.
the case of this story-telling task, this was accomplished with the stipulation that
children say only ‘one thing’ about each page. Second, in order to be appropriate for
testing hypotheses about creativity, the task had to allow for a wide variety of
responses. In other words, the target activity had to be an open-ended one for which
a wide variety of responses were possible (McGraw, 1978; Amabile, 1982b; Hen-
nessey & Amabile, 1999). Finally, like all the creativity tasks used in research of this
type, it was important that the story-telling procedure be pre-tested to ensure that
children of this age group did, in fact, find it to be intrinsically interesting.
Elementary school teachers familiar with children’s writing later rated the
stories relative to one another on creativity and a variety of other dimensions. A high
level of inter-rater reliability was reached, and results indicated that, overall, stories
produced by children in the no-reward condition were judged to be more creative
than were stories produced by children in the reward condition. This main effect of
reward was, in fact, statistically significant. Importantly, the only difference in the
experience of the rewarded and non-rewarded children in this paradigm was their
perception of the picture taking reward as contingent or not contingent on the target
story-telling activity.
possible hallmarks of product creativity, are best able to make such judgements.
Over 20 years of research have, in fact, clearly established that product creativity can
be reliably and validly assessed based on the consensus of experts. Although
creativity in a product may be difficult to characterise in terms of specific features,
it is something that people can recognise and agree on when they see it.
The CAT is grounded on two complementary definitions of creativity. The
underlying conceptual definition that has been used in building a theoretical formu-
lation of the creative process states that a product will be judged as creative to the
extent that (i) it is both a novel and appropriate, useful, correct or valuable response
to the task at hand and (ii) the task is heuristic rather than algorithmic (Amabile,
1996, p. 35). The operational definition upon which the CAT is based is readily
applicable to empirical research: a product or response is creative to the extent that
appropriate observers agree it is creative. Appropriate observers are those familiar
with the domain in which the product was created or the response articulated
(Amabile, 1996, p. 33). Importantly, this consensual definition is based on the
creative product rather than the creative process. Not only has a clear articulation of
the creative process yet to be developed but, more importantly, any identification of
a thought process as ‘creative’ must finally depend on the fruit of that process, a
product or response.
Amabile and her colleagues have attempted to capture the essential characteris-
tics of the conceptual definition of creativity in the CAT (Amabile, 1982b; Hen-
nessey & Amabile, 1999). First, study participants are presented with tasks that
leave room for considerable flexibility and novelty of response. Second, these are
tasks for which the range of appropriate responses has been clearly identified in
participants’ instructions. Finally, the experimental activities employed are all
heuristic in nature; judges are only asked to make ratings of open-ended tasks, tasks
with more than one solution and a variety of paths to those solutions.
This approach is especially well suited to investigate environmental influences
on creativity. The majority of available assessment techniques resemble personality
or IQ tests in that they view creativity as an enduring personality trait. Whether they
request that a picture be completed, unusual uses for a brick be generated, adjectives
describing the self be selected or remote associations be discovered, most paper-and-
pencil measures have been specifically constructed to maximise individual differ-
ences. They had been constructed to do exactly what we in the study described
earlier were trying to avoid. Researchers like myself who take a social-psychological
approach must control for and, as much as possible, eliminate within-group varia-
bility in their dependent measures in order that they might detect more global
between-group differences produced by their direct experimental manipulations of
social and environmental factors. In this investigation involving schoolchildren,
individual differenes constitute the error variance. We were not interested in
whether a particular child was likely to consistently evidence greater levels of
creativity than the majority of his/her peers. We were interested in creativity not as
a relatively enduring and stable trait, but as the result of a fleeting and delicate
motivational state: a state brought on by environmental factors such as the presence
or absence of reward. What we needed was a measurement tool that de-emphasised
258 B. A. Hennessey
with an additive model that recognises that under certain specific conditions, the
expectation of reward can sometimes increase levels of extrinsic motivation without
having any negative impact on intrinsic motivation or performance. In fact, some
types of extrinsic motivation can actually enhance creativity of performance.
Research reveals that study participants given a choice about task engagement
can come to perceive their receipt of a reward as a kind of ‘bonus’ rather than a
controlling extrinsic constraint. In one instance, college students who believed that
they had freely chosen to take part in a research study and who had been offered a
reward for their participation were the most creative and most intrinsically motivated
of any group, including a no-reward ‘control’ condition (Amabile et al., 1986, Study
3). Intrigued by these findings, researchers have gone on to discover an additive
effect of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation in a variety of circumstances. They now
understand, for example, that the powerful undermining effect of expected reward
is most likely to occur when what have come to be termed ‘task-contingent’ rewards
have been promised. Task-contingent rewards are rewards made conditional simply
on task completion (e.g. a pre-schooler is told ‘I’ll give you a good player certificate
if you draw a picture for me’). The impact of so-called ‘performance-contingent’
rewards, rewards promised and delivered only if a certain level of competency or
proficiency is reached (e.g. a college student is told ‘If you earn a score of 80 or
above on this creative writing assessment, you may waive the requirement that you
take the introductory level course’). Under certain specific circumstances, in fact,
the informational value implicit in performance-contingent rewards has been shown
to augment feelings of self-efficacy, intrinsic task interest and qualitative aspects of
performance (see, for example, Deci, 1975; Deci & Ryan, 1980, 1985b; Harack-
iewicz et al., 1984).
Building on this body of evidence, researchers have gone on to uncover the
deleterious impact of a variety of other environmental constraints, such as deadlines,
surveillance and competition (see, for example, Amabile, 1982a; Amabile et al.,
1990). In recent years, the investigative focus has also been expanded to include the
impact of evaluation, and the data reveal that the expectation that one’s work will be
judged may well be the most deleterious extrinsic constraint of all. Perhaps because
situations of evaluation often combine aspects of each of the other ‘killers’ of
motivation and creativity, the promise of an evaluation has been shown to severely
undermine the task interest and performance of persons across the entire age span,
individuals from all walks of life, from pre-schoolers to professionals whose very
livelihood depends upon the creativity of their work.
One prototypical study of the effects of evaluation was conducted by Amabile
in 1982. The primary purpose behind this investigation was to investigate the impact
of a competitive evaluation situation on the creativity of girls, ages 7–11. These
young study participants were randomly selected to attend either a Saturday or a
Sunday ‘Art Party’ held in the common room of their apartment complex. Girls
attending the Saturday (non-competition control) session were greeted at the
entrance by a table of desirable toys and gifts and which they were told would be
raffled off at the end of the party. They then spent the afternoon engaged in a variety
of fun activities, including a collage making task that they completed without any
260 B. A. Hennessey
A PROPOSED MECHANISM
Researchers have found it all too easy to undermine task motivation and creativity
of performance with the promise of a reward or an impending evaluation. Demon-
strating how to kill motivation and creativity has been the straightforward part. What
Social Psychology of Creativity 261
has not been as easy is understanding why these extrinsic task constraints can have
such a negative impact. What are the internal mechanisms that bring about the
undermining effects of expected reward and impending evaluation?
What we have come to learn is that most of us are not all that in touch with our
own motivations. We do not always know why it is that we do the things we do.
Almost as if we were outside observers of even our own actions, we seem to use
essentially the same rubrics for explaining our own behaviours as we do for
explaining why others behave in the ways that they do. In situations where both a
plausible internal and external (intrinsic and extrinsic) cause of behaviour are
present, we tend to discount the internal cause in favour of the external cause. For
example, a pre-schooler in the seminal ‘Magic Marker’ study (Lepper et al., 1973)
thinks to herself: ‘I must be making this picture not because it’s fun and I love using
markers but because this man has told me that I will get a Good Player Award’.
When multiple explanations for their behaviour are available, young and old
alike have been found to discount their own intrinsic interest in favour of a purely
external explanation for task engagement. Some social psychologists have come to
refer to this process as the ‘discounting principle’ (see, for example, Kelley, 1973).
Other theorists propose a related explanation termed the ‘over-justification’ hypoth-
esis, a formulation derived from the attribution theories of Bem (1972), Kelley
(1967, 1973) and de Charms (1968). According to this model, when a behaviour is
over-justified (when there exists both a possible internal and external cause for one’s
own or another’s behaviour), each of us will tend to overlook the internal cause (the
presence of intrinsic task motivation) in favour of the external cause (a reward or
evaluation was at stake). In effect, we discount the excess justification for explaining
why we did something.
Whatever the particulars of the theoretical explanation evoked, the fact remains
that in the face of expected evaluation or reward, competition, surveillance or time
limits, intrinsic motivation is bound to suffer. And without high levels of intrinsic
motivation, creative performance is highly unlikely. Why is intrinsic motivation so
necessary to creative performance? A number of cognitive psychologists have offered
empirical evidence and models that have proven useful in understanding how the
type of motivation brought to a task can influence performance on that task. For
example, Simon (1967) has proposed that the most important function of task
motivation is the control of attention. He postulates that task motivation determines
which of many goal hierarchies will be activated, and goes on to suggest that the
more intense the motivation to achieve a goal, the less attention will be paid to
environmental aspects that are seemingly irrelevant to achieving that goal. This
formulation can be used to explain the widely reported finding that incidental or
latent learning is impaired when a reward or evaluation is at stake (see, for example,
Spence, 1956; Kimble, 1961). It can also help to explain the negative effects of
extrinsic constraints on creativity.
Amabile (1996) offers a maze metaphor that is helpful in illustrating the
undermining effects of reward or evaluation. She asks that we think of an open-
ended ‘creativity-type’ task as a maze. There is one starting point, one entrance, into
this maze but there are a variety of exit points and many different paths to those
262 B. A. Hennessey
exits. Most importantly, some of those exits, or solutions, are more ‘elegant’ or
creative than others. In the face of an expected reward or evaluation, the goal is to
get in and out of the maze as quickly as possible. The ‘safest’, most straightforward
path will be chosen, as all behaviour is narrowly directed toward attaining that goal.
In order for a creative idea to be generated, however, it is often necessary to
temporarily ‘step away’ from environmental constraints (Newell et al., 1962), to
become immersed in the maze itself, to experiment with alternative pathways and to
direct attention toward more seemingly incidental aspects of the task. The more
focused an individual is about a promised reward or evaluation, the less likely it is
that risks will be taken and that these alternative paths to a solution will be explored.
Some theorists trained in the behaviourist tradition, most notably Eisenberger
and colleagues, have suggested that this undermining effect of reward or evaluation
on intrinsic motivation and creativity of performance, this unwillingness or inability
to experiment within the maze, can be explained by a simple ‘diffusion of attention’
or ‘competing response’ model (see, for example, Reiss & Sushinsky, 1975). In other
words, when individuals are distracted by their excitement about a soon-to-be-
delivered prize or their anxiety surrounding an impending evaluation, their intrinsic
motivation and enjoyment of the task at hand are undermined and they rush through
their work as quickly as possible. Importantly, while this diffusion of attention or
competing response hypothesis may explain the undermining impact of reward and
other extrinsic constraints under some circumstances, these models fail to ade-
quately explain the negative effects of rewards in all situations. Recall the ‘Picture
Taking’ study described earlier (Amabile et al., 1986). In that investigation, even a
reward promised and delivered prior to task engagement was found to undermine
subjects’ interest and performance. The mere labelling of the opportunity to use a
camera as a reward contingency was enough to kill intrinsic motivation and creativ-
ity, while children in a control condition who also participated in the picture taking
but believed this was just one in a series of ‘things to do’ suffered no such
decrements. Furthermore, investigations examining the interactive effects of reward
and choice (see, for example, Amabile et al., 1982) also call into question the
diffusion of attention explanation. These studies reveal that when subjects who
perceive they have no choice but to participate in an investigation are offered a
reward, their task motivation and creativity do not suffer the usual decrements.
When working under environmental constraints, people may indeed pay less
attention to a task or less attention to aspects of their environment that might prove
useful in generating a response to that task. However, this shift in focus need not
always occur simply because they are distracted by the reward they are to receive or
by their worries about what they have to do to obtain a favourable evaluation. Under
constraint conditions, people may simply feel less intrinsically involved. They may
feel less positively toward the task and less inclined to devote their energy and
attention to it.
There is good reason to believe, in fact, that affective processes can and do play
an important role in the mediation of the impact of extrinsic constraints on interest
and creativity. Earlier in this chapter, explanations of cognitive mechanisms such as
discounting and over-justification were offered. While these models have proven
Social Psychology of Creativity 263
useful for understanding the negative effects of reward and evaluation in adults, they
fail to adequately explain why young children have also been observed to suffer
decreases in intrinsic motivation and creativity. Simply stated, children under the
age of 7 or 8 years have consistently been shown to lack the cognitive capabilities
necessary for weighing multiple sufficient causes and employing discounting (see, for
example, Shultz et al., 1975; Smith, 1975). In fact, some studies have indicated that
many young children seem to employ an additive algorithm and interpret the
expectation of reward as an augmentation of intrinsic interest (see, for example,
Di Vitto & McArthur, 1978; Morgan, 1981). How is it that, when working under
the expectation of reward, young children frequently demonstrate decreases in
intrinsic motivation and creativity of performance yet they seem cognitively
incapable of engaging in the thought processes that underlie the over-justification
paradigm?
One viable alternative to the discounting explanation is that the reduction in
intrinsic interest in young children (and perhaps all of us) is driven primarily by the
learned expectation that rewards, evaluation, time limits and competitive elements
are usually paired with activities that need to be done, activities that are often not
fun and sometimes even aversive. The undermining of intrinsic interest may result
as much from emotion or affect as it does from thoughts or cognitive analysis.
Children may learn to react negatively to a task as ‘work’ when their behaviour is
controlled by socially imposed factors (such as rewards) and they may react posi-
tively to a task as ‘play’ when there are no constraints imposed. A negative affect
resulting from socially learned stereotypes or scripts of work (see Ransen, 1980;
Morgan, 1981; Lepper et al., 1982) may be what leads to decrements in intrinsic
interest (see Hennessey, 1999).
their already positive feelings about the tasks they were doing. In an effort to test
these hypotheses, two follow-up investigations of our intrinsic motivation focus
techniques (Hennessey et al., 1989, Study 2; Hennessey & Zbikowski, 1993) were
subsequently carried out. Each was designed as a conceptual replication of Study 1.
Essentially the same experimental design was employed, and it was again the
children who had received immunisation training and who were expecting a reward
who produced the most creative products. Yet, in these subsequent two studies, the
effect of training was far less dramatic. In Study 2, statistical comparisons revealed
that the creativity of those children receiving training and expecting a reward for
their performance was significantly different from only one of the other three design
groups, and in Study 3, although children assigned to the intrinsic motivation
focus/reward condition again produced the most creative products, their perform-
ance was only significantly different from that of the no training/reward group.
Taken together, the results of Studies 2 and 3 indicate that we cannot expect that
children exposed to our intrinsic motivation training and offered a reward for their
performance will demonstrate unusually high levels of creativity. We can expect,
however, that these children will be able to maintain baseline levels of intrinsic
motivation and creativity under reward conditions.
What is it about our immunisation procedures that allow children to maintain
their creativity even when they expect a reward? It appears that our efforts to help
them learn to de-emphasise the importance of extrinsic incentives and concentrate
instead on their own intrinsic interest and task enjoyment paid off. Even in the face
of reward, the children were able to maintain a positive, intrinsically motivated
approach. They brought to our experimental tasks a playfulness and a willingness to
take risks that many researchers believe are crucial to creativity (Campbell, 1960;
Crutchfield, 1962; Lieberman, 1965; Barron, 1968; Stein, 1974; Dansky & Silver-
man, 1975; Amabile, 1983, 1996).
Evidence from non-experimental studies coupled with observations of and
interviews with artists and other persons who rely upon their creativity for their life’s
work echo our ‘immunisation’ results. While many of the ‘killers’ of motivation and
creativity that have been isolated experimentally have also been found to be detri-
mental in the ‘real world’ of work, these negative effects have not proven universal.
For some people, certain extrinsic motivators have been shown to have either no
effects or even a positive effect on task interest and creativity of performance. For
example, in a study of commissioned and non-commissioned works done by
professional artists, the extrinsic incentive of a commission was seen by some artists
as a highly controlling constraint, and the creativity of their work plummeted. Yet
for those who looked at the commission as an opportunity to achieve recognition or
a confirmation of their competence by respected others, creativity was enhanced
(Amabile et al., 1993).
How can these individual differences be explained? Our data on these pro-
fessional artists and the children taking part in our immunisation studies parallel
nicely earlier work exploring the relevance of self-perception processes to the
over-justification effect. In a 1981 investigation carried out by Fazio, the negative
impact of expected reward was also mitigated in young children for whom initial
266 B. A. Hennessey
intrinsic interest in the target activity had been made salient (Fazio, 1981). In other
words, it may not be the expectation of reward per se that undermines intrinsic
motivation, rather it may be the individual’s interpretation of that reward and his or
her role in the reward process that in large part determines whether task motivation
will be undermined, enhanced or remain unchanged.
REFERENCES
AMABILE, T.M. (1982a). Children’s artistic creativity: detrimental effects of competition in a field
setting. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 8, 573–578.
AMABILE, T.M. (1982b). Social psychology of creativity: a consensual assessment technique. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 997–1013.
AMABILE, T.M. (1983). The Social Psychology of Creativity. New York: Springer-Verlag.
AMABILE, T.M. (1993). Motivational synergy: toward new conceptualizations of intrinsic and extrinsic
motivation in the workplace. Human Resource Management Review, 3, 185–201.
AMABILE, T.M. (1996). Creativity in Context. Boulder, CO: Westview.
AMABILE, T.M., GOLDBERG, N. & CAPOTOSTO, D. (1982). Effects of reward and choice on adults’
artistic creativity, unpublished manuscript, Brandeis University.
AMABILE, T.M., HENNESSEY, B.A. & GROSSMAN, B. (1986). Social influences on creativity: the effects of
contracted-for reward. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 14–23.
AMABILE, T.M., GOLDFARB, P. & BRACKFIELD, S.C. (1990). Social influences on creativity: evaluation,
coaction, and surveillance. Creativity Research Journal, 3, 6–21.
AMABILE, T.M., PHILLIPS, E.D. & COLLINS, M.A. (1993). Creativity by contract: social influences on the
creativity of professional artists, unpublished manuscript, Brandeis University.
AMABILE, T.M., HILL, K., HENNESSEY, B.A., & TIGHE, E. (1994). The work preference inventory:
assessing intrinsic and extrinsic motivational orientations. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 66, 950–967.
BARRON, F. (1968). Creativity and Personal Freedom. New York: Van Nostrand.
BEM, D. (1972). Self-perception theory. In L. BERKOWITZ (Ed.) Advances in Experimental Social
Psychology, Vol. 6. New York: Academic Press.
BERLYNE, D.E. (1960). Conflict, Arousal, and Curiosity. New York: McGraw-Hill.
CAMPBELL, D. (1960). Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge
processes. Psychological Review, 67, 380–400.
CHEEK, J.M. & STAHL, S. (1986). Shyness and verbal creativity. Journal of Research in Personality, 20,
51–61.
CONTI, R. & AMABILE, T.M. (1995). Problem solving among computer science students: the effects of
skill, evaluation expectation and personality on solution quality, paper presented at the Annual
Meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, Boston, MA.
CONTI, R., COLLINS, M.A. & PICARIELLO, M. (1995). Differential effects of competition on the artistic
creativity of girls and boys, unpublished manuscript, Brandeis University.
CRUTCHFIELD, R. (1962). Conformity and creative thinking. In H. GRUBER, G. TERRELL & M.
WERTHEIMER (Eds) Contemporary Approaches to Creative Thinking. New York: Atherton Press.
DANSKY, J. & SILVERMAN, I. (1975). Play: a general facilitator of fluency. Developmental Psychology, 11,
104.
DE CHARMS, R. (1968). Personal Causation. New York: Academic Press.
DECI, E.L. (1971). Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 18, 105–115.
DECI, E.L. (1972). The effects of contingent and noncontingent rewards and controls on intrinsic
motivation. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 8, 217–229.
DECI, E.L. (1975). Intrinsic Motivation. New York: Plenum.
Social Psychology of Creativity 269
DECI, E.L. & RYAN, R.M. (1980). The empirical exploration of intrinsic motivational processes. In L.
BERKOWITZ (Ed.) Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 13. New York: Academic Press.
DECI, E.L. & RYAN, R.M. (1985a). The general causality orientations scale: self-determination in
personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 19, 109–134.
DECI, E.L. & RYAN, R.M. (1985b). Intrinsic Motivation and Self-determination in Human Behavior. New
York: Plenum.
DI VITTO, B. & MCARTHUR, L.Z. (1978). Developmental differences in the use of distinctiveness,
consensus, and consistency information for making causal attributions. Developmental Psychology,
14, 474–482.
FAZIO, R.H. (1981). On the self-perception explanation of the overjustification effect: the role of the
salience of initial attitude. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 17, 417–426.
GALTON, F. (1870). Hereditary Genius. London: Macmillan.
GARBARINO, J. (1975). The impact of anticipated reward upon cross-age tutoring. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 32, 421–428.
GREENE, D. & LEPPER, M. (1974). Effects of extrinsic rewards on children’s subsequent interest. Child
Development, 45, 1141–1145.
GUILFORD, J.P. (1950). Creativity. American Psychologist, 5, 444–454.
HARACKIEWICZ, J. M., MANDERLINK, G. & SANSONE, C. (1984). Rewarding pinball wizardry: the effects
of evaluation on intrinsic interest. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 287–300.
HARACKIEWICZ, J.M., ABRAHAMS, S. & WAGEMAN, R. (1991). Performance evaluation and intrinsic
motivation: the effects of evaluative focus, rewards and achievement orientation. Journal of Person-
ality and Social Psychology, 63, 1015–1029.
HARTER, S. (1978). Effectance motivation reconsidered: toward a developmental model. Human Devel-
opment, 21, 34–64.
HEBB, D.O. (1955). Drives and the CNS. Psychological Review, 62, 243–254.
HENNESSEY, B.A. (1996). Teaching for creative development: a social-psychological approach. In N.
COLANGELO & G. DAVIS (Eds) Handbook of Gifted Education, 2nd Edn. Needham Heights, MA:
Allyn & Bacon.
HENNESSEY, B.A. (1999). Intrinsic motivation, affect and creativity. In S. RUSS (Ed.) Affect, Creative
Experience and Psychological Adjustment. Philadelphia, PA: Taylor & Francis.
HENNESSEY, B.A. (2000a). Rewards and creativity. In C. SANSONE & J. HARACKIEWICZ (Eds) Intrinsic
and Extrinsic Motivation: the search for optimal motivation and performance. New York: Academic
Press.
HENNESSEY, B.A. (2000b). The intrinsic motivation principle of creativity: does real-world classroom
evidence support the model? In J. HOUTZ (Chair), Development of Creativity, symposium conducted
at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Washington, DC.
HENNESSEY, B.A. & AMABILE, T.M. (1988). Story-telling: a method for assessing children’s creativity.
Journal of Creative Behavior, 22, 235–246.
HENNESSEY, B.A. & AMABILE, T.M. (1999). Consensual assessment. In M. RUNCO & S. PRITZKER (Eds)
Encyclopedia of Creativity. New York: Academic Press.
HENNESSEY, B.A. & ZBIKOWSKI, S. (1993). Immunizing children against the negative effects of reward:
a further examination of intrinsic motivation training techniques. Creativity Research Journal, 6,
297–307.
HENNESSEY, B.A., AMABILE, T.M. & MARTINAGE, M. (1989). Immunizing children against the negative
effects of reward. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 14, 212–227.
HILL, K., AMABILE, T.M., COON, H.M. & WHITNEY, D. (1994). Testing the componential model of
creativity, unpublished manuscript, Brandeis University.
JUSSIM, L.S., SOFFIN, S., BROWN, R., LEY, J. & KOHLHEPP, K. (1992). Understanding reactions to
feedback by integrating ideas from symbolic intereactionism and cognitive evaluation theory.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 402–421.
KELLEY, H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. In D. LEVINE (Ed.) Nebraska Symposium on
Motivation, Vol. 15. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska.
270 B. A. Hennessey
KELLEY, H. (1973). The processes of causal attribution. American Psychologist, 28, 107–128.
KERNOODLE-LOVELAND, K. & OLLEY, J. (1979). The effect of external reward on interest and quality of
task performance in children of high and low intrinsic motivation. Child Development, 50, 1207–
1210.
KIMBLE, G. (1961). Hilgard and Marquis’ Conditioning and Learning, 2nd Edn. New York: Appleton-
Century-Crofts.
KRUGLANSKI, A.W., FRIEDMAN, I. & ZEEVI, G. (1971). The effects of extrinsic incentive on some
qualitative aspects of task performance. Journal of Personality, 39, 606–617.
LEPPER, M., GREENE, D. & NISBETT, R. (1973). Undermining children’s intrinsic interest with extrinsic
rewards: a test of the ‘overjustification’ hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28,
129–137.
LEPPER, M., SAGOTSKY, G., DAFOE, J.L. & GREENE, D. (1982). Consequences of superfluous social
constraints: effects on young children’s social inferences and subsequent intrinsic motivation.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 51–65.
LIEBERMAN, J.N. (1965). Playfulness and divergent thinking: an investigation of their relationship at the
kindergarten level. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 107, 219–224.
MACKINNON, D.W. (1962). The nature and nurture of creative talent. American Psychologist, 17,
484–495.
MCGRAW, K. (1978). The detrimental effects of reward on performance: a literature review and a
prediction model. In M. LEPPER & D. GREENE (Eds) The Hidden Costs of Reward. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
MCGRAW, K. & MCCULLERS, J. (1979). Evidence of a detrimental effect of extrinsic incentives on
breaking a mental set. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 15, 285–294.
MORGAN, M. (1981). The overjustification effect: a developmental test of self-perception interpreta-
tions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 809–821.
NEWELL, A., SHAW, J. & SIMON, H. (1962). The processes of creative thinking. In H. GRUBER, G.
TERRELL & M. WERTHEIMER (Eds) Contemporary Approaches to Creative Thinking. New York:
Atherton.
PITTMAN, T.S., EMERY, J. & BOGGIANO, A.K. (1982). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivational orientations:
reward-induced changes in preference for complexity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
42, 789–797.
POLLAK, S. (1992). The effects of motivational orientation and constraint on the creativity of the artist,
unpublished manuscript, Brandeis University.
RANSEN, D. (1980). The mediation of reward-induced motivation decrements in early and middle
childhood: a template matching approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 49–55.
REISS, S. & SUSHINSKY, L.W. (1975). Overjustification, competing responses, and the acquisition of
intrinsic interest. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 1116–1125.
RYAN, R.M. & DECI, E.L. (2000a). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation,
social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68–78.
RYAN, R.M. & DECI, E.L. (2000b). When rewards compete with nature: the undermining of intrinsic
motivation and self-regulation. In C. SANSONE & J.M. HARACKIEWICZ (Eds) Intrinsic and Extrinsic
Motivation: the search for optimal motivation and performance. San Diego, CA: Academic.
SHAPIRA, Z. (1976). Expectancy determinants of intrinsically motivated behavior. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 39, 1235–1244.
SHULTZ, T., BUTKOWSKY, I., PEARCE, J. & SHANFIELD, H. (1975). The development of schemes for the
attribution of multiple psychological causes. Developmental Psychology, 11, 502–510.
SIMON, H. (1967). Motivational and emotional controls of cognition. Psychological Review, 74, 29–39.
SMITH, M.C. (1975). Children’s use of the multiple sufficient cause schema in social perception. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 737–747.
SPENCE, K. (1956). Behavior Theory and Conditioning. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
STEIN, M.I. (1974). Stimulating Creativity, Vols. 1 and 2. New York: Academic Press.
STERNBERG, R.J. & LUBART, T.I. (1991). An investment theory of creativity and its development. Human
Development, 34, 1–32.
Social Psychology of Creativity 271
TORRANCE, E.P. (1974). Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking: norms-technical manual. Lexington, MA:
Ginn.
WALLAS, G. (1926). The Art of Thought. New York: Harcourt Brace.
WHITE, R.W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: the concept of competence. Psychological Review, 66,
297–333.