SLK Chapter7
SLK Chapter7
SLK Chapter7
Selective attention Is the ability to filter out distractions and focus on certain inputs
Research shows that the brain can only handle one attention demanding task at a time
meaning that multitasking can have negative impacts.
According to Craik and Lockhart's theory, we can process information at different levels:
• Structural encoding: It’s a shallow processing that emphasises the physical structure
of the stimulus. For example, if words are flashed on a screen, structural encoding
registers such things as how the words are printed (capital, lowercase and so on) or
their length (how many letters). (shallow)
• Phonemic encoding: This level is a bit deeper and involves focusing on how the
words sound. For example, saying the words out loud or silently in your head.
(intermediate)
• Semantic encoding: This is the deepest level of processing. It involves thinking about
the meaning of the words and what they represent. For example, considering what
actions or objects the words describe. (deep)
The most influential model was proposed by Atkinson and Shiffrin (1968, 1971). According
to this model, memory has three main stages:
Sensory Memory:
• Sensory memory is a brief storage system that retains information in its original
sensory form (e.g., visual, auditory, or tactile) for a very short time, usually just a
fraction of a second.
• It allows the sensation of visual patterns, sounds, or touches to linger momentarily
after the original stimulus is gone.
Example:
• When you move a light quickly in a circle in the dark, you see a complete circle even
though there's only a single point of light. This is due to sensory memory retaining the
image briefly, allowing you to perceive continuity.
Durability of Storage:
• Without rehearsal, information decays rapidly. Research by Peterson and Peterson
(1959) demonstrated that participants could recall consonants poorly after just 15
seconds when prevented from rehearsing them. This indicated that information in
short-term memory is lost quickly, and it was later suggested that interference from
new information is a significant factor in this loss.
Capacity of Storage:
• The capacity of short-term memory is limited. George Miller (1956) proposed that
people can typically remember about seven items (plus or minus two).
• However, more recent research by Nelson Cowan (2010) suggests that the capacity
might actually be closer to four items, emphasizing that covert rehearsal and chunking
(grouping items into larger units) can increase recall capacity.
• Chunking helps to overcome the limitations of short-term memory by allowing
individuals to remember larger groups of familiar stimuli as single units. For example,
remembering "FNB" as one chunk is easier than remembering "F," "N," and "B"
separately
Working Memory
Key Takeaways
• Short-term memory is limited in duration and capacity but can be extended through
rehearsal and chunking.
• Working memory expands on the concept of short-term memory, incorporating
multiple components for processing information.
• Long-term memory is more complex, with evidence suggesting that while some
memories may last a lifetime, inaccuracies and forgetting do occur, challenging the
notion of permanent storage.
• Categorization: People tend to organize information into categories for easier storage
and recall. For example, in a study by Bousfield (1953), participants recalled words
more effectively when they naturally clustered similar items, such as animals or
professions.
• Conceptual Hierarchies: Information can also be organized into multilevel systems
based on shared properties among items. Gordon Bower (1970) suggested that
structuring information this way can significantly improve memory recall.
Schemas:
Semantic Networks:
• Not all information fits neatly into categories or schemas; some knowledge is
structured in networks called semantic networks. These networks consist of nodes
(representing concepts) linked by pathways (representing relationships between
concepts).
• The concept of spreading activation is central to semantic networks. When one node
is activated, related nodes are also activated, facilitating recall. For example, thinking
about "fruit" can make related words like "peach" easier to remember due to the short
pathways connecting them.
Tip-of-the-Tongue Phenomenon:
• This is when you cannot remember something, like a name, even though you feel you
know it.
• Common for everyone, happening about once a week, especially as you get older.
Retrieval Cues:
• They tested people on hard words and found that people could guess the first letter of
the word correctly 57% of the time, even if they couldn't recall the whole word. This
shows that partial memory helps.
1. Context Cues:
o These are hints from your surroundings or situation that help trigger your
memory.
o For example, thinking about where you usually eat breakfast can help you
remember what you had.
2. Experiencing Context Cues:
o People often recall forgotten memories when they revisit places where they
used to live. This is because the familiar environment triggers memories
associated with that place.
o Another common situation is when you walk into a room to get something,
forget what it was, and then remember it as soon as you go back to the original
room.
3. Importance in Eyewitness Testimony:
o In legal settings, hypnosis is sometimes used to help witnesses remember
details by getting them to imagine being back at the crime scene.
o However, studies show that hypnosis doesn’t reliably improve memory recall
and can actually lead to more mistakes, such as recalling false information.
Reconstructing Memories
Source Monitoring
Memory can be categorized into two primary systems: declarative memory and
nondeclarative memory. These systems serve different functions and have distinct
characteristics.
Nondeclarative memory is often automatic, requiring little conscious effort. You can
ride a bike without thinking about each movement involved, which contrasts with the
more conscious effort needed to recall a specific fact.
3. Key Differences:
o Conscious Awareness: Declarative memory requires conscious effort to
retrieve, while nondeclarative memory often operates automatically.
o Forgetting: Declarative memories are more susceptible to forgetting over
time, whereas procedural memories tend to remain intact even after long
periods without practice.
o Neuroscientific Evidence: Patterns of memory loss in individuals with
amnesia (like H.M.) demonstrate the separation of these two systems. H.M.
could learn new motor skills but could not recall facts or experiences from his
past, highlighting the intactness of his procedural memory despite impaired
declarative memory.
Semantic Memory and Episodic Memory are two subtypes of declarative memory
identified by Endel Tulving. While both involve the storage of factual information, they
differ in their nature and function.
1. Episodic Memory:
o Definition: Episodic memory refers to the recollection of personal experiences
and specific events in time. It is characterized by temporal tagging, meaning it
involves recalling when an event occurred.
o Characteristics:
▪ Involves personal, autobiographical memories (e.g., memories of
family vacations, significant life events).
▪ Enables "mental time travel," allowing individuals to re-experience
past events (Tulving, 2001).
▪ Provides a basis for imagining and planning future events, which can
impact psychological well-being (D’Argembeau et al., 2011).
▪ Associated with a sense of "remembering" during retrieval, which is
often vivid and detailed.
o Example: Remembering your first day at school, the smell of the food at a
wedding you attended, or the sound of a friend's laughter at a concert.
2. Semantic Memory:
o Definition: Semantic memory contains general knowledge and facts that are
not linked to a specific time or personal context. This information is more
abstract and universal.
o Characteristics:
▪ Includes knowledge about the world, concepts, and meanings (e.g.,
knowing that water boils at 100 degrees Celsius or that a cat is a type
of animal).
▪ Typically lacks temporal context; individuals generally do not
remember when or where they learned these facts (McNamara, 2013).
▪ Associated with a sense of "knowing" during retrieval, which is less
vivid than episodic memory.
o Example: Knowing the capital city of France is Paris or that the Earth orbits
the Sun.
3. Key Differences:
o Content: Episodic memory is personal and contextual, while semantic
memory is general and factual.
o Temporal Context: Episodic memory is temporally dated; semantic memory
is not.
o Retrieval Experience: Episodic memories evoke a feeling of recollection,
while semantic memories evoke a feeling of familiarity or knowing.
4. Neuroscientific Evidence:
o Evidence from studies of amnesia shows that episodic and semantic memories
can be dissociated. Some individuals may lose their episodic memories but
retain semantic knowledge (Szpunar & McDermott, 2009). This suggests that
the two memory types may operate through different neural mechanisms,
although there is ongoing debate regarding their distinct neural bases.
Prospective Memory and Retrospective Memory are two distinct types of memory tasks
that focus on different aspects of remembering.
1. Retrospective Memory:
o Definition: This type of memory is concerned with recalling information from
the past. It involves retrieving previously learned facts or experiences.
o Characteristics:
▪ Involves remembering events or knowledge that have occurred (e.g.,
facts, experiences, or skills learned).
▪ Examples include recalling who won the last Rugby World Cup,
reminiscing about your high school days, or retrieving information
learned in class.
▪ Generally, retrospective memory relies on cues that trigger
recollections of past events or learned information.
2. Prospective Memory:
o Definition: This type of memory involves remembering to perform actions in
the future. It requires an individual to recall intended actions without external
prompts.
o Characteristics:
▪ Involves planning and remembering to carry out future intentions (e.g.,
bringing an umbrella, walking the dog, calling a friend).
▪ A key aspect is that the individual must actively remember to
remember the task at hand, often leading to challenges, especially with
interruptions or distractions.
▪ While examples may seem trivial, failures in prospective memory can
have significant consequences, particularly in critical settings like
aviation or healthcare (Dismukes, 2012).
3. Key Differences:
o Temporal Focus: Retrospective memory focuses on the past, while
prospective memory is oriented towards future actions.
o Cues for Recall: Retrospective memory often relies on external prompts or
cues, whereas prospective memory requires internal reminders to execute
tasks.
o Consequences of Failure: Mistakes in prospective memory can lead to
serious consequences in professional settings, whereas retrospective memory
failures typically involve forgetting facts or experiences.
4. Individual Variability:
o There is considerable variability among individuals in their ability to perform
prospective memory tasks. Those who struggle with these tasks are often
described as "absentminded."
o Research indicates that older adults may face greater challenges with
prospective memory compared to younger individuals, though findings can
vary depending on context (Niedźwieńska & Barzykowski, 2012).
o Interestingly, the increasing reliance on smartphones for managing prospective
memory tasks has been linked to a decline in performance, although this
decline seems reversible with practice (Dumbrava, 2016).